ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Preliminary findings are presented of work undertaken with a prodigious ‘savant’ pianist–Derek Paravicini–in which he learnt a specially-composed piece (The Chromatic Blues) over a number of hearings. All Derek's efforts were recorded digitally, which enabled precise transcriptions to be made. These were analysed in the context of a newly-developed theory of how musical structure is recognised and understood. The results are startling in musicological terms and (it is hypothesised) yield fascinating insights into the subtlety, ingenuity and creativity of Derek's musical mind. It is hoped that the findings from this study will in due course inform the wider debate on the nature of musical learning, memory and creativity.
Content may be subject to copyright.
UNCORRECTED PROOF
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1
2Learning and creativity in a prodigious
3musical savant
4A. Ockelford
a,
*, Linda Pring
b
5
a
Royal National Institute of the Blind, 105 Judd Street, London WC1H 9NE, UK
6
b
Goldsmith’s College, University of London, United Kingdom
7Abstract. Preliminary findings are presented of work undertaken with a prodigious dsavantT
8pianist–Derek Paravicini–in which he learnt a specially-composed piece (The Chromatic Blues)
9over a number of hearings. All Derek’s efforts were recorded digitally, which enabled precise
10transcriptions to be made. These were analysed in the context of a newly-developed theory of
11how musical structure is recognised and understood. The results are startling in musicological
12terms and (it is hypothesised) yield fascinating insights into the subtlety, ingenuity and creativity
13of Derek’s musical mind. It is hoped that the findings from this study will in due course inform
14the wider debate on the nature of musical learning, memory and creativity. D2005 Published by
15Elsevier B.V.
16Keywords: Musical learning; Creativity; Savant; Musical structure; Blues
17
181. Introduction
19This paper presents initial findings from a study that forms part of the dREMUST
20Project–dResearching Exceptional MUsical SkillT–which is a joint initiative of the
21Royal National Institute of the Blind, London and the Psychology Department of
22Goldsmith’s College, University of London. The project focuses on the abilities of
23musical dsavantsT: people with exceptional skill in the context of learning disabilities.
24The current study was undertaken with Derek Paravicini, 25, a prodigiously skilled
25pianist, despite have severe learning difficulties and being totally blind (through
0531-5131/ D2005 Published by Elsevier B.V.
doi:10.1016/j.ics.2005.05.051
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 20 7391 2149.
E-mail address: adam.ockelford@rnib.org.uk (A. Ockelford).
International Congress Series xx (2005) xxx – xxx
www.ics-elsevier.com
ICS-05572; No of Pages 5
DTD 5
UNCORRECTED PROOF
ARTICLE IN PRESS
26dretinopathy of prematurityT). It is one of a series of studies that aims to glean new
27insights into the nature of savants’ musical ability, in particular learning, memory,
28reproduction and creativity, and to use these findings to generate models of the
29processes involved, adopting a fusion of music-psychological and music-theoretical
30approaches [1]. The underlying methodological assumption is that the musical pieces
31and fragments that savants produce following exposure to controlled musical input–
32specifically the extent to and manner in which their responses can be considered to
33derive from the stimuli with which they are presented–provide powerful evidence of
34the cognitive processes involved [2]. In the longer term, it is intended that our findings
35will be used to underpin new didactic approaches in working with savants (and others),
36inform the wider debate on the nature of musical ability in general and stimulate
37further research.
382. Method
39A piece was especially composed for the experiment which was entitled The
40Chromatic Blues—dCBT, whose structure is shown in Fig. 1 (a score and digital
41recording of this and other material cited in the paper are available from the first
42author).
43Statistically, CB comprises 312 events occurring within 49 s (an average rate of 6.37
44events per second). The opening four bars are shown in Fig. 2.
45CB was presented to Derek over a number of sessions which were structured as
46follows:
47
48(a) Derek plays CB as well as he is able (in all except the first session)
49(b) Derek listens to CB
50(c) Derek plays CB (again)
51(d) Derek listens to CB again
52
Fig. 1. Structure of The Chromatic Blues.
Fig. 2. Opening bars of The Chromatic Blues (nAdam Ockelford, 2003).
A. Ockelford, L. Pring / International Congress Series xx (2005) xxx–xxx2
UNCORRECTED PROOF
ARTICLE IN PRESS
53All material was performed on a Korg touch-sensitive keyboard SP-200 (using dPiano
541Tsound), recorded as MIDI and digital sound files, and reproduced using the same
55system. The pattern of sessions is shown in Fig. 3.
563. Results
57Derek’s first attempt at playing CB (immediately having heard it for the first time) began as
58follows (Fig. 4). Overall, this version (dCB-D1T) was derived from CB with a strength of derivation
59of 0.31 (using the excerpts for analysis set out in Fig. 1)[1].
60As this excerpt illustrates, it is a feature of Derek’s playing (rather like a natural language
61speaker) that what he produces invariably makes musical sense. As he was asked to play CB as well
62as he could, because he achieved a derivation index of only 0.31, we can assume that the piece was
63beyond the capacity of his working memory and that he was forced to modify, introduce or create
64material to dplug the gapsT. It appears that he did this, quite intuitively, in a number of ways. First,
65within the general style of the opening of CB (including tonality, metre and phrase structure) he
66utilised a number of subtle transformations of its musical material, including re-ordering fragments,
67and separating pitch and rhythmic elements and re-combining them in new ways. Second, he
68appeared to introduce material from other pieces in similar style including the dbass riffTfound in
69bars 1 and 2 (typical of Count Basie, for example), whose harmonic structure is based on a common
70dBlues turnaroundT, with the opening four bars outlining a design reminiscent of the chorus of Paper
71Moon. It is extremely doubtful that these apparent borrowings were part of Derek’s conscious
72thinking, but they give an insight into the possible nature of his nonconscious musical processing
73that was in train.
Fig. 3. Pattern of sessions with Derek and The Chromatic Blues.
Fig. 4. The opening four bars of Derek’s first attempt at The Chromatic Blues (dCB-D1T).
A. Ockelford, L. Pring / International Congress Series xx (2005) xxx–xxx 3
UNCORRECTED PROOF
ARTICLE IN PRESS
74Derek’s next effort at reproducing CB (in Session 2, which was separated from Session 1 by 2
75days) resulted in a further version (dCB-D2T) that was comparable to, though distinct from, CB-D1.
76Having heard CB again, Derek then reverted to CB-D1. In Session 3 (after a further 5 days) he
77returned to CB-D2 (see Fig. 5). However, his second attempt in this session revealed a distinct and
78audible transition from CB-D1 to a rendition much closer to CB (Fig. 6).
79The indices of derivation that Derek achieved over the entire period are as follows (see Fig. 7). It
80is worthy of note that there are marked improvements following the first break of a month (0.66 to
810.74) and 3 months (0.76 to 0.84), and that even following the 6 months break, a level of 0.83 is
82achieved, followed by the highest score of all (0.88) at the second attempt in that session. For
83reference, an exponential trend is indicated. 84
854. Discussion
86These preliminary data from dREMUS Study 1Treveal a number of findings that are of
87interest. First, it is evident that Derek does indeed have an exceptional memory. Another
88jazz pianist (who, like Derek, had absolute pitch and was used to learning pieces by ear)
89attempted to learn CB under precisely the same conditions and, although his data await full
90analysis, it is evident that his performance was significantly inferior to Derek’s. However,
91Derek’s recall was by no means straightforward: musical analysis suggests that initially he
92was able to remember only certain salient fragments and features—details within an
93overall outline of what had occurred. Derek’s instinct to produce something that made
94musical sense subsequently drove him to manipulate what he could remember, whereby
95the moment-to-moment demands of musical coherence apparently took precedence over
96his powers of recall. Transformations of material from CB were made as well as the
97introduction of fragments and features from stylistically congeneric sources (memories of
98which hearing CB had evidently stimulated in Derek).
99Interestingly, analysis of data beyond this paper indicates that the most potent source of
100material for Derek at any given juncture was not CB itself (even immediately after having
101heard it), but his previous renditions, suggesting a strong interference effect. This was
102particular marked in the early stages of learning, when, as we have seen, three different
103versions of CB were maintained in parallel. Indeed, in renditions 7 and 9, Derek used CB-
104D2 as an introduction to a convincing performance of CB itself!
105Clearly, then, Derek’s developing model(s) of CB was far more than a series of surface
106percepts which he was able to reproduce, automaton-like at the keyboard. Evidently,
Fig. 5. The opening four bars of CB-D2.
Fig. 6. The audible switch in rendition 7 from CB-D1 to CB.
A. Ockelford, L. Pring / International Congress Series xx (2005) xxx–xxx4
UNCORRECTED PROOF
ARTICLE IN PRESS
107despite his inability to verbalise almost anything about his musical achievements, Derek
108has a lively and engaging musical mind with considerable strengths but with human
109frailties too. His efforts are informed by an intuitive understanding of musical syntax and
110structure that would do credit to any mature musician. His ability to take material from a
111range of sources and mould it in real time into something that is of musical value in its
112own right indicates a genuine creativity that merits further research. And, following
113completion of the current study, it is to this area that the REMUS project will turn its
114attention.
115References
116
117[1] A. Ockelford, Repetition in music: Theoretical and metatheoretical perspectives. London: Ashgate.
118[2] J. Sloboda, Exploring the Musical Mind: Cognition, Emotion, Ability, Function, Oxford University Press,
119Oxford, 2005.
120
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7
0.8
0.9
1
Series1 0.31 0.18 0.18 0.19 0.41 0.49 0.66 0.6 0.66 0.74 0.75 0.73 0.74 0.74 0.73 0.77 0.7 0.76 0.84 0.87 0.83 0.88
1 2a 2b 3a 3b 4a 4b 5a 5b 6a 6b 7a 7b 8a 8b 9a 9b 10a 11a 11b 12a 12b
Fig. 7. Derek’s indices of derivation for CB over the test period (12 months).
A. Ockelford, L. Pring / International Congress Series xx (2005) xxx–xxx 5
... All these are characteristic of him in everyday life, and therefore have the ecological validity that is so particularly important in research with people with learning difficulties. The first involves hearing an entire piece of music and then trying to re-create it, a process that is re-run with increasing periods of time between sessions (see, for example, Ockelford & Pring, 2005;Ockelford, 2008aOckelford, , 2012a) -the so-called "listen and play" approach. The second procedure entails Derek endeavouring to learn a piece by hearing and attempting to replicate it in short sections, a procedure that, again, is reiterated over weeks, months and then years (Mazzeschi, 2014), and is termed "a bit at a time". ...
... The result of this (music-historically unlikely) Wagner/Brahms merger (see Figure 12) is a motif, whose secure rhythmic framework is capable of supporting fluctuating arrays of harmonies, that lends itself readily to repetition, transposition and variation. The manner in which the motif was used in global terms was determined by the need for Romantic Rollercoaster to be of the same structure as, and similar in length and complexity to, the other pieces used in the Fragment of Genius research project: Chromatic Blues (Ockelford & Pring, 2005) and Classical Turn (Mazzeschi, 2014) -see Table 1. ...
... Derek's next unanticipated success is correct prediction of the C 7 chord formed by Events 80, 81, 82, 84, which could not have been foreseen from the materials presented in Romantic Rollercoaster up to that point. However, the somewhat unusual (and therefore stylistically salient) progression of a ninth chord with a raised fourth resolving onto a seventh chord whose root is a semitone lower is one that the second author frequently uses in jazz improvisation with Derek, while a very similar progression (in which the seventh chord has an added thirteenth) is used in the first bar of Chromatic Blues (the piece that was used in the first of the longitudinal memory experiments undertaken with him; see Ockelford & Pring, 2005;Ockelford, 2012a). Hence, Derek's anticipation of the seventh chord would appear to be a case of veridical prognostication (between groups) occurring between pieces ( Figure 1); again, see Figure 32. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates expectations in music evoked during the course of hearing a piece for the first time, particularly those which stem from recently appearing groups of notes (Ockelford, 2006). A prodigious musical savant (Derek Paravicini) attempted to reproduce a novel composition on the piano at the same time as hearing it. The piece was designed to minimise the impact of the more general expectations that arise from tonality, whereby different pitch transitions are felt to occur with different probabilities according to their level of past exposure. The design of the study was informed by ‘zygonic’ theory (Ockelford, 2009, 2012b), which holds that structural regularities in music suggest future continuations, whose perceived likelihood of occurrence is proportional to the number of ways in which their existence is implied in what has gone before. Using this principle, a ‘strength of implication’ factor was calculated for each note of the stimulus piece (following the first). It was hypothesised that the higher the implication factor, the more likely Derek would predict its occurrence (and therefore play it correctly at the appropriate point in time). Data gathered from Derek’s performance support the principles of the zygonic model, although they also suggest certain refinements.
... The empirical studies in this manuscript were examined under the themes of (a) creativity of savant individuals, (b) creativity of individuals with autism spectrum disorder and Asperger's syndrome, and (c) creativity concepts and savant individuals. Studies examining the creativity of savants have revealed that savants are creative (Dowker et al, 1996;Ockelforda & Pring, 2005;Ryder et al, 2002;Strauss, 2014). Contrary to these results, a study concluded that savants exhibit limited creativity (Sarı, 2019). ...
... İncelenen çalışmalarda savantların yaratıcılıklarının sınırlı ya da düşük olduğu sonucuna ulaşan çalışmalar olmasına karşın, savantların yaratıcılıklarının yüksek olduğu sonucuna ulaşan çalışmaların sayısı daha çoktur. İncelenen araştırmalar savantların (Dowker vd., 1996;Ockelforda & Pring, 2005;Pring vd., 2011;Ryder vd., 2002;Strauss, 2014), otizm spektrum bozukluğuna sahip olan bireylerin ve Asperger sendromuna sahip olan bireylerin (Fung, 2009) yüksek düzeyde yaratıcı olduklarını belirtmiştir. Bu bulgular da literatürde savantların yaratıcı olabileceklerini savunan fikirleri destekler niteliktedir (Simonton, 2012;Treffert, 2014;Viscott, 1970). ...
Article
Full-text available
Bu araştırmada savant bireylerin yaratıcılıklarına dair literatürde varolan çalışmaların incelenmesi, otizm spektrum bozukluğu olan bireylerin, Asperger sendromuna sahip olan bireylerin ve savantların yaratıcılıklarının sonucunda ne gibi bulgulara ulaşıldığının belirlenmesi amaçlanmıştır. Bu amaçla yaratıcılık ve savant anahtar kelimeleri kullanılarak sistematik bir literatür taraması yapılmıştır. Anahtar kelimeler sosyal bilimler alanında yaygın olan veri tabanlarında kullanılarak taramalar yapılmıştır. Ulaşılan ampirik çalışmalar belirli kriterlere göre araştırmaya dahil edilmiş, belirli kriterlere göre ise dışlanmıştır. Yapılan taramalar sonucunda ulaşılan çalışmalar savantların yaratıcılıkları, otizm spektrum bozukluğuna sahip olan bireylerin ve Asperger sendromuna sahip bireylerin yaratıcılıkları ve yaratıcılık konseptleri temaları oluşturularak incelenmiştir. İncelenen çalışmalarda katılımcıların yaratıcılıklarını değerlendirmek amacıyla standart testler ve ürün odaklı değerlendirmeler kullanıldığı gözlemlenmiştir. İncelemeler sonucunda otizm spektrum bozukluğu olan bireylerin, Asperger sendromuna sahip olan bireylerin ve savantların yaratıcılıklarının yüksek olduğu görülmüştür. Savantların yaratıcılıklarına dair ulaşılan sonuçlar alanyazınla tartışılmıştır.
... They include extraordinary memory for numbers and facts (Hu, Ericsson, Yang, & Lu, 2009;Treffert, 2010), rapid mental mathematical calculation (Anderson, O'Connor, & Hermelin, 1999;Fehr, Weber, Willmes, & Herrmann, 2010), mental calendar calculation (Dubischar-Krivec et al., 2009;Fehr, Wallace, Erhard, & Herrmann, 2011;Heaton & Wallace, 2004), and extraordinary autobiographical memory (Kennedy & Squire, 2007;. Savant/ prodigious skills also include accurate music replication after hearing a musical piece a single time (McPherson, 2007;Ockelford & Pring, 2005), and drawing an image or scene with detailed fidelity after viewing it for a single time (Crane, Pring, Ryder, & Hermelin, 2011;Drake & Winner, 2009;Golomb, 1999). ...
... The violinist Mischa Elman played the violin with beauty and control at the age of 4, and Keng-Yuen Tseng, a department chair at the Peabody Conservatory in the USA, had memorized all 42 Kreutzer studies by the time he turned 7 years old (Nelson, 2009). Ockelford and Pring (2005) tested Derek Paravacini, a British music savant not diagnosed with autism, and found he had exceptional memory for music, beyond even that of a comparable music prodigy. Young and Nettelbeck (1995) tested a music savant with autism. ...
Article
Full-text available
This extended article introduces Adam Ockelford’s ‘zygonic’ theory of music-structural understanding, which holds that imitation, which can occur in all domains of perceived sound and at all levels, is the ultimate organising force in music. Hence the theory is potentially of value not only in theoretical terms (shown here in relation to the first movement of Mozart’s Piano Sonata K. 333), but metatheoretically too, as a tool to interrogate other systems of musical analysis (an example is provided in relation to Allan Forte’s ‘set-theoretical’ method). The zygonic approach also enables the powers of influence at work in group improvisation to be captured, permitting the evolution of musical ideas to be charted as they unfold in time between performers, and a zygonic analysis of a short, improvised song with piano accompaniment is provided by way of illustration. Finally, zygonic theory prospectively offers an epistemological link between the sister (though sometimes apparently incompatible) disciplines of music psychology and music theory – an avenue that is explored briefly in conclusion.
Article
Disability, understood as culturally stigmatized bodily difference (including physical and mental impairments of all kinds), is a pervasive and permanent aspect of the human condition. While the biology of bodily difference is the proper study for science and medicine, the meaning that we attach to bodily difference is the proper study of humanists. The interdisciplinary field of Disability Studies has recently emerged to theorize social and cultural constructions of the meaning of disability. Although there has been an astonishing outpouring of humanistic work in Disability Studies in the past ten years, there has been virtually no echo in musicology or music theory. Sounding Off: Theorizing Disability in Music is the first book-length work to focus on the historical and theoretical issues of music as it relates to disability. It shows that music, like literature and the other arts, simultaneously reflects and constructs cultural attitudes toward disability. Sounding Off: Theorizing Disability in Music promises to be a landmark study for scholars and students of music, disability, and culture.
Article
This book re-examines the case of Nadia, discovered as a child aged six, who had been drawing with phenomenal skill and visual realism from the age of three, despite having autism and severe learning difficulties. The original research was published in 1977 and caused great international interest. Nadia Revisited updates her story and reconsiders the theories that endeavour to explain her extraordinary talent.
Article
There is a near hysteria over dramatic increases in autism prevalence at a time when we still don't know what causes the disorder and have no known cures. More and more money is being poured into autism research with conflicting research findings, none of which appear to be getting us closer to prevention or treatment. This book carefully examines the psychology, neuroscience, genetic, and medical research findings to date from autism research and provocatively suggests that what we've labeled "autism" is several different disorders, and the way forward for more effective research for prevention and treatment. Autism is a massive worldwide problem with increasing prevalence rates, now thought to be as high as 1 in 38 children (Korea) and 1 in 100 children (CDC- US) Autism is the 3rd most common developmental disability. 400,000 people in the US alone have autism Autism effects entire brain including communication, social behavior, reasoning and is lifelong There is no known cause and no cure Funding for Autism research quadrupled from 1995 to 2000 up to $45m, and the Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee has recommended $ 1billion funding from 2010-2015.
Article
Although research in music psychology, education and therapy has expanded exponentially in the 21st century, there is something of a 'black hole' around which much of the discourse circles: music itself. While writers have largely been occupied with what people think about musical engagement, the little musical analysis that exists has tended to be at a low level compared to the sophisticated non-musical exploration that is present. This highlights the tenuous connection between musical enquiry in the context of the humanities and that occurring within the social sciences, the one exception being the partial intersection of music theory and psychology. Here, however, progress has largely been in one direction, with something of the objectivity that characterizes psychological research reading across to music analysis, and taking the form of what has been called 'empirical musicology'. This book takes a further, reciprocal step, in which certain of the techniques of empirical musicology (in particular, the author's 'zygonic' theory) are used to inform thinking in the domains of music-psychological, educational and therapeutic research. A new, interdisciplinary sphere of endeavour is sketched out, for which the term 'applied musicology' is coined. The book adopts a phenomenological, inductive approach, using the analysis of hundreds of real-life examples of musical engagement and interaction to build new theories of musical intentionality and influence, and to shed new light on our understanding of aspects of music perception and cognition. This book is intended to lay the foundations upon which a new category of interdisciplinary work will be built.
Book
Since the publication in 1986 of the book The Musical Mind, music psychology has developed as a vibrant area of research, exerting influence on areas as diverse as music education and cognitive neuroscience. This new book brings together twenty-three chapters and reviews on music and the mind, focusing on ideas, interpretation, argument and application rather than on technical issues.************************************************** The book is a curated compilation of single-author chapters and articles published elsewhere which mainly do not report primary data but rather reflect on broader issues which research in the field of music psychology raises. Each chapter (excepting Chapter 23 specially written for this volume) is therefore the original copyright of a different publisher, and this compilation thus cannot be shared in open access. Some Individual chapters may be available online, others may be shareable privately on request.******************************************* Part A Cognitive Processes 1 The Psychology of Music Reading 2 Experimental Studies of Music Reading: A Review 3 The Uses of Space in Music Notation 4 Immediate Recall of Melodies 5 Cognition and Real Music: The Psychology of Music Comes of Age 6 Psychological Structures in Music: Core Research 1980–1990 7 Review of Language, Music, And Mind by Diana Raffman 8 Does Music Mean Anything? Part B Emotion and Motivation 9 Music as a Language 10 Music Psychology and the Composer 11 Empirical Studies of Emotional Response to Music 12 Emotional Response to Music: A Review 13 Musical Performance and Emotion: Issues and Developments Part C Talent and Skill Development 14 Musical Expertise 15 Musical Ability 16 The Acquisition of Musical Performance Expertise: Deconstructing the ‘Talent’ Account of Individual Differences in Musical Expressivity 17 Are some Children more Gifted for Music than Others? Part D Music in the Real World 18 Everyday Uses of Music Listening: A Preliminary Study 19 Music: Where Cognition and Emotion Meet 20 Music and Worship: A Psychologists Perspective 21 Emotion, Functionality, and the Everyday Experience of Music: Where does Music Education Fit? 22 The ‘Sound of Music Versus the Essence of Music’: Dilemmas for Music-Emotion Researchers: Commentary on Current Trends in the Study of Music and Emotion, a Special Issue of Musicae Scientiae (2001–2) Edited by Patrik N Juslin and Marcel R ZentnerG 23 Assessing Music Psychology Research: Values, Priorities, and Outcomes