ArticlePDF Available

Modulation of pheromone trail strength with food quality in Pharaoh's ant, Monomorium pharaonis

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Pheromone trails are self-organized processes, where colony-level behaviour emerges from the activity of many individuals responding to local information. The Pharaoh's ant is an important model species for investigating pheromone trails. Here we show that Pharaoh's ant foragers mark with trail pheromones, using their stinger, on both the outward and return leg of foraging trips. Examination of trail markings showed that 10.5% of returning fed ants simply made marks by dragging their engorged gaster, because stinger marks were absent. After discounting gaster-dragging hair marks we found that fed ants (42.5%) did not mark significantly more frequently than unfed ants (36.0%). However, we found that trail-marking fed ants marked pheromone trails with a significantly greater intensity, as compared to trail-marking unfed ants, if the food source was high quality (1.0 M sucrose). When the food quality was low (0.01 M sucrose) we detected no significant difference in marking intensity between fed and unfed trail-marking ants. Our results show that in Pharaoh's ants individual trail marking occurs at a frequency of ∼40% among fed and unfed foragers, but the frequency of individuals marking with high intensity (continuous marking) is significantly greater when a food source is high quality. This contrasts with another model species, Lasius niger, where trail strength is modulated by an all-or-nothing individual response to food quality. The reason for this fundamental difference in mechanism is that Pharaoh's ant is highly reliant on pheromone trails for environmental orientation, so must produce trails, whereas L. niger is proficient at visual-based orientation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Modulation of pheromone trail strength with food quality
in Pharaoh’s ant, Monomorium pharaonis
DUNCAN E. JACKSON*&NICOLASCHA
ˆLINE†‡
*Department of Computer Science, University of Sheffield
yDepartment of Animal and Plant Sciences, University of Sheffield
zLEEC, UMR CNRS 7153, Universite
´Paris 13, 93430 Villetaneuse, France
(Received 22 June 2006; initial acceptance 19 September 2006;
final acceptance 16 November 2006; published online 22 August 2007; MS. number: 9009R)
Pheromone trails are self-organized processes, where colony-level behaviour emerges from the activity of
many individuals responding to local information. The Pharaoh’s ant is an important model species for
investigating pheromone trails. Here we show that Pharaoh’s ant foragers mark with trail pheromones, us-
ing their stinger, on both the outward and return leg of foraging trips. Examination of trail markings
showed that 10.5% of returning fed ants simply made marks by dragging their engorged gaster, because
stinger marks were absent. After discounting gaster-dragging hair marks we found that fed ants (42.5%)
did not mark significantly more frequently than unfed ants (36.0%). However, we found that trail-marking
fed ants marked pheromone trails with a significantly greater intensity, as compared to trail-marking unfed
ants, if the food source was high quality (1.0 M sucrose). When the food quality was low (0.01 M sucrose)
we detected no significant difference in marking intensity between fed and unfed trail-marking ants. Our
results show that in Pharaoh’s ants individual trail marking occurs at a frequency of w40% among fed and
unfed foragers, but the frequency of individuals marking with high intensity (continuous marking) is
significantly greater when a food source is high quality. This contrasts with another model species, Lasius
niger, where trail strength is modulated by an all-or-nothing individual response to food quality. The rea-
son for this fundamental difference in mechanism is that Pharaoh’s ant is highly reliant on pheromone
trails for environmental orientation, so must produce trails, whereas L. niger is proficient at visual-based
orientation.
Ó2007 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: communication; Monomorium pharaonis; Pharaoh’s ant; pheromone trail; self-organization
The foraging trails of ants are often presented as a para-
digm of self-organization (e.g. Camazine et al. 2001). The
behavioural feedback system found in foraging trails per-
fectly illustrates the principle of emergence, where the
activities of many agents responding only to local infor-
mation leads to a global adaptive process. Simple mathe-
matical and computational models have been used to
show how adaptive global solutions can arise in such a sys-
tem, where a trail pheromone provides positive feedback.
However, from a biological perspective, models may have
led to an oversimplified view of this process. Increasingly
research is uncovering great sophistication in the use
of pheromone communication throughout ant trail net-
works. For example, most trail-following ants actually use
multiple trail pheromones secreted from one or more
glands (Witte & Maschwitz 2002; Jackson et al. 2006). Fur-
thermore, behavioural specialization has been found in
the response to particular pheromones and in their selec-
tive deposition (Jackson et al. 2006; Hart & Jackson
2006). Understanding how pheromone trails work clearly
demands a deeper investigation of the behaviour of indi-
vidual ants throughout the foraging process.
The Pharaoh’s ant is an important model species for
conducting empirical research into pheromone trails.
Foraging trails produced by Pharaoh’s ant form complex
branching networks that can extend up to 10 m from the
nest (Sudd 1960). The characteristic geometrical structure
of trail networks is exploited as an aid in orientation,
whilst the long-lived nature of the pheromone used in
Correspondence: D. E. Jackson, Department of Computer Science, Uni-
versity of Sheffield, Regent Court, 211 Portobello Street, Sheffield S1
4DP, U.K. (email: duncan@dcs.sheffield.ac.uk).
463
0003e3472/07/$30.00/0 Ó2007 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
AN IM AL B EH A VI OU R, 2007, 74, 463e470
doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.11.027
networks facilitates rapid exploration of the environment
from day-to-day (Jackson et al. 2004, 2006). Pheromone
trails of Pharaoh’s ant also contain a short-lived compo-
nent that lasts for only 25 min (Jeanson et al. 2003),
which contrasts with the long-lived component that lasts
for over 2 days (Jackson et al. 2006). Seven pheromones
known to elicit trail following in Pharaoh’s ant have
been chemically isolated and synthesized. Monomorines,
isolated from the poison glands, have trail following activ-
ity at concentrations as low as 10
8
g/cm, with blends of
monomorines I and III being the most active (Ritter
et al. 1977a). Faranal, found in trace amounts in the Du-
four’s glands (Ritter et al. 1977b), is active at concentra-
tion as low as 10
12
g/cm, although maximal activity
occurs at 10
9
g/cm (Ritter et al. 1977b; Ho
¨lldobler &
Wilson 1990). Trail pheromones are deposited on surfaces
using the extruded stinger. Both the poison gland and the
Dufour’s gland empty through the stinger, but secretions
from either gland can be deposited separately (or simulta-
neously) because both glands possess reservoirs with
sphincters to control release (Billen 1987).
It is frequently assumed that ants only lay pheromone
after discovering food, whilst they are returning to the
nest. This is only true of a returning ant that can navigate
back to the nest using her individual orientation skills,
such as memory of visual landmarks, path integration or
a sun compass (Ho
¨lldobler & Wilson 1990). Once the suc-
cessful forager returns to the nest its pheromone trail can
then guide recruited ants to the food source. However,
many ant species are reliant on pheromone trails for nav-
igation during foraging and are obliged to lay trail pher-
omone to guide them back to the nest. Pharaoh’s ant is
reliant on pheromone trails for orientation in the forag-
ing environment. Foragers on active Pharaoh’s ant trails
are known to deposit trail pheromone both when fed
and unfed, and when walking to or from the nest (Hart
& Jackson 2006). It has been observed that Pharaoh’s
ant produces ‘exploratory’ trails in the absence of food,
but only after the discovery of food are such trails termed
foraging trails, or ‘exploitation’ trails (Fourcassie
´& De-
neubourg 1994). This terminology is confusing and sug-
gests that there are qualitative differences between
exploratory trails and foraging trails, where none has
been shown.
A major problem in understanding pheromone trail
dynamics is determining the contribution individual ants
make to a pheromone trail and how that contribution
leads to the colony-level selection of food sources. Pre-
vious studies have suggested that fed ants contribute most
to trails but these studies have relied on indirect measures
of trail deposition, particularly behavioural observation of
body postures such as gaster curling that might indicate
trail marking (Beckers et al. 1993; Mailleux et al. 2000). We
determined the contribution that fed and unfed Pharaoh’s
ant foragers made to pheromone trails, but used a direct
measure of pheromone deposition. We studied the physi-
cal marks made by ants walking on a smoked glass surface
(Hangartner 1969; Jackson et al. 2004). Using this direct
measure we analysed trail-marking frequency on active
foraging trails and the intensity of marking with variable
food quality.
METHODS
Study Species
Four study colonies of Pharaoh’s ant (Formicidae:
Myrmicinae) each contained 1200e2500 workers, brood
of all stages and multiple queens (12e50). Pharaoh’s ants
are small (ca. 2 mm in length), monomorphic ants and
easily maintained in the laboratory. Colonies were
housed in wooden nestboxes (11 8 cm and 2 cm high)
held within a large plastic foraging box (45 30 cm and
15 cm high) in a climate controlled room (24 2C,
RH ¼30%, 12:12 h light:dark cycle). Colonies were given
fresh water ad libitum in glass tubes sealed with cotton
wool and fed with Tenebrio larvae, sugar syrup, dried liver
and dried egg yolk.
Trail-marking Frequency
To determine the frequency of pheromone trail marking
when walking to and from a feeder, we constrained ants to
produce pheromone trails in a narrow corridor placed
upon smoked glass (Fig. 1). A sheet of toughened glass
(74 28 cm) was held over a wax candle flame so as to
coat it with a fine layer of soot. A glass sheet was then
placed on top of a clear glass tank. A colony accessed
one edge of the smoked glass via a bridge, which led to
a corridor (1 cm internal width) made of two polycarbon-
ate strips (60 4 cm and 0.5 cm). The inner surfaces of the
corridor were coated with Fluon to prevent ants from
climbing them. A syrup feeder was placed at the far end
of the corridor. Observations began when colonies had
successfully established foraging trails, which we opera-
tionally defined as when 20 ants had returned from the
feeder. This was to ensure pheromone trails were not
heavily marked before we began observations, but also
to ensure a continuous trail had been established. In the
early stages of the trail establishment process the trails
of ants are still very weak, or possibly discontinuous,
and trail following behaviour on such unestablished trails
may differ from that on strong, established trails. We
chose the point after which 20 ants had returned from
the feeder because trail traffic was regular at this point, in-
dicating trail establishment.
Strong light sources were placed above and below the
corridor to facilitate observation of marking by individual
ants. Individuals were observed as they walked the full
length of the corridor from the feeder to the bridge or vice
versa. The corridor usually contained multiple ants,
because this was an active trail. Focal ants were randomly
selected as they either left the feeder, or stepped off the
bridge into the corridor, and they were followed until they
either reached the bridge (fed) or feeder (unfed). We
observed each ant continually using a 20magnifying
hand lens, which was moved along the top of the corridor
as the focal ant walked along. We paid close attention to
the smoked glass for any markings made by the focal ant.
Any trail marking was recorded (excluding footprint
marks). We observed 50 ants walking towards the food
source (unfed) and 50 ants returning to the nest from the
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 74,3
464
feeder (fed). This procedure was repeated for four colonies
using a fresh sheet of smoked glass for each colony.
Trail Marking
We examined the isolated trail markings of individual
ants to discriminate between different types of marking
and also to measure the intensity of trail marking. We
modified the apparatus shown in Fig. 1 and made the cor-
ridor 2.5 cm wide. Once colonies had established a foraging
trail (>20 ants returned from the 1.0 M sugar syrup feeder)
we placed a smoked glass microscopy slide (7.5 2.5 cm)
lengthways in the middle of the widened foraging corridor.
When an ant stepped on the slide, the slide was carefully
removed from the corridor using forceps and elevated to
a height of approximately 20 cm. The slide was kept in
a horizontal position to minimize disturbance to the ant.
We removed the slide to ensure only one ant walked on
the slide. We allowed the ant to walk across the slide until
it reached an edge at which point the ant was shaken from
the slide. This procedure was repeated using four colonies
in total, and for each colony we tested 50 unfed ants and
50 fed ants. A fresh slide was used for each ant tested.
The same experimental operator always performed the
slide manipulation so as to eliminate operator bias. Each
slide was suspended above a bright light source and exam-
ined with a 20magnifying lens.
Using Hangartner’s (1969) scheme we classified the
markings made by individual Pharaoh’s ants. Hangartner
(1969) showed that the fire ant, Solenopsis geminata, pro-
duces easily classified trail markings on smoked glass, de-
pending on whether the stinger is extended and the
degree of contact with the substrate. We also measured
the distance marked per unit distance walked upon the
slide. Markings were classified as either trail markings
(continuous streaks and discontinuous dots); hair mark-
ings produced by gaster dragging; or footprints. Combina-
tions of these markings were also observed. Intensity of
trail marking was measured as the proportion of the total
distance walked on the slide when trail markings were
made. To do this we placed transparent plastic over the
back-lit slide and traced the path of the ant onto the plas-
tic with a pen. Some ants made sinuous paths across the
slide, so intensity was normalized to give total trail
marked as a proportion of total distance walked (trail
marking per 1 cm walked).
A further measure of intensity, also first noted by Hangart-
ner (1969), is whether ants mark with continuous lines or
discontinuous dots. These different markings indicate vari-
ability in pressure applied by the stinger to a surface and
this determines the amount of pheromone likely to be
deposited. We quantified the number of fed and unfed
ants that marked continuously or discontinuously on slides.
Trail Marking with Low Quality Food Source
We repeated this trail marking investigation procedure
with a low quality (0.01 M) sugar syrup feeder using two
colonies, each time for 50 fed and 50 unfed ants.
Classification of Trail Markings
We produced images of the different markings made by
Pharaoh’s ant by allowing a single colony to access a sheet
of smoked glass using a bridge. The set-up used was as
shown in Fig. 1 but the corridor was omitted, which al-
lowed ants to forage without constraint. The smoked glass
sheet was removed after 2 h and cleared of ants by shak-
ing. We then produced an image of trail markings on
the glass by scanning it using a Canon Canoscan Lide
Scanner, with backlighting from a Canon slide copier
illuminator.
RESULTS
Trail-marking Frequency
Table 1 shows the trail-marking frequency of fed and
unfed ants from four test colonies. We found no signifi-
cant colony effect (two-way ANOVA: F
3,392
¼0.32,
P¼0.814) on marking frequency, but found a highly
significant effect of fed/unfed status (F
1,392
¼15.81,
Nestbox
Syrup feeder
Corridor
Bridge
Foraging box
Light source
Light source
Glass tank
Smoked glass sheet
Figure 1. Experimental apparatus used for investigation of pheromone trails on smoked glass. Ants accessed the corridor leading to the sugar
syrup feeder, via a bridge from the foraging box. The corridor constrained foraging ants to form a straight trail to the feeder on the smoked
glass surface, whilst strong illumination from both above and below facilitated observation of trail marking. For closer examination of trail mark-
ings a smoked glass slide (7.5 2.5 cm) was placed lengthways in the centre of the corridor and removed with forceps when an ant stepped
onto it. An ant was allowed to walk on the slide until it reached an edge. Fifty fed and 50 unfed ants from each of four colonies were tested.
JACKSON & CHA
ˆLINE: ANT PHEROMONE TRAIL MARKING 465
P<0.001). There was no significant interaction effect be-
tween colony and fed/unfed status (colony*fed/unfed:
F
3,392
¼0.84, P¼0.471). Overall (colonies pooled) 54.5%
of fed ants made trail markings compared to only 35.0%
unfed ants marked.
Trail Markings
Categories of pheromone trail markings made by Phar-
aoh’s ant are shown in Fig. 2. Markings made by foragers
took the form of discontinuous trails (spots: Fig. 2d) con-
tinuous trails (streaks: Fig. 2b, f), footprints (Fig. 2c), hair
marks (Fig. 2e) or combinations of these markings.
When we examined markings made on unconstrained
smoked glass (no corridor) we also found that the regions
between trails (Fig. 2a) were marked with discontinuous
sections of trail and infrequent spots indicating sparing
deposition of trail pheromone.
Markings made in trials by four colonies are presented in
Table 2. The categories we used for classification of mark-
ings were trail markings (continuous and discontinuous
stinger markings or combined stinger plus hair markings);
hair marks alone; and footprints alone (no trail marking).
When hair marks were included as trail markings results
were very similar to those shown in Table 1. We found no
significant colony effect (two-way ANOVA: F
3,392
¼0.19,
P¼0.903) on marking frequency, but found a highly
significant effect of fed/unfed status (F
1,392
¼10.45,
P¼0.001). There was no significant interaction effect be-
tween colony and fed/unfed status (colony*fed/unfed:
F
3,392
¼0.35, P¼0.786). However, when we excluded
hair marking as trail markings we found there was no
significant difference in the trail marking (stinger marks)
frequency of fed and unfed ants (F
1,392
¼1.75, P¼0.186).
We found no significant colony effect (F
3,392
¼0.34,
P¼0.795) and there was no significant interaction effect
between colony and fed/unfed status (colony*fed/unfed:
F
3,392
¼0.29, P¼0.835). Markings with abdomen hairs
(stinger markings absent) were made by 10.5% of all
fed ants (colonies pooled) compared to only 1% of all unfed
ants (colonies pooled), and were a consequence of fed ants
dragging their engorged gaster.
We found a significant difference in intensity of mark-
ing between trail-marking fed and unfed ants, using two
separate measures. We found that fed ants laid trail
(stinger marking) with a slight but significantly greater
intensity (as proportion of distance marked) compared to
unfed trail-laying ants (Student’s ttest: t¼2.06,
P¼0.044). Unfed ants made trail markings for 0.64 cm
(SD ¼0.36) of every 1.0 cm walked, whilst fed ants
marked 0.75 cm per 1.0 cm (SD ¼0.31). In our second
measure of marking intensity we found that trail-marking
fed ants made continuous markings on glass slides signif-
icantly more frequently (72.9%) than trail-marking unfed
(50.0%) ants (chi-square test: c2
1¼8:75, N¼157,
P¼0.003).
Trail Marking with Low Quality Food Source
Table 3 shows results obtained when ants foraged to
a 0.01 M feeder. We found no significant effect of fed/
unfed status (two-way ANOVA: F
1,196
¼0.33, P¼0.564)
on marking frequency. There was no colony effect
(F
1,196
¼0.75, P¼0.387) and no significant interaction
effect between colony and fed/unfed status (colony*fed/
unfed: F
1,196
¼0.08, P¼0.773). The intensity of marking
as measured in marking per unit distance did not differ
significantly between fed (0.68 cm marked per 1 cm,
SD ¼0.41, N¼40) and unfed (0.60 cm marked per 1 cm,
SD ¼0.38, N¼36) ants, with the low quality (0.01 M)
feeder (Student’s ttest: t¼0.879, P¼0.382). In our second
analysis of intensity we found that only 55% of trail-
marking fed ants (22/40) marked continuously, which
was not significantly different to the frequency of contin-
uously marking unfed trail-marking ants (16/36 ¼44.4%;
chi-square test: c2
1¼0:844, N¼76, P¼0.358).
The frequency of trail-marking fed ants that continu-
ously marked when a 1.0 M feeder was present (62/
85 ¼72.9%) was significantly greater than when
a 0.01 M feeder (22/40 ¼55%) was used (chi-square test:
c2
1¼3:962, N¼125, P¼0.046). We found no difference
between the proportions of unfed trail-marking ants that
continuously marked with the two different feeder quali-
ties (c2
1¼0:297, N¼108, P¼0.586). Our results suggested
that w50% of unfed trail-marking ants always marked
trails with a consistent intensity, but that trail-marking
fed ants increased the intensity of their marking as a func-
tion of feeder quality. This increased intensity was evident
in an increased proportionate distance for which trail was
laid and also in an increased proportion of ants marking
continuously, when a 1.0 M feeder was used.
DISCUSSION
Our data confirm that Pharaoh’s ant foragers mark pher-
omone trails whilst walking to food and the nest. Initially,
we found that the frequency of trail marking was higher in
fed ants (54.5%) than unfed ants (35.0%). However, 10.5%
of fed ants were making parallel hair markings (Fig. 2e) by
dragging their engorged gaster. These hair markings were
not accompanied by marking with the stinger, which is
the source of all trail pheromones in Pharaoh’s ant
Table 1. Frequency of pheromone trail marking (þ) by individual
ants foraging within a 60 cm corridor placed on a sheet of smoked
glass
Unfed Fed
Trail marks
þ
Trail marks
Trail marks
þ
Trail marks
Colony 1 15 35 27 23
Colony 2 17 33 29 21
Colony 3 20 30 23 27
Colony 4 18 32 30 20
All colonies 70 (35%) 130 (65%) 109
(54.5%)
91 (45.5%)
We recorded no trail marking () when only footprints were added
to markings already present in the soot. We observed 50 fed ants
walking to the nest and 50 unfed ants walking to the food source
from each of four colonies.
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 74,3
466
(monomorine trail pheromones were chemically unde-
tectable in hair markings, D.E. Jackson, unpublished
data). When hair markings were excluded from the analy-
sis we found fed and unfed ants marked trails with equal
frequency. We found a small, but significant, difference
in the intensity of trail marking, where trail-marking fed
ants laid trail for a greater proportion of the distance
they walked on a slide. However, a more significant mea-
sure of intensity was that 50% of unfed marking ants
laid discontinuous trail markings, but only 17.1% of fed
marking ants made such intermittent markings. Fed and
unfed Pharaoh’s ant workers laid trail with equal probabil-
ity on an established trail to a high quality (1.0 M sucrose)
feeder, but trail-marking fed ants laid trail with a much
greater intensity. When a low quality food source
(0.01 M sucrose) was present fed and unfed ants marked
with the same frequency and the same intensity.
With a 1.0 M sucrose feeder we found that overall 39.3%
of foragers (fed and unfed pooled) made trail markings in-
dicative of pheromone deposition, which closely matches
the proportion of ants (43.0%) found to make frequent
U-turns on trails in the study of Hart & Jackson (2006).
U-turners were shown to be specialized ants with a high
probability (88%) of trail marking. In Lasius niger, a species
where trail-laying behaviour (not physical marking) has
been extensively investigated, trail-laying frequency varies
from 34% to 77% on inbound and outbound legs of forag-
ing trips, but the frequency declines as foraging bouts
Figure 2. Markings formed by a Monomorium pharaonis colony foraging on a large (39.2 27.6 cm) sheet of smoked glass, but without the
corridor shown in Fig. 1. (a) Discontinuous, spotted markings typical of off-trail regions (bar ¼0.25 mm); (b) continuous streaked markings
that form a main trail (bar ¼1.0 mm); (c) area of intense footprint marking only (bar ¼0.20 mm); (d) long section of spotted trail
(bar ¼0.15 mm); (e) parallel markings made by hairs whilst dragging the abdomen (bar ¼0.02 mm); and (f) continuous, unbroken streak
typical of main trail (bar ¼0.25 mm).
JACKSON & CHA
ˆLINE: ANT PHEROMONE TRAIL MARKING 467
progress (Detrain et al. 2001; Mailleux et al. 2005). It seems
unlikely that a similar decline in marking frequency would
be found during Pharaoh’s ant foraging bouts. This is be-
cause pheromone trails are essential for the orientation
of Pharaoh’s ant, when searching for food and transporting
food back to the nest. Because of this reliance on trails
Pharaoh’s ant forms an ‘exploratory trail’ network when-
ever new territory is encountered (Fourcassie
´& Deneu-
bourg 1994). Sections of these exploratory trails become
active ‘exploitation’ trails once food is discovered. How-
ever, this frequently used distinction between ‘explor-
atory’ and ‘exploitation’ trails is an artificial one and
a source of confusion. In every respect all pheromone trails
formed by foragers are simply foraging trails, whether they
currently lead to food sources or not. Pharaoh’s ant is ob-
liged to maintain pheromone trails throughout foraging
persists but L. niger can use visual cues, such as landmarks
and polarised light, to navigate between the nest and food
(Aron et al. 1993). We suggest that the proportion of indi-
viduals engaged in trail marking does not vary in Pharaoh’s
ant because pheromone trails are so essential for
orientation.
We found that trail marking intensity varied signifi-
cantly between fed and unfed Pharaoh’s ant foragers with
a 1.0 M feeder, but there was no difference in intensity of
marking, between fed and unfed ants, when a 0.01 M
feeder was present. In L. niger the probability of trail mark-
ing in response to a food source varies idiosyncratically be-
tween individuals as a function of the food quality
(Mailleux et al. 2005). Individuals will only lay trail pher-
omone if the quality of a food source satisfies, or exceeds,
their personal selection criteria (Mailleux et al. 2000).
Thus if a food source is of poor quality, fewer individuals
will choose to mark and the pheromone trail will be
weak. In L. niger this all-or-nothing response is the key
component of trail strength modulation (Mailleux et al.
2003). Our results with Pharaoh’s ant are similar to the sit-
uation found in the fire ant, Solenopsis geminata, where
a constant proportion of individuals mark with trail pher-
omone, but they can modulate trail strength through vari-
able intensity of deposition. Trail deposition intensity is
modulated by varying the pressure of stinger contact
with the surface (Hangartner 1969). The result is that
either strong, continuous marks or weak, discontinuous
Table 2. Intensity and frequency of trail marking by individual ants on smoked glass slides
Unfed Fed
Footprints
only Trail marks
Trail marked
per cm
Hair marks
only
Footprints
only Trail marks
Trail marked
per cm
Hair marks
only
Colony 1 30 19 0.620.33 1 22 20 0.730.32 8
Colony 2 32 17 0.660.37 1 25 19 0.820.28 6
Colony 3 34 16 0.610.39 0 22 23 0.740.31 5
Colony 4 30 20 0.660.36 0 25 23 0.710.32 2
Total 126 72 d29485d21
Mean
(%)SD
633.82 363.65 0.640.36 11.15 473.46 42.54.12 0.750.31 10.55.00
Food source was a 1.0 M sucrose syrup feeder. Fifty fed (returning) and 50 unfed (outbound) foraging ants, from each of four colonies were
observed (fed ants: total N¼200; unfed ants: total N¼200). Trail markings were those made either by the stinger alone (continuous streaks,
short dashes or dots) or a combination of hair and stinger markings. No trail marking was characterised by footprints alone. Hair markings were
those made by abdomen hairs alone and were a consequence of engorged ants dragging their gaster. For trail-laying ants (stinger marking) we
measured the proportion of the total distance walked in crossing the slide where trail was laid, as a measure of intensity (distance trail laid per
cm walked).
Table 3. Intensity and frequency of trail marking by individual ants on smoked glass slides, where the food source was a 0.01 M sucrose syrup
feeder
Unfed Fed
Footprints
only Trail marks
Trail marked
per cm
Hair marks
only
Footprints
only Trail marks
Trail marked
per cm
Hair marks
only
Colony 1 28 20 0.600.38 2 27 21 0.690.38 2
Colony 2 34 16 0.590.38 0 29 19 0.670.43 2
Total 62 36 d25640d4
Mean
(%)SD
628.5 365.7 0.600.38 22.8 562.8 402.8 0.680.41 40
Fifty fed (returning) and 50 unfed (outbound) foraging ants, from each of two colonies were observed (fed ants: total N¼100; unfed ants:
total N¼100).
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 74,3
468
(spotted) marks are made. The surface area of the stinger
making contact with the substrate determines the volume
of pheromone deposited. In S. geminata the intensity of in-
dividual marking was shown to be modulated with food
quality, food distance and volume consumed. As with
Pharaoh’s ant, foragers of S. geminata are reliant on trails
for orientation. Our results show that trail pheromone
must be continually deposited throughout foraging bouts
in species that rely on trails for orientation. A variation in
trail marking intensity with food quality is found in Phar-
aoh’s ant, where fed ants marked trail with greatest inten-
sity but this intensity was not different to unfed when
a lower quality food source was present. Our results sug-
gest that individual responses to food must be the key to
modulating trail strength in Pharaoh’s ant. This contrasts
with L. niger, where individual choice as to whether they
mark, or not, is most important.
The two different methods of modulating trail phero-
mone concentration have important consequences for the
performance of ant colonies when faced with different
foraging challenges. But modulation of trail concentration
through variation of individual intensity of marking and
all-or-nothing individual choice both achieve the same
end for the colony; a graded response to food quality.
However, for an obligate (or highly reliant) trail-following
ant species a modulation of trail marking intensity is the
only viable option. But how does this affect foraging
performance? Sumpter & Beekman (2003) showed that
Pharaoh’s ant selected the better of two food sources in
14 out of 18 trials and suggested that ‘the amount of pher-
omone that the ants deposit.depends on the strength of
the food sources’. We have confirmed this suggestion and
shown that the mechanism is quite different from that
found in the model species L. niger. Likewise Beckers
et al. (1993) found that L. niger selected the best feeder
in 12 out of 14 trials. So both strategies perform almost
equally when presented with a simple binary choice be-
tween two unequal food sources. However, a challenge
of greater importance from an ecological viewpoint is
the ability to select and concentrate efforts on the best
available resource in a changing, competitive environ-
ment. In L. niger switching to a better resource is either
very slow, or never occurs, when the introduction is after
an initial resource has been selected (Beckers et al. 1990).
Lasius niger usually becomes ‘stuck’ on an initial poor
resource even though a better one is available. In L. niger
visual memory of routes play a major part in foraging so
switching from one source to another may require more
than the decay of a pheromone trail. Sudd (1960) showed
that Pharaoh’s ant is slow to redirect workers when better
food resources are made available, but colonies can usually
do so in about an hour. However, Sudd (1960) was work-
ing in a natural environment with trails of up to 10 m
in length, compared to trails typically of less than 1 m
used in experimental studies of L. niger (Beckers et al.
1990). Sudd (1960) also observed that in Pharaoh’s ant
it is commonplace for a single colony to forage on multi-
ple resources simultaneously, using several pheromone
trails.
A recognized benefit of trail marking in both di-
rections is that the shortest path to food can be selected
without recourse to visual information (Goss et al.
1989). In a binary choice trail set-up, where two differ-
ent length paths to the same food source, the shortest
route can be selected when ants mark in both direc-
tions, because for an initial period the shortest branch
is marked by ants moving in both directions whilst
the longest branch is only marked by ants walking in
one direction during that period. Thus the shortest
branch accumulates pheromone faster and is selected.
The greater the difference in the amount of marking
made by ants walking to the food and those returning
is, then the more random the overall choice between
two different length branches. This mechanism predicts
that L. niger will be poor at selecting the shortest of two
paths, because it only marks trails after food is found,
whereas in fact L. niger performs well. The solution to
this apparent paradox is that L. niger can use additional
information, such as visual cues, and can identify the
most direct route back to the nest (Beckers et al. 1993;
Camazine et al. 2001). Lasius niger foragers remember
the general direction to the nest and those returning
on the longer path can turn around, and then follow
the shorter route, when they see that the route they
are taking does not lead directly to the nest. However,
some individuals also prefer to return to the nest using
the same route they followed to the food (Beckers et al.
1992). It is clear that a foraging mechanism which is re-
liant on pheromones performs just as well as one with
recourse to visual memory.
Pheromone trails enable rapid mass recruitment to food
discoveries (Ho
¨lldobler & Wilson 1990) but they also
impose constraints on the overall foraging strategy of
a species. The characteristics of trail pheromones used,
particularly their decay rate, impose major constraints
on foraging flexibility. For example, if pheromone decay
is slow the ability to switch to better food sources is simi-
larly slow. Ant species vary in their reliance on pheromone
trails and visual cues for orientation (Aron et al. 1993). A
high reliance on pheromone trails requires that trails are
marked constitutively as an essential aid for forager navi-
gation between the food and nest. When trails are laid
constitutively there is a requirement for a more sophisti-
cated mechanism of redirecting foragers, one that does
not rely on trails decaying until they are undetectable.
When trails are important for orientation the modulation
of trail pheromone concentration enables foragers to se-
lect the most rewarding of two or more food sources. As
we have shown, this modulation can be achieved by two
different means, depending on whether the trail is essen-
tial for orientation. In Pharaoh’s ant modulation of trail
strength is by a change in the intensity of trail marking
by fed workers returning to the nest. Orientation trails
will be maintained but where they lead to food the trail
pheromone concentration will be increased because of
more intense marking by fed ants. Understanding the pa-
rameters constraining pheromone trail function provides
a better appreciation of the ecological limits of this forag-
ing strategy. In determining key parameters we can also
better understand the many subtle variations on the
general mechanism of self-organized information sharing
using pheromone trails.
JACKSON & CHA
ˆLINE: ANT PHEROMONE TRAIL MARKING 469
Acknowledgments
D.J. wishes to thank BT Exact and the EPSRC for funding
this research through the project ‘Simulation of Social
Insect Communities’. We also thank Francis Ratnieks for
providing facilities and Mike Holcombe for contributing
useful ideas.
References
Aron, S., Beckers, R., Deneubourg, J. L. & Pasteels, J. M. 1993.
Memory and mass communication in the orientation of two
mass-recruiting ant species. Insectes Sociaux,40, 369e380.
Beckers, R., Deneubourg, J. L., Goss, S. & Pasteels, J. M. 1990.
Collective decision-making through food recruitment. Insects
Sociaux,27, 258e267.
Beckers, R., Deneubourg, J. L. & Goss, S. 1992. Trails and U-turns
in the selection of a path by the ant Lasius niger.Journal of Theoret-
ical Biology,159, 397e415.
Beckers, R., Deneubourg, J. L. & Goss, S. 1993. Modulation of trail-
laying in the ant Lasius niger and its role in the collective selection
of a food source. Journal of Insect Behaviour,6, 751e759.
Billen, J. P. 1987. New structural aspects of the Dufour’s and venom
glands in social insects. Naturwissenschaften,74, 340e341.
Camazine, S., Deneubourg, J., Franks, N. R., Sneyd, J., Theraulaz,
G. & Bonabeau, E. 2001. Self-organisation in Biological Systems.
Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Detrain, C., Natan, C. & Deneubourg, J. L. 2001. The influence of
the physical environment on the self-organised foraging patterns
of ants. Naturwissenschaften,88, 171e174.
Fourcassie
´, V. & Deneubourg, J. L. 1994. The dynamics of collec-
tive exploration and trail-formation in Monomorium pharaonis:
experiments and model. Physiological Entomology,19, 291e300.
Goss,S.,Aron,J.L.,Deneubourg,J.L.&Pasteels,J.M.1989. Self-organ-
ised shortcuts in the Argentine ant. Naturwissenschaften,76, 579e581.
Hangartner, W. 1969. Structure and variability of the individual
odor trail in Solenopsis geminata.Zeitscrift fuer Vergleichende Phys-
iologie,62, 111e120.
Hart, A. & Jackson, D. E. 2006. U-turns on ant pheromone trails.
Current Biology,16,42e43.
Ho
¨lldobler, B. & Wilson, E. O. 1990. The Ants. Cambridge, Massa-
chusetts: Belknap Press of Harvard University.
Jackson, D. E., Holcombe, M. & Ratnieks, F. L. W. 2004. Trail
geometry gives polarity to ant foraging networks. Nature,432,
907e909.
Jackson, D. E., Martin, S. J., Holcombe, M. & Ratnieks, F. L. W.
2006. Longevity and detection of persistent foraging trails in Phar-
aoh’s ants, Monomorium pharaonis (L.). Animal Behaviour,71,
351e359.
Jeanson, R., Ratnieks, F. L. W. & Deneubourg, J. L. 2003. Pher-
omone trail decay rates on different substrates in the Pharaoh’s
ant, Monomorium pharaonis.Physiological Entomology,28,
192e198.
Mailleux, A. C., Deneubourg, J. M. & Detrain, C. 2000. How do
ants assess food volume? Animal Behaviour,59, 1061e1069.
Mailleux, A. C., Deneubourg, J. M. & Detrain, C. 2003. Regula-
tion of ants’ foraging to resource productivity. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London, Series B,270, 1609e1616.
Mailleux, A. C., Detrain, C. & Deneubourg, J. M. 2005. Triggering
and persistence of trail-laying in foragers of the ant Lasius niger.
Journal of Insect Physiology,51, 297e304.
Ritter, F. J., Bruggemann, I. E. M., Persoons, C. J., Talman, E.,
van Oosten, A. M. & Verwiel, P. E. J. 1977a. Evaluation of
social insect pheromones in pest control, with special reference
to subterranean termites and Pharaoh’s ants. In: Crop Protection
Agents eTheir Biological Evaluation (Ed. by N. R. McFarlane),
pp. 201e222. London: Academic Press.
Ritter, F. J., Bruggemann, I. E. M., Verwiel, P. E. J., Persoons,
C. J. & Talman, E. 1977b. Trail pheromone of the Pharaoh’s
ant, Monomorium pharaonis: isolation and identification of faranal,
a terpenoid related to juvenile hormone II. Tetrahedron Letters,30,
2617e2618.
Sudd, J. H. 1960. The foraging method of the Pharaoh’s ant. Animal
Behaviour,8,67e75.
Sumpter, D. J. T. & Beekman, M. 2003. From nonlinearity to opti-
mality: pheromone trail foraging by ants. Animal Behaviour,66,
273e280.
Witte, V. & Maschwitz, U. 2002. Coordination of raiding and
emigration in the ponerine army ant Leptogenys distin-
guenda: a signal analysis. Journal of Insect Behaviour,15,
195e217.
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 74,3
470
... Likewise, ants expecting a very mild reward are more likely to accept a moderate reward than ants which were expecting that quality -a positive contrast (Wendt et al., 2019). These changes are also mirrored in the ants' deposition of recruitment pheromone: ants deposit more pheromone to resources they perceive as higher quality (Beckers et al., 1993;Jackson and Châline, 2007), and indeed ants deposit more pheromone for moderate rewards if they had been expected poor quality, and less pheromone if they were expecting high quality. This demonstrates that a key aspect of Prospect Theory -relative value perception -is present in insects as well as humans. ...
... However, the pattern of pheromone deposition, which correlates strongly with perceived food quality in these and other ants (Beckers et al., 1993;Czaczkes et al., 2018b;Jackson and Châline, 2007;Wendt et al., 2019), was fully in line with our predictions: ants deposited most pheromone when the costs were bundled and rewards segregated, and a lot less in the other two treatments (Figure 3). Our weaker prediction of 'Bundled' being "liked" more than 'Segregated All' was also supported. ...
... When travelling away from a food source, the animal must by definition be divorced from "wanting", as it is heading away from the reward. As pheromone deposition correlates very strongly with perceived reward value (Beckers et al., 1993;Breed et al., 1987;Crawford and Rissing, 1983;Jackson and Châline, 2007;Wendt et al., 2019), we consider it to be a good metric of "liking" which is, at least on the return to the nest, disassociated from "wanting". While pheromone deposition ("liking") very often co-vary with choice behaviour ("wanting") (Czaczkes et al., 2018a;Wendt et al., , 2019, the results of the current experiment empirically demonstrates that they can be dissociated. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Behavioural economists have identified many psychological manipulations which affect perceived value, although value in humans is not a unitary experience, with “liking” and “wanting” being neurologically separate processes. A prominent example of this is bundling, in which several small gains (or costs) are experienced as more valuable (or costly) than if the same total amount is presented together. While extensively demonstrated in humans, to our knowledge this effect has never been investigated in an animal, let alone an invertebrate. We trained individual Lasius niger workers to two of three conditions in which either costs (travel distance), gains (sucrose reward), or both were either bundled or segregated: A) both costs and gains bundled, B) both segregated, and C) only gains segregated. We recorded pheromone deposition on the ants’ return trips to the nest as measure of “liking”. After training, we offer the ants a binary choice between odours associated with the treatments, as a measure of “wanting”. While bundling treatment did not affect choice, i.e. “wanting”, it strongly influenced pheromone deposition, i.e. “liking”. Ants deposited c. 80% more pheromone when rewards were segregated but costs bundled as compared with both costs and rewards being bundled. This pattern is further complicated by the pairwise experience each animal made, and which of the treatments it experiences first during training. The current study is the first to demonstrate a bundling effect in an animal, and the first to report a dichotomy between “liking” and “wanting” in an insect. We propose that the deviation between “wanting” and “liking” in this case is due to the unique nature of distance perception in ants, which is recorded linearly, while almost all other sensory perception in animals is logarithmic.
... Likewise, ants expecting a very mild reward are more likely to accept a moderate reward than ants which were expecting that quality -a positive contrast (Wendt et al., 2019). These changes are also mirrored in the ants' deposition of recruitment pheromone: ants deposit more pheromone to resources they perceive as higher quality (Beckers et al., 1993;Jackson and Châline, 2007), and indeed ants deposit more pheromone for moderate rewards if they had been expected poor quality, and less pheromone if they were expecting high quality. This demonstrates that a key aspect of prospect theory -relative value perception -is present in insects as well as in humans. ...
... However, the pattern of pheromone deposition, which correlates strongly with perceived food quality in these and other ants (Beckers et al., 1993;Czaczkes et al., 2018b;Jackson and Châline, 2007;Wendt et al., 2019), was partially in line with our predictions: ants deposited most pheromone when the costs were bundled and rewards segregated, and less in the other two treatments (Figure 4). Our weaker prediction of 'bundled' presenting a higher deposition than 'segregated all' was also supported. ...
Article
Full-text available
Behavioural economists have identified many psychological manipulations which affect perceived value. A prominent example of this is bundling, in which several small gains (or costs) are experienced as more valuable (or costly) than if the same total amount is presented together. While extensively demonstrated in humans, to our knowledge this effect has never been investigated in an animal, let alone an invertebrate. We trained individual Lasius niger workers to two of three conditions in which either costs (travel distance), gains (sucrose reward), or both were either bundled or segregated: (1) both costs and gains bundled, (2) both segregated, and (3) only gains segregated. We recorded pheromone deposition on the ants' return trips to the nest as measure of perceived value. After training, we offer the ants a binary choice between odours associated with the treatments. While bundling treatment did not affect binary choice, it strongly influenced phero-mone deposition. Ants deposited c. 80% more pheromone when rewards were segregated but costs bundled as compared with both costs and rewards being bundled. This pattern is further complicated by the pairwise experience each animal made, and which of the treatments it experiences first during training. This demonstrates that even insects are influenced by bundling effects. We propose that the deviation between binary choice and pheromone deposition in this case may be due to a possible linearity in distance perception in ants, while almost all other sensory perception in animals is logarithmic. Editor's evaluation This innovative study investigates the presence of biases in value perception in ants. The authors were able to show that the distribution of rewards and costs influences perceived reward value in ants, an effect that was observed for pheromone deposition but not choice behaviour.
... Preferential consumption of certain sugar solutions (figures 4-8) is the result of behavioural choices made by foraging ants that are dependent upon characteristics of the specific sugar solution (sugar type and concentration). In response to the sugar solution they encountered, individual ants decide how much to carry back in their crop to the nest [95][96][97], how many return trips to the resource to make [6], and how much (if any) trail pheromone to deposit [35,[98][99][100]. More ants are recruited by strongly marked trails, ultimately leading to collective choices by ants for the most appealing resource [7]. ...
... The sugar concentration of resources does affect crop load of ants, but it is not necessarily the highest sugar concentration that elicits uptake of the largest crop load, as shown with the carpenter ant, Camponotus mus, the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile, and the ponerine ant, Odontomachus chelifer [95][96][97]. Ants mark trails more intensely in response to more concentrated sugar solutions [98,99]. More Ca. modoc nest-mates may have been recruited to high-dose fructose solutions, if foraging antson their return trip to the nest-deposited trail pheromone, and if recruited nest-mates reinforced the trail with their own pheromone deposits. ...
Article
Full-text available
Ants select sustained carbohydrate resources, such as aphid honeydew, based on many factors including sugar type, volume and concentration. We tested the hypotheses (H1–H3) that western carpenter ants, Camponotus modoc, seek honeydew excretions from Cinara splendens aphids based solely on the presence of sugar constituents (H1), prefer sugar solutions containing aphid-specific sugars (H2) and preferentially seek sugar solutions with higher sugar content (H3). We further tested the hypothesis (H4) that workers of both Ca. modoc and European fire ants, Myrmica rubra , selectively consume particular mono-, di- and trisaccharides. In choice bioassays with entire ant colonies, sugar constituents in honeydew (but not aphid-specific sugar) as well as sugar concentration affected foraging decisions by Ca. modoc . Both Ca. modoc and M. rubra foragers preferred fructose to other monosaccharides (xylose, glucose) and sucrose to other disaccharides (maltose, melibiose, trehalose). Conversely, when offered a choice between the aphid-specific trisaccharides raffinose and melezitose, Ca. modoc and M. rubra favoured raffinose and melezitose, respectively. Testing the favourite mono-, di- and trisaccharide head-to-head, both ant species favoured sucrose. While both sugar type and sugar concentration are the ultimate cause for consumption by foraging ants, strong recruitment of nest-mates to superior sources is probably the major proximate cause.
... De acordo com Beckes e colaboradores(1989), as pesquisas sobre comportamento desses indivíduos são extremante relevantes, pois possibilitam observação das estratégias de seus comportamentos; umas das estratégias é o forrageamento que, quanto maior o tamanho da colônia, maior a probabilidade de usar pistas de feromônios para forragear. Forrageamento é então uma área de pesquisa muito desenvolvida, e consiste a maioria das vezes em registrar o comportamento de indivíduos ou de colônias ao explorar fonte de alimentos(Jackson & Châline, 2007).Dessarte, objetivo geral desse trabalho foi analisar o comportamento alimentar das formigas mediante ...
Article
Full-text available
As formigas lideram o espaço de seres considerados dotados de uma extraordinária capacidade de adaptação a qualquer ambiente, com estratégias de sobrevivência baseadas numa divisão de trabalho entre a colônia, que atrai atenção para diversas pesquisas e descobertas científicas. Sendo assim, o trabalho teve como objetivo realizar a observação do comportamento alimentar de formigas em ambientes domésticos nas cidades de Arapiraca e Major Isidoro, em Alagoas, mediante a oferta de atrativos: mel, açúcar e formitol (inseticida) como iscas. Foram elaboradas armadilhas em forma de “fitas” com diferentes tamanhos (10cm, 12cm e 20 cm) em superfícies lisas e em turnos distintos para atraí-las. Ao observar o comportamento das formigas, foram identificadas três espécies diferentes de formigas nos locais da pesquisa: Formiga fantasma (Tapinoma melanocephalu); Formiga louca (Paratrechina longicornis) e Formiga argentina (Linepithema humile). Esta última apareceu em maior quantidade (64,4%) dos 414 indivíduos identificados ao total. As formigas apresentaram maior índice de permanência e quantidade de indivíduos no turno da noite, o índice de tempo e quantidade foram moderados no turno da tarde, e menor índice de permanência e quantidade foi no turno da manhã. Concluiu-se que a finalidade do estudo foi alcançada, de acordo com as observações feitas através das variáveis (o ambiente, a identificação das espécies, a preferência alimentar das formigas e a estimativa do tempo de permanência em torno do alimento).
... Other concurrent orientation methods can be used by some species (Wolf and Wehner 2000;Banks and Srygley 2003;Harris et al. 2007;Müller and Wehner 2010), but pheromone substrate marking is usually the key mechanism to recruit workers (David Morgan 2009). As a rule, the signal can be modulated according to the quality of the resource: the better the resource the stronger and more persistent the signal (Jackson and Châline 2007). The presence of a pheromone trail induces the recruited ants to follow it and mark the route themselves, thus reinforcing the signal. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite several restrictions to their use, neonicotinoid insecticides are still widely employed worldwide. Residual sub-lethal amounts of these chemicals can have detrimental effects on the behavior of non-target insects. Toxic effects on economically important species such as bees have been widely documented, but less is known about their toxic action on other social insects, such as ants. In this study, we assessed the effect of different sub-lethal doses of the neonicotinoid imidacloprid on the ability of colonies of the invasive ant Lasius neglectus to select the most profitable resource. We used Y-shaped mazes having an imidacloprid-polluted or an unpolluted sucrose solution on the two branches. Two sucrose (0.1 M, 0.5 M) and two imidacloprid (1 μg/ml, 10 μg/ml) concentrations were used. In parallel, we evaluated the marking activity of foragers who fed on the same solutions. We found that the 0.1 M sugar solution polluted with 1 μg/ml imidacloprid was significantly more frequently selected in binary choices experiments than the unpolluted resource. Moreover, the ingestion of the same combination of sugar and imidacloprid significantly increased the marking rate of foragers. The higher concentration of the pollutant had lower effects, probably because of the hormesis phenomenon. Results suggest that the lower sub-lethal dose of imidacloprid can lead ants to select again the polluted resource. This “active” selection of the pollutant may magnify the negative effects on the colonies. Due to their ecological role, any impairment of ant survival or behavior may have detrimental cascade effects on the whole ecosystem.
... The level of exploitation of a newly discovered food source will then depend on the decision of successful foragers to actively recruit nestmates by direct contacts and/or by the laying a pheromone trail on their way back to the nest 2,3 . The release of a recruitment signal by foragers, which will trigger a collective exploitation of food, depends on numerous factors such as the carbohydrate concentration of a food droplet [4][5][6][7] , the quantity of food [8][9][10] , its nature (e.g. prey or sugar solution) [11][12][13] , its distance from the nest 14,15 but also the colony's current nutritional status 13,[16][17][18] . ...
Article
Full-text available
Eusocial insects are exposed to a wide range of pathogens while foraging outside their nest. We know that opportunistic scavenging ants are able to assess the sanitary state of food and to discriminate a prey which died from infection by the entomopathogenic fungus Metarhizium brunneum. Here, we investigate whether a contamination of the environment can also influence the behaviour of foragers, both at the individual and collective level. In a Y-maze, Myrmica rubra ants had the choice to forage on two prey patches, one of which containing sporulating items. Unexpectedly, the nearby presence of sporulating bodies did not deter foragers nor prevent them from retrieving palatable prey. Ant colonies exploited both prey patches equally, without further mortality resulting from foraging on the contaminated area. Thus, a contamination of the environment did not prompt an active avoidance by foragers of which the activity depended primarily on the food characteristics. Generalist entomopathogenic fungi such as M. brunneum in the area around the nest appear more to be of a nuisance to ant foragers than a major selective force driving them to adopt avoidance strategies. We discuss the cost–benefit balance derived from the fine-tuning of strategies of pathogen avoidance in ants.
... food source profitability (honeybees: von Frisch 1967; ants: Czaczkes et al. 2015;Beckers et al. 1993;Jackson and Chaline 2007;stingless bees: Nieh et al. 2003;Jarau and Hrncir 2009). This means that recruited individuals using this filtered information, will have a higher chance of finding a better resource. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social information is widely used in the animal kingdom and can be highly adaptive. In social insects, foragers can use social information to find food, avoid danger or choose a new nest site. Copying others allows individuals to obtain information without having to sample the environment. When foragers communicate information they will often only advertise high quality food sources, thereby filtering out less adaptive information. Stingless bees, a large pantropical group of highly eusocial bees, face intense inter- and intra-specific competition for limited resources, yet display disparate foraging strategies. Within the same environment there are species that communicate the location of food resources to nest-mates and species that do not. Our current understanding of why some species communicate foraging sites while others do not is limited. Studying freely foraging colonies of several co-existing stingless bee species in Brazil, we investigated if recruitment to specific food locations is linked to (1) the sugar content of forage, (2) the duration of foraging trips and (3) the variation in activity of a colony from one day to another and the variation in activity in a species over a day. We found that, contrary to our expectations, species with recruitment communication did not return with higher quality forage than species that do not recruit nestmates. Furthermore, foragers from recruiting species did not have shorter foraging trip durations than those from weakly-recruiting species. Given the intense inter- and intraspecific competition for resources in these environments, it may be that recruiting species favour food resources that can be monopolised by the colony rather than food sources that offer high-quality rewards.
... In the case of regulating the pheromone, the personal pheromone for each ant agent is applied additionally on the path including the best solution that each ant agent has found out in the past tours, apart from the pheromone as social information. In the natural world, it is known that when real ants discover a shorter path from the nest to the feeding site, they increase the amount of pheromone they apply along that route [13,14]. On the one hand, in ASIM, when the best solution cannot be updated, each ant agent can regulate the personal pheromones on the path in the best solution that has been found so far. ...
Article
Full-text available
Ant colony optimization (ACO), which is one of the metaheuristics imitating real ant foraging behavior, is an effective method to find a solution for the traveling salesman problem (TSP). The rank-based ant system (ASrank) has been proposed as a developed version of the fundamental model AS of ACO. In the ASrank, since only ant agents that have found one of some excellent solutions are let to regulate the pheromone, the pheromone concentrates on a specific route. As a result, although the ASrank can find a relatively good solution in a short time, it has the disadvantage of being prone falling into a local solution because the pheromone concentrates on a specific route. This problem seems to come from the loss of diversity in route selection according to the rapid accumulation of pheromones to the specific routes. Some ACO models, not just the ASrank, also suffer from this problem of loss of diversity in route selection. It can be considered that the diversity of solutions as well as the selection of solutions is an important factor in the solution system by swarm intelligence such as ACO. In this paper, to solve this problem, we introduce the ant system using individual memories (ASIM) aiming to improve the ability to solve TSP while maintaining the diversity of the behavior of each ant. We apply the existing ACO algorithms and ASIM to some TSP benchmarks and compare the ability to solve TSP.
Article
Full-text available
RESUMO: As formigas lideram o espaço de seres considerados dotados de uma extraordinária capacidade de adaptação a qualquer ambiente, com estratégias de sobrevivência baseadas numa divisão de trabalho entre a colônia, que atrai atenção para diversas pesquisas e descobertas científicas. Sendo assim, o trabalho teve como objetivo realizar a observação do comportamento alimentar de formigas em ambientes domésticos nas cidades de Arapiraca e Major Isidoro, em Alagoas, mediante a oferta de atrativos: mel, açúcar e formitol (inseticida) como iscas. Foram elaboradas armadilhas em forma de “fitas” com diferentes tamanhos (10cm, 12cm e 20 cm) em superfícies lisas e em turnos distintos para atraí-las. Ao observar o comportamento das formigas, foram identificadas três espécies diferentes de formigas nos locais da pesquisa: Formiga fantasma (Tapinoma melanocephalu); Formiga louca (Paratrechina longicornis) e Formiga argentina (Linepithema humile). Esta última apareceu em maior quantidade (64,4%) dos 414 indivíduos identificados ao total. As formigas apresentaram maior índice de permanência e quantidade de indivíduos no turno da noite, o índice de tempo e quantidade foram moderados no turno da tarde, e menor índice de permanência e quantidade foi no turno da manhã. Concluiu-se que a finalidade do estudo foi alcançada, de acordo com as observações feitas através das variáveis (o ambiente, a identificação das espécies, a preferência alimentar das formigas e a estimativa do tempo de permanência em torno do alimento).
Preprint
Full-text available
Executive control refers to the regulation of cognition and behavior by mental processes and is a hallmark of higher cognition. Most approaches to understanding its mechanisms begin with the assumption that our brains have anatomically segregated and functionally specialized control modules. The modular approach is intuitive: control is conceptually distinct from basic mental processing, so an organization that reifies that distinction makes sense. An alternative approach sees executive control as self-organizing principles of a distributed organization. In distributed systems, control and controlled processes are co-localized within large numbers of dispersed computational agents. Control then is often an emergent consequence of simple rules governing the interaction between agents. Because these systems are unfamiliar and unintuitive, here we review several well-understood examples of distributed control systems, group living insects and social animals, and emphasize their parallels with neural systems. We then re-examine the cognitive neuroscience literature on executive control for evidence that its neural control systems may be distributed.
Article
1. 1. Foraging workers of Monomorium pharaonis orientated to scent marks on the ground. The scent marks were laid by other workers when they were returning to the nest after finding food. Such successful foragers orientated to the nest partly by scent marks and partly by another orientation, probably a visual one. 2. 2. When a successful forager arrived in the nest it activated a small group of workers. These recruits followed a trail of scent laid by the scout, fed and returned to the nest by the same trail, reinforcing it with new scent. In the nest they activated more recruits. 3. 3. Recruits were not informed by the scout of the nature of the food to which they were recruited, nor of its distance from the nest. There was ao evidence that recruits became habituated to a particular kind of bait after feeding on it for 1–2 hours. 4. 4. When recruits were running on a system with two branch trails, the branch which they took appeared to be decided by chance. The abandonment of a trail which no longer led to food was probably due to the fading of the scent in the absence of successful foragers to reinforce it. 5. 5. The vestiges of the foraging trails formed a system of 1–4 trunk routes leading from the nest. One of these routes formed the proximal part of every scout's return path; hence this part of the route was continually reinforced with new scent and was constant in position. All; scouts left the nest along a route so that the distribution of scouts was related to the distribution of finds of food in the past.
Article
L.niger choose the shorter of two aths. U turns are the main mechanism of selection. proportion of U turns depends on length, geometry and trail strength. given equal choice ants often (18/25) concentrate on one branch. High trail traffic reduces the amount of 'intrinsic (random) U turns. Trail laying is greater when returning to the nest than when leaving it. L niger does not lay trail until it has fed at source. orientation apears to modulate trail laying. Foragers are not affected by their previous choice of trail (no memory). U turns have a strong effect - stronger than the bi-directional laying effect (where shorter route means individuals get round to makring the trail more, as they mark both coming and going, and the turnover is faster). (goss et al 1989 show this earlier in diff species) Laying ants lay more pheromone on trails on an angle closer to the nest-food axis,
Article
  Many ants use pheromone trails to organize collective foraging. This study investigated the rate at which a well-established Pharaoh's ant, Monomorium pharaonis (L.), trail breaks down on two substrates (polycarbonate plastic, newspaper). Workers were allowed to feed on sucrose solution from a feeder 30 cm from the nest. Between the nest and the feeder, the trail had a Y-shaped bifurcation. Initially, while recruiting to and exploiting the feeder, workers could only deposit pheromone on the branch leading to the feeder. Once the trail was established (by approximately 60 ants per min for 20 min), the ants were not allowed to reinforce the trail and were given a choice between the marked and unmarked branches. The numbers of ants choosing each branch were counted for 30 min. Initially, most went to the side on which pheromone had been deposited (80% and 70% on the plastic and paper substrates, respectively). However, this decayed to 50% within 25 min for plastic and 8 min for paper. From these data, the half-life times of the pheromone are estimated as approximately 9 min and 3 min on plastic and paper, respectively. The results show that, for M. pharaonis, trail decay is rapid and is affected strongly by trail substrate.
Article
When exploring a chemically unmarked area devoid of food sources, workers of the pest ant Monomorium pharaonis L. (Formicidae, Myrmicinae) leave scent marks on the ground and after 30–60min a network of diverging exploratory trails begins to emerge. Exploratory activity is affected by the nutritional state of the colony and a period of food deprivation induces a dramatic increase in the number of workers leaving the nest. A mathematical model based on a logistic growth equation is proposed to describe the exploratory recruitment observed. When travelling along exploratory trails the proportion of ants displaying trail-laying behaviour is higher for outbound than for nestbound workers. Outbound ants also show a greater propensity than nestbound ants to follow the scent marks of their nestmates. The chemical used to mark a novel area does not appear to be colony-specific and thus does not have a territorial function sensu stricto. The adaptive value of the collective exploratory behaviour observed in this study is discussed in relation to the common features of other pest ant species described in the literature.