ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Of the offensive yet non-pathological personalities in the literature, three are especially prominent: Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy. We evaluated the recent contention that, in normal samples, this ‘Dark Triad’ of constructs are one and the same. In a sample of 245 students, we measured the three constructs with standard measures and examined a variety of laboratory and self-report correlates. The measures were moderately inter-correlated, but certainly were not equivalent. Their only common Big Five correlate was disagreeableness. Subclinical psychopaths were distinguished by low neuroticism; Machiavellians, and psychopaths were low in conscientiousness; narcissism showed small positive associations with cognitive ability. Narcissists and, to a lesser extent, psychopaths exhibited self-enhancement on two objectively scored indexes. We conclude that the Dark Triad of personalities, as currently measured, are overlapping but distinct constructs.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Brief report
The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism,
Machiavellianism, and psychopathy
Delroy L. Paulhus
*
and Kevin M. Williams
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver V6T 1Z4, Canada
Abstract
Of the offensive yet non-pathological personalities in the literature, three are es-
pecially prominent: Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psy-
chopathy. We evaluated the recent contention that, in normal samples, this ÔDark
TriadÕof constructs are one and the same. In a sample of 245 students, we measured
the three constructs with standard measures and examined a variety of laboratory
and self-report correlates. The measures were moderately inter-correlated, but cer-
tainly were not equivalent. Their only common Big Five correlate was disagree-
ableness. Subclinical psychopaths were distinguished by low neuroticism;
Machiavellians, and psychopaths were low in conscientiousness; narcissism showed
small positive associations with cognitive ability. Narcissists and, to a lesser extent,
psychopaths exhibited self-enhancement on two objectively scored indexes. We con-
clude that the Dark Triad of personalities, as currently measured, are overlapping
but distinct constructs.
Ó2002 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Among the socially aversive personalities cited in Kowalski (2001), three
have attracted the most empirical attention: Machiavellianism, narcissism,
and psychopathy. The construct of Machiavellianism—in short, the manip-
ulative personality—emerged from Richard ChristieÕs selection of state-
ments from MachiavelliÕs original books (see Christie & Geis, 1970).
Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563
www.academicpress.com
JOURNAL OF
RESEARCH IN
PERSONALITY
*
Corresponding author.
0092-6566/02/$ - see front matter Ó2002 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 9 2 - 6 5 6 6 ( 0 2 ) 00 5 0 5 - 6
Christie fashioned those statements into a measure of normal personality by
demonstrating reliable differences in respondentsÕagreement with the items.
Further research showed that respondents who agreed with these statements
were more likely to behave in a cold and manipulative fashion in laboratory
and real world studies (Christie & Geis, 1970).
The construct of subclinical or ÔnormalÕnarcissism emerged from Raskin
and HallÕs (1979) attempt to delineate a subclinical version of the DSM-de-
fined personality disorder. Facets retained from the clinical syndrome in-
cluded grandiosity, entitlement, dominance, and superiority. Items were
refined on large samples of students and assembled in the Narcissistic Per-
sonality Inventory (NPI). The successful migration from clinical to subclin-
ical construct is well supported by a strong research literature (Morf &
Rhodewalt, 2001).
The adaptation of psychopathy to the subclinical sphere is the most re-
cent of the three (Hare, 1985; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996). Central charac-
ter elements include high impulsivity and thrill-seeking along with low
empathy and anxiety. The self-report psychopathy (SRP) scale was assem-
bled from items that differentiated clinically diagnosed psychopaths from
non-psychopaths (Hare, 1985). It was later validated in non-criminal sam-
ples (Forth, Brown, Hart, & Hare, 1996). Recent research by Williams
and Paulhus (2002) confirmed that the SRP has the same four-factor solu-
tion as the Psychopathy Check List (Hare, 1991), which is the gold standard
in the measurement of psychopathy. Moreover, SRP scores predict anti-so-
cial behavior in forensic and non-forensic populations (Paulhus, Hemphill,
& Hare, in press).
Despite their diverse origins, the personalities composing this ÔDark
TriadÕshare a number of features. To varying degrees, all three entail a
socially malevolent character with behavior tendencies toward self-promo-
tion, emotional coldness, duplicity, and aggressiveness. In the clinical liter-
ature, the links among the triad have been noted for some time (e.g., Hart
& Hare, 1998). The recent development of non-clinical measures of all
three constructs has permitted the evaluation of empirical associations in
normal populations. As a result, there is now empirical evidence for the
overlap of (a) Machiavellianism with psychopathy (Fehr, Samsom, &
Paulhus, 1992; McHoskey, Worzel, & Szyarto, 1998), (b) narcissism with
psychopathy (Gustafson & Ritzer, 1995), and (c) Machiavellianism with
narcissism (McHoskey, 1995). Given such associations, the possibility
arises that, in normal samples, the Dark Triad of constructs may be equiv-
alent.
In the present study, we exploited three methods for teasing apart the
triad of constructs. First, we mapped the triad onto the Big Five domains
to examine similarities and differences on fundamental dimensions of per-
sonality. Second, we compared the triad with respect to two measures
of cognitive ability. Finally, we determined whether the substantial self-
Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563 557
enhancement observed in narcissists (John & Robins, 1994; Paulhus, 1998;
Raskin, Novacek, & Hogan, 1991) would also be evident in Machiavellian
and subclinically psychopathic individuals. Two objective measures of
self-enhancement were developed: One was an index of the tendency to
over-claim general knowledge items; the other was a measure of discrepancy
between self-reported intelligence and objectively scored intelligence.
2. Method
Two hundred and forty-five undergraduate psychology students (65%
female) participated in the present study for extra course credit. The pro-
cedure involved two steps. First, each participant took home an anony-
mous questionnaire package, which included standard self-report
measures of the Dark Triad and the Big Five as well as self-ratings of in-
telligence. Participants later returned to the lab for a supervised adminis-
tration of the over claiming measure and an objective measure of global
cognitive ability (IQ).
The NPI (Raskin & Hall, 1979) was used to measure narcissism. The NPI
is a 40 item forced-choice questionnaire, currently the standard measure of
subclinical narcissism. The Mach-IV inventory (Christie & Geis, 1970) was
used to measure Machiavellianism: It consists of 20 5-point Likert items. To
measure subclinical psychopathy, we used the SRP III (Hare, 1985) consist-
ing of 31 5-point items. In our sample, the alpha reliabilities for the NPI,
SRP, and Mach-IV scale were .84, .79, and .74, respectively.
The Big Five inventory (BFI) is a 44 item questionnaire designed to mea-
sure the Big Five personality traits (extraversion, agreeableness, conscien-
tiousness, neuroticism, and openness). Its validity is well-established (John
& Srivastava, 1999). The alpha reliabilities in our sample were .87, .81,
.81, .86, and .80, respectively.
To measure self-perceptions of intelligence, we summed four 5-point
scales anchored by Not at all (1) to Very much (5). The items were intelligent,
smart, good at school, and known as brainy. In our sample, the alpha reliabil-
ity was .83.
The 50-item speeded Wonderlic Personnel Test (Wonderlic, 1977) was
used to measure global cognitive ability, including both verbal and nonver-
bal IQ. The Over Claiming Questionnaire (OCQ) was designed as an unob-
trusive measure of both cognitive ability and self-enhancement bias
(Paulhus, Harms, Bruce, & Lysy, in press). The task requires rating the fa-
miliarity of 90 persons, events, and things, 20% of which do not exist. Signal
detection formulas were then calculated to index accuracy of general knowl-
edge (cognitive ability) and response bias (knowledge self-enhancement). In
our sample, the alpha reliabilities for accuracy and bias indexes were .84 and
.93, respectively.
558 Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563
3. Analysis and results
Males scored significantly higher on all three of the Dark Triad: Biserial
correlations were .11, .21, and .40, for NPI, Mach IV, and SRP, respectively
(all significant, p<:05, two-tailed). Within gender, however, the correla-
tional patterns with external variables were remarkably similar: Therefore,
we pooled the data across gender. The measures overlapped considerably,
as evident from Fig. 1. Nonetheless, the maximum inter-correlation of .50
suggests that they cannot be considered equivalent. Even disattenuated,
the highest correlation—that between psychopathy and narcissism—reaches
only .61.
Big Five traits. In Table 1, several correlations between Big Five scores
and the Dark Triad measures reached significance using two-tailed tests,
p<:01. Agreeableness showed correlations of ).36, ).47, and ).25, for nar-
cissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy, respectively. Narcissists and
psychopaths also tended to have higher Extraversion (.41 and. 34) and
Openness scores (.38 and .24). Machiavellians and psychopaths showed
low scores on Conscientiousness ().34 and ).24). Finally, psychopaths
tended to report lower levels of Neuroticism ().34).
Cognitive ability. Table 1 shows small positive correlations of both cogni-
tive ability measures with narcissism but no links with Machiavellianism or
psychopathy. After separating the IQ items into verbal and nonverbal sub-
scales, a discrepancy score was calculated by subtracting the standardized
verbal from the standardized nonverbal subscale. Table 1 shows significant
positive correlations of the difference score with Machiavellianism (.20) and
psychopathy (.13), indicating a higher nonverbal IQ score relative to verbal.
Self-enhancement bias. Over-claiming bias was operationalized as the sig-
nal detection parameter (ÔcÕ) that indexes any claim of familiarity with an
Fig. 1. Correlations among measures of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy.
Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563 559
OCQ item, regardless of its true existence. Over-claiming correlated signifi-
cantly with narcissism (.17), but not with Machiavellianism or psychopathy
(see Table 1). The OCQ accuracy index (dÕ) was unrelated to any of the Dark
Triad variables.
A second measure of self-enhancement was calculated by partialing IQ
scores out of self-rated intelligence using regression analysis (see Paulhus
& John, 1998). This residual represents the discrepancy between self-ratings
and objective performance. As Table 1 shows, narcissists and, to a lesser ex-
tent, psychopaths tended to overestimate their intelligence (rs¼.24 and .14),
whereas Machiavellians did not.
4. Discussion
Our goal was to evaluate the similarities and differences among the Dark
Triad of personality traits: narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy.
Our data do not support the contention that, when measured in normal
populations, these three constructs are equivalent. The intercorrelations
were all positive and ranged from .25 to .50. Their distinctiveness became
most apparent in our examination of the external correlates, including both
self-report and performance measures. Their locations in the five factor
space of personality revealed only one commonality across the triad,
namely, low agreeableness. Both narcissism and psychopathy were also as-
sociated with extraversion and openness: Thus they fall in the circumplex
quadrant labeled ‘‘unmitigated agency’’ (Helgeson & Fritz, 1999; Paulhus
& John, 1998). Also consistent, Machiavellianism and psychopathy were
Table 1
Correlations of the Dark Triad with the Big Five, cognitive ability, and self-enhancement
Narcissism Machiavellianism Psychopathy
Big Five Inventory
Extraversion .42 ).05 .34
Agreeableness ).36 ).47 ).25
Conscientiousness ).06 ).34 ).24
Neuroticism .02 .12 ).34
Openness .38 ).03 .24
Cognitive ability
OCQ accuracy index .09 .04 .09
IQ test .15 .04 .05
Verbal–nonverbal discrepancy .05 .20 .13
Self-enhancement
Discrepancy of self-rating vs.
IQ test
.24 ).02 .14
Over-claiming bias index .17 .08 .09
Note. N ¼245. All correlations in bold are significant at p<:05, two-tailed.
560 Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563
negatively associated with conscientiousness, a communal trait. Finally,
only psychopaths were low on neuroticism, consistent with their traditional
characterization as lacking anxiety (Hare, 1991).
On two objective measures, narcissists exhibited the most self-enhance-
ment, followed by the psychopaths. The narcissism finding replicates previ-
ous research (John & Robins, 1994; Paulhus, 1998) but the establishment of
self-enhancement in subclinical psychopaths is novel. In contrast, machia-
vellians showed no sign of self-enhancement. This difference is consistent
with previous evidence that Machiavellians are more grounded, or reality-
based, in their sense of self (Christie & Geis, 1970), whereas narcissists have
a strong self-deceptive (i.e., low insight) component to their personality (Ra-
skin et al., 1991; Paulhus, 1998). Such grandiosity and poor insight have also
been noted in clinical-range psychopathy (Hart & Hare, 1998).
The only association with cognitive ability was the small positive correla-
tion between IQ and narcissism. However, we did find significant associa-
tions of psychopathy and Machiavellianism with a relatively higher
nonverbal to verbal IQ score. A breakdown on ethnicity and gender indi-
cated the strongest correlate of this difference score (r¼.27) was psychopa-
thy in males of European heritage (i.e., white people with dark
personalities). This finding is consistent with previous work showing that
a parallel performance-verbal difference score is higher in delinquent than
in non-delinquent adolescents (e.g., Lynam, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber,
1993) and higher in psychopathic than non-psychopathic delinquents (Gret-
ton, 1998). These samples, too, were primarily white males.
The tendency for dark personalities to exhibit relatively higher levels of
nonverbal IQ is intriguing but the implications are unclear. The finding de-
fies the stereotype of the smooth talking manipulator but supports the no-
tion of a complex intellectual deficit. One possibility is that the frustration
arising from an inability to communicate oneÕs ideas eventuates in more ma-
levolent interpersonal strategies. Another possibility is some subtle neuro-
logical deficit.
As for fundamental personality features, our findings suggest that, in
non-clinical samples, members of the Dark Triad share a common core of
disagreeableness. Thus the root of their social destructiveness is disturbingly
normal - even banal. In combination with disagreeableness, the minimal
anxiety of psychopaths may make them the most treacherous of the three
- even within the normal range of personality found in our sample. Our
more recent work has supported this fear. A wide variety of self-report
and behavioral measures of antisocial behavior were significantly predicted
by psychopathy but not by Machiavellianism or narcissism (Paulhus & Wil-
liams, 2002; Williams & Paulhus, 2002).
Which of the triad is most maladaptive? Our view is that no personality
trait is universally adaptive or maladaptive (Paulhus, Fridhandler, & Hayes,
1997). Indeed, Machiavellians and narcissists may be more of an interper-
Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563 561
sonal irritant than a threat: Data suggest that such characters are a mixed
blessing in personal life (Robins & Beer, 2001), interpersonal life (Paulhus,
1998), and some organizational contexts (Hogan, Raskin, & Fazzini, 1990;
Robins & Paulhus, 2001). Adaptive interpersonal correlates of subclinical
psychopathy may be more difficult to find. Their positive self-view and lack
of anxiety, however, can be viewed as adaptive in an intrapsychic sense
(Taylor & Armor, 1996).
To summarize, our comparison of the Dark Triad of personalities does
not support the proposition that they are equivalent in normal populations.
Even in non-forensic, non-pathological, high-achievement populations, they
are distinctive enough to warrant separate measurement.
References
Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press.
Fehr, B., Samsom, D., & Paulhus, D. L. (1992). The construct of Machiavellianism: Twenty
years later. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment
(Vol. 9, pp. 77–116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Forth, A. E., Brown, S. L., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1996). The assessment of psychopathy in
male and female noncriminals: Reliability and validity. Personality and Individual
Differences, 20, 531–543.
Gretton, H. M. (1998). Psychopathy and recidivism in adolescence: A ten-year retrospective
follow-up. Unpublished dissertation, University of British Columbia.
Gustafson, S. B., & Ritzer, D. R. (1995). The dark side of normal: A psychopathy-linked
pattern called aberrant self-promotion. European Journal of Personality, 9, 147–183.
Hare, R. D. (1985). Comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 7–16.
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare psychopathy checklist-revised (PCL-R). Toronto, Ontario: Multi-
Health Systems.
Hart, S., & Hare, R. D. (1998). Association between psychopathy and narcissism: Theoretical
views and empirical evidence. In E. F. Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of narcissism:
Diagnostic, clinical, and empirical implications (pp. 415–436). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Helgeson, V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (1999). Unmitigated agency and unmitigated communion:
Distinctions from agency and communion. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 131–158.
Hogan, R., Raskin, R., & Fazzini, D. (1990). The dark side of charisma. In K. E. Clark et al.
(Eds.), Measures of leadership (pp. 343–354). West Orange, NJ: Leadership Library of
America.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in
self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
66, 206–219.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality:
Theory and research (2nd ed.., pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford.
Kowalski, R. M. (Ed.). (2001). Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-
report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 66, 488–524.
562 Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563
Lynam, D. R., Moffitt, T., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1993). Explaining the relation between
IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school-failure, or self-control? Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 102, 187–196.
McHoskey, J. (1995). Narcissism and Machiavellianism. Psychological Reports, 77, 755–759.
McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 192–210.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Expanding the dynamic self-regulatory processing model
of narcissism: Research directions for the future. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 243–251.
Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A
mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1197–1208.
Paulhus, D. L., Harms, P. D., Bruce, M. N., & Lysy, D. (in press). The Over-Claiming
Questionnaire: Measuring self-enhancement independent of ability. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology.
Paulhus, D. L., Hemphill, J., & Hare, R. (in press). Manual for the Self-report psychopathy scale
(SRP-III). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic bias in self-perceptions: The
interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of Personality, 66,
1024–1060.
Paulhus, D. L., Fridhandler, B., & Hayes, S. (1997). Psychological defense: Contemporary
theory and research. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of
personality psychology (pp. 543–579). San Diego: Academic Press.
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark side of normal personality: Self-report and
behavioral correlates. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia.
Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Psychological Reports,
45, 590.
Raskin, R. N., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. T. (1991). Narcissism, self-esteem, and defensive self-
enhancement. Journal of Personality, 59, 19–38.
Robins, R. W., & Beer, J. S. (2001). Positive illusions about the self: Short-term benefits and
long-term costs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 340–352.
Robins, R. W., & Paulhus, D. L. (2001). The character of self-enhancers: Implications for
organizations. In B. W. Roberts & R. Hogan (Eds.), Personality psychology in the workplace
(pp. 193–222). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive illusions and coping with adversity. Journal of
Personality, 64, 873–898.
Wonderlic, E. F. (1977). Wonderlic Personnel Test manual. Northfield, IL: Personnel Testing.
Williams, K., & Paulhus, D. L. (2002). The hierarchical factor structure of the Self-Report
Psychopathy scale. Presented at the meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association,
Vancouver, Canada.
Brief report / Journal of Research in Personality 36 (2002) 556–563 563
... One way of exploring the first question, which could help advance discussion of the second, is the Turing test (Turing, 1950) along with a moral variation of it (Allen et al., 2000(Allen et al., , 2006. After all, at the artificial level, just as at the human level, intelligence is considered linked to morality (see Serafimova, 2020;Luccioni and Bengio, 2019;Shank and DeSanti, 2018 (Paulhus and Williams, 2002) entail, to varying degrees, a socially malevolent character with tendencies toward emotional coldness, self-promotion, duplicity and aggressiveness, but otherwise seem very intelligent putting in place the means to achieve their objectives. However, intelligence in this case is perhaps not so given. ...
... However, intelligence in this case is perhaps not so given. Paulhus and Williams (2002) found small positive correlation only between IQ and narcissism as far as cognitive ability is concerned. In addition, the psychopath argument can be also addressed if we consider the psychopath's intelligence as suffering or problematic and at least different from that of other people. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
In this chapter, the following argument will be presented: Human intelligence has a moral imprint that prima facie distinguishes it from its artificial counterpart. This leads to the finding that true artificial intelligence (AI) is or has to be directly related to an artificial morality. That link already exists since the Turing test (TT) is, at its core, a moral test. These premises are especially important today because of the “mechanization” of humans˙ a state we are involved in. Machines are setting the regulatory framework of interaction with people. In this way, they “mechanize” them, with the encouragement of their, already initiated, fellow humans. Furthermore, AI-based systems acquire a moral dimension that is often overlooked and could lead to challenging situations. As to what criteria should be used to determine artificial moral behaviour, there is a phenomenological aspect behind them. What most people believe, is what is most likely to lead to an "intuitive algorithm". An algorithm corresponding to human intuitions, outside strict normative standards, that may lead to artificial autonomy and moral conduct. This kind of system that would have succeed in passing the TT will be the one that has already programmed humans in such a way that we will accept it as a (disguised) member of our species.
... One way of exploring the first question, which could help advance discussion of the second, is the Turing test (Turing, 1950) along with a moral variation of it (Allen et al., 2000(Allen et al., , 2006. After all, at the artificial level, just as at the human level, intelligence is considered linked to morality (see Serafimova, 2020;Luccioni and Bengio, 2019;Shank and DeSanti, 2018 (Paulhus and Williams, 2002) entail, to varying degrees, a socially malevolent character with tendencies toward emotional coldness, self-promotion, duplicity and aggressiveness, but otherwise seem very intelligent putting in place the means to achieve their objectives. However, intelligence in this case is perhaps not so given. ...
... However, intelligence in this case is perhaps not so given. Paulhus and Williams (2002) found small positive correlation only between IQ and narcissism as far as cognitive ability is concerned. In addition, the psychopath argument can be also addressed if we consider the psychopath's intelligence as suffering or problematic and at least different from that of other people. ...
... 7,8 Many of these patterns of negative behaviors have been conceptualized in a theoretical model called the Dark Triad of personality, which has been studied for several decades due to its common antagonistic social characteristics. 9 Individuals with these traits are characterized by a profile of manipulative, dominant, and dishonest intentionality in social relationships, as well as exhibiting behavior lacking in empathy. 10 Machiavellianism involves a pattern of manipulation tactics in social interactions, low levels of empathy, a focus on obtaining long-term rewards or power, and indifference to moral norms. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The study of Dark Triad traits and emotional variables has been conducted in numerous regions; however, there is a lack of research focusing on Latin American samples and considering variables such as sex and age. Incorporating these variables could enhance our understanding of underlying aversive patterns. Purpose: This study aimed to analyze the associations between Dark Triad traits and emotional intelligence in Peruvian adults. Methods: Data were collected in 2021 and 272 adults (M=31.8 years, 68% female) participated through online surveys via non-probability purposive sampling. Dirty Dozen and Trait Meta-Mood Scales were used. Two partial correlation network models were applied between the study variables with and without controlling for gender and age. Bridge-expected influence and predictability analysis were calculated to find interconnected traits and higher connections, respectively. Results: Subclinical psychopathy primarily showed negative associations with emotional clarity (r=−0.17), followed by emotional attention (r=−0.08) and emotional repair in networks with and without age and gender variables. Machiavellianism consistently displayed a negative association with emotional repair (r=−0.13) in both estimated networks. Conversely, subclinical narcissism exhibited a positive association solely with emotional attention (r=0.15). Furthermore, younger age was linked to higher Machiavellianism (r=−0.23), females showed higher emotional attention (r=0.11), and narcissism connected intrapersonal emotional domains with Dark Traits (bridge expected influence >1.25) while Machiavellianism exhibited greater predictability (r 2 >0.45) due to reinforcement by other Dark Traits. Conclusion: This study revealed negative connections between the Dark Triad and emotional intelligence traits, except for narcissism, which was positively associated with emotional attention and functioned as a "bridge" between all traits. However, relying solely on this single positive emotional aspect of narcissism may not be adequate to characterize it as possessing fully adaptive emotional traits. Findings suggest that detailed aspects of emotional intelligence and sociodemographic factors can provide valuable insights into the complex nature of aversive patterns and their broader impact on society.
... In this study we investigate whether personality traits that reflect prosocial orientation are linked to coping strategies teachers are using. The Light Triad of personality In the last two decades, theoretical models that describe personality in terms of the dark triad have gained popularity. Dark triad refers to a series of negative personality traits that describe people who are manipulative, duplicitous, interested in self-promotion, emotionally cold and aggressive: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, Psychopathy (Paulhus and Williams, 2002). In recent years, interest has turned to personality attributes that reflect prosocial aspects such as empathy, compassion and altruism. ...
Article
Studies show that work-related stress among teachers is high. Job satisfaction and interactions with students are influenced by teachers’ coping skills with stressful situations and differences in response to stressful events can be explained by personality traits. The present study uses the Light Triad model of personality to examine whether prosocial traits such as empathy, compassion and altruism are associated with cognitive-emotional coping strategies in teachers. The prosocial orientation was measured using the Light Triad Scale (Johnson, 2018) and The Cognitive-Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (Garnefski et al., 2007) was used to identify teachers’ cognitive-emotional coping strategies, in relation to negative events or situations. The participants in this study included 120 Romanian teachers from different levels of preuniversitary education. The results revealed that empathy and altruism have small negative correlations with catastrophizing; all three prosocial traits are associated with the positive reappraisal, empathy positively correlates with accepting negative events and refocusing on planning and altruism positively correlates with accepting negative events and putting into perspective. We also found a positive correlation between empathy and rumination. Our findings extend the current understanding of the relationship between teachers’ prosocial personality traits and the coping strategies they use following negative life events.
... This concern has arisen in light of Zhou et al.'s (2015) findings where, contrary to negative grandiose narcissism-agreeableness relationship found in Western studies (e.g. Paulhus and Williams 2002), grandiose narcissism consistently showed a positive relationship with agreeableness in Chinese samples. Moreover, informant-reported agreeableness of people high on grandiose narcissism was not only significantly lower than self-reported agreeableness, but also negatively related to grandiose narcissism controlling for selfesteem. ...
... These four traits have vastly been found to be intercorrelated (e.g., Vernon et al., 2008; for reviews, see Muris et al., 2017;Schreiber & Marcus, 2020), but each trait has its own distinctive features. Narcissism is characterized by inflated selfesteem, feelings of grandiosity, an excessive need for admiration (e.g., Paulhus & Williams, 2002;Paulhus, 1998), and a tendency for leadership (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Psychopathy involves a superficial charm, emotional coldness, a lack of feelings of remorse, reduced empathy (e.g., Wilson & McCarthy, 2011), impulsivity, and a need for stimulation (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). ...
Article
Previous studies have established relationships between the Dark Tetrad traits and sexual violence and its acceptance through myths about this type of violence. Sexual violence is positively associated with machismo, with sexist beliefs having been found to be linked with the Dark Tetrad and with the acceptance of sexual violence. Using a community sample comprising 362 adults between the ages of 18 and 70 (M = 35.6, SD = 14.3) and three self-report measures, this cross-sectional study aimed to explore the mediating role of sexual machismo in the relationship between each of the Dark Tetrad traits and the acceptance of sexual violence, both in the overall sample and by gender, as well as whether a significant variance in this acceptance is explained by the "dark" traits, sexual machismo, and gender. In the regression, sexual machismo, (the male) gender, and only Machiavellianism were uniquely associated with the acceptance of sexual violence, and sexual machismo partially mediated the associations between the Dark Tetrad and the acceptance of sexual violence. These findings indicate that being male and higher in sexism is more closely linked with the acceptance of sexual violence than most Dark Tetrad traits. Moreover, the associations between the Dark Tetrad, sexual machismo, and the acceptance of sexual violence were stronger in men, consistent with the notion that these traits facilitate a "male" exploitive mating strategy, which likely also extends to victim-blaming and positive attitudes about sexual violence more broadly. Lastly, the results emphasize the pervasiveness of beliefs about male superiority over women and its relationship with victim-blaming even in women.
Article
Ostracism and social exclusion are the two close phenomena that describe being ignored by others without explicit declaration and being kept away from others with or without an explanation, respectively. It is possible that ostracism and social exclusion are the concepts related to personality. Connectedly, dark triad (narcissism, psychopathy, Machiavellianism) and humility may influence one’s perceived ostracism or perceived social exclusion. The aim of the current study is to examine the mediating role of humility in the relationship between dark triad traits and perceived ostracism and social exclusion. The sample consists of 329 university students. Data was gathered by using the Ostracism Experience Scale, the Humility Scale and the Short Dark Triad Scale. The results demonstrate that dark triad is positively related to perceived ostracism, and this relationship was fully mediated by humility. Similarly, dark triad traits were positively related to social exclusion and this association is fully mediated by humility, as well. Humility might be said to contribute as a mediator between the dark triad and perceived ostracism and social exclusion by mitigating the detrimental impact of these characteristics on ostracism. The negative consequences of the dark triad and ostracism could be reduced by encouraging humility. The results are discussed in the light of the literature.
Article
Full-text available
Reactions to trait self-enhancers were investigated in 2 longitudinal studies of person.perception in discussion groups. Groups of 4-6 participants met 7 times for 20 rain. After Meetings 1 and 7, group members rated their perceptions of one another. In Study 1, trait self-enhancement was indexed by measures of narcissism and self-deceptive enhancement. At the first meeting, self-enhancers made positive impressions: They were seen as agreeable, well adjusted, and competent. After 7 weeks, however, they were rated negatively and gave self-evaluations discrepant with peer evaluations they received. In Study 2, an independent sample of observers (close acquaintances) enabled a pretest index of discrepancy self-enhancement: It predicted the same deteriorating pattern of interpersonal perceptions as the other three trait measures. Nonetheless, all self-enhancement measures correlated positively with self-esteem.
Article
Full-text available
The literature on personality traits and defense mechanisms suggests individual differences in two self-favoring tendencies, which we label “egoistic bias” and “moralistic bias.” The two biases are self-deceptive in nature and can be traced to two fundamental values, agency and communion, that impel two corresponding motives, nPower and nApproval. The two sequences of values, motives, and biases form two personality constellations, Alpha and Gamma. Associated with Alpha is an egoistic bias, a self-deceptive tendency to exaggerate one's social and intellectual status. This tendency leads to unrealistically positive self-perceptions on such traits as dominance, fearlessness, emotional stability, intellect, and creativity. Self-perceptions of high Alpha scorers have a narcissistic, “superhero” quality. Associated with Gamma is a moralistic bias, a self-deceptive tendency to deny socially deviant impulses and to claim sanctimonious “saint-like” attributes. This tendency is played out in overly positive self-perceptions on such traits as agreeableness, dutifulness, and restraint. The Alpha-Gamma conception provides an integrative framework for a number of central issues in personality psychology.
Chapter
The Psychopathy Checklist (PCL; Hare, 1980) and its revision (PCL-R; Hare, 1985a, in press) are clinical rating scales that provide researchers and clinicians with reliable and valid assessments of psychopathy. Their development was spurred largely by dissatisfactions with the ways in which other assessment procedures defined and measured psychopathy (Hare, 1980, 1985b).
Article
Several clinical-behavioral and self-report measures of psychopathy were compared in a sample of 274 male prison inmates. The assessment procedures included global clinical ratings, a 22-item checklist, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980) criteria for Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD), a self-report version of the 22-item checklist, the Socialization scale of the California Psychological Inventory, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Agreement among the various assessment procedures was evaluated with correlational analyses, discriminant function analyses, and kappa coefficients of diagnostic agreement. There was much stronger agreement among the clinical-behavioral measures (ratings, checklist, and DSM-III) than there was among the self-report measures. Agreement between these two measurement domains was, with few exceptions, rather poor.