ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Social–emotional competence is a key developmental task during early childhood. This study examined concurrent relationships between maternal education and employment status, children's sex, ethnicity, age, receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge, attention skills, inhibitory control and social–emotional competence in a sample of 146 preschool, low-income, ethnically diverse children from Head Start classrooms. Multilevel models demonstrated that inhibitory control played a significant role in the concurrent prediction of teacher ratings of social–emotional competence above and beyond other variables associated with social–emotional competence. Children who demonstrated better inhibitory control were more likely to be rated higher on social skills and lower in internalizing behaviors. Findings suggest that early identification of inhibitory control difficulties may be beneficial for targeting children at risk for maladaptive outcomes. The contribution of environmental experience to the development of inhibitory control skills suggests there are many opportunities to intervene during early childhood.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The contribution of inhibitory control to preschoolers'
socialemotional competence
Brittany L. Rhoades , Mark T. Greenberg, Celene E. Domitrovich
Pennsylvania State University, USA
article info abstract
Available online 23 February 2009 Socialemotional competence is a key developmental task during early childhood. This study
examined concurrent relationships between maternal education and employment status,
children's sex, ethnicity, age, receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge, attention skills,
inhibitory control and socialemotional competence in a sample of 146 preschool, low-income,
ethnically diverse children from Head Start classrooms. Multilevel models demonstrated that
inhibitory control played a signicant role in the concurrent prediction of teacher ratings of
socialemotional competence above and beyond other variables associated with social
emotional competence. Children who demonstrated better inhibitory control were more likely
to be rated higher on social skills and lower in internalizing behaviors. Findings suggest that
early identication of inhibitory control difculties may be benecial for targeting children at
risk for maladaptive outcomes. The contribution of environmental experience to the
development of inhibitory control skills suggests there are many opportunities to intervene
during early childhood.
© 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
Executive function
Inhibitory control
Socialemotional competence
Preschoolers
1. Introduction
The development of socialemotional competence is a process of acquiring a set of interrelated skills that promote emotion
recognition and management, compassion for others, positive social relationships, and responsible and effective decision-making
(Denham & Weissberg, 2004; Greenberg, Weissberg, O'Brien, Zins, Fredericks, Resnik, 2003). The ability to manage behaviors and
emotions in accordance with societal expectations is an essential component of social-competence that is especially important
during early childhood as it has been shown to play an important role in school adjustment and achievement during kindergarten
(Howse, Calkins, Anastopoulos, Keane, & Shelton, 2003; Shields, Dickstein, Seifer, Giusti, & Spritz, 2001). There has been
considerable theorizing and some data indicating that executive function, and in particular inhibitory control, may facilitate this
process (Hughes, Dunn, & White,1998; Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Hershey, 1994). Inhibitory control, the
cognitive-related ability to inhibit a strong dominant response in favor of a subdominant one, could serve as a key internal resource
for young children faced with the social challenges presented by a preschool classroom.
To better understand the role inhibitory control plays in preschool children's development of socialemotional competence, the
present study used an at-risk, low-income sample of preschoolers to examine the concurrent relationships between inhibitory
control abilities and teacher-rated social skills, internalizing and externalizing problems above and beyond other factors known to
be associated with these behaviors.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
Support for this research was provided by Head Start/University Partnership Grant (#90YD0063) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA 017629).
Corresponding author. Pennsylvania State University, 113 S. Henderson Bldg., University Park, PA 16802, USA. Tel.: +1 814 865 9373; fax: +1 814 865 2530.
E-mail address: blr162@psu.edu (B.L. Rhoades).
0193-3973/$ see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.012
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
1.1. Inhibitory control in early childhood
Preschool represents an important developmental period to study individual differences in inhibitory control and their
associations with behavioral outcomes. Inhibitory control emerges in late infancy and exhibits dramatic development in the
preschool years (Diamond, 2002; Rothbart, 1989). Research consistently shows that young children have difculty inhibiting
impulses (Diamond, Kirkham, & Amso, 2002; Diamond & Taylor,1996; Dowsett & Livesey, 2000; Gerstadt, Hong, & Diamond,1994).
Although age-related improvements in inhibitory control occur from three to six years, children less than ve years old still have
substantial difculty inhibiting a prepotent response (Gerstadt et al., 1994).
Inhibitory control has been conceptualized in a variety of ways throughout the early childhood literature (see Kochanska,
Murray, & Harlan, 2000). Effortful control was rst introduced by Rothbart and colleagues as the self-regulatory aspect of
temperament that enables children to regulate their anger and approach systems (Rothbart,1989; Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Effortful
control is part of the active inhibitory system and has been described as the ability to inhibit a prepotent or dominant response in
favor of a subdominant one (Kochanska et al., 2000; Olson, Sameroff, Kerr, Lopez, & Wellman, 2005). It is believed that effortful
control allows children to regulate their actions in the service of current and future goals (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005).
Effortful control rst emerges in the second half of the rst year of life with individual differences becoming increasingly stable
during toddlerhood and into preschool (Kochanska et al., 2000).
Other researchers suggest that inhibitory control is one aspect of the multi-component construct of executive function (EF). EF
contributes to the ability to coordinate thoughts and actions in a goal-directed manner (Kopp, 1989; Perner & Lang, 1999) and
includes planning, working memory, and attentional control skills in addition to inhibitory control (Hughes, 1998; Pennington,
1997; Welsh, Pennington, & Groisser,1991). Rudimentary forms of EF emerge in the rst year of life and continue to develop until at
least early adulthood (Dempster & Corkhill, 1999; Diamond, 1991; Welsh & Pennington, 1988). EF research suggests that inhibitory
control is especially relevant to the developmentof children's abilities to regulate their behaviors and emotions (Carlson, Moses, &
Brenton, 2002; Hughes et al., 1998; Hughes, White, Sharpen & Dunn, 2000 ). However, it is important to note that because EF tasks
are inherently complex, no pure measure of any single EF component exists. Most EF tasks (including the ones used in the present
study) tap into multiple components (Diamond, Kirkham, & Amso, 2002; Pennington, 1997).
Olson and colleagues posit that inhibitory control is one facet of the broader multi-dimensional construct of impulse control
(Olson, 1989; Olson, Schilling, & Bates, 1999). Their ndings suggest that impulse control is composed of a cognitive dimension and
a delay dimension. Inhibitory control falls under the cognitive dimension and is generally measured using highly structured tasks
where children are required to inhibit action in a non-emotional task. On the other hand, the delay dimension of impulse control
has been measured using tasks like delay of graticationor resistance to temptationwhere children are required to comply
with social expectations of appropriate behavior in a more emotionally-eliciting situation (Olson, 1989). Some research indicates
that the delay dimension of impulse control may be a stronger predictor of children's outcomes than the cognitive dimension
(Olson, 1989; Olson & Hoza, 1993; Olson et al., 1999). However, the cognitive dimension remains a signicant aspect of impulse
control, which may inuence children's classroom behaviors.
Children's ability to inhibit impulses plays a crucial role in everyday peer and adult interactions in the preschool classroom.
For instance, preschool children are likely to encounter peer conict in situations when other children obstruct or interfere with
their goals (e.g., another child takes away the toy he/she was playing with/takes his/her place in line etc.). For many children,
their natural tendency may be to use physical aggression (e.g., hit the child who took his/her toy away, push the child who took
his/her space in line) to attain their goals. However, children who are better able to inhibit their natural tendency to use physical
aggression (i.e., dominant response) and instead use their words (i.e., subdominant response) to attain their goals are likely to
be viewed by their peers and teachers as better play partners and therefore more socially competent. On the other hand, those
children who are unable to inhibit their impulses are more likely to exhibit externalizing behaviors such as physical aggression
and to be viewed by their peers and teachers as poor play partners. They may be rejected from play groups, which may lead to
internalizing behaviors.
1.2. The relationship between inhibitory control and behavior
Several studies have investigated the relationship between inhibitory control skills and behavioral outcomes in middle-class
children. Kochanska and colleagues reported that effortful/inhibitory control in early childhood was longitudinallyrelated to better
emotion regulation, fewer externalizing problems, stronger conscience, and greater committed compliance later in childhood
(Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Kochanska et al., 2000, 20 01). Olson et al. (2005) reported that children's effortful control at age three
was a signicant negative predictor of externalizing behavior above and beyond IQ, dispositional anger, and psychosocial adversity
in a middle-class sample. In another study, Olson et al. (1999) found that laboratory measures of both cognitive and delay
dimensions of inhibitory control at six and eight years old predicted parent and self-ratings of externalizing behavior problems
during adolescence.
Research also has examined inhibitory control in young children at risk for developing ADHD (Campbell, Pierce, March,
Ewing, & Szumowski, 1994; Hughes et al., 1998, 2000; Perner, Winfried, & Barchfeld, 2002). Hughes and colleagues found that
although preschoolers with high rates of hyperactive symptoms were more likely to fail a variety of EF tasks (i.e., planning,
working memory, inhibition, attentional exibility), inhibitory control was the only EF dimension that remained signicant
after controlling for verbal skill, age, and family background (Hughes et al., 1998). Additionally, inhibitory control skills were
negatively related to antisocial behavior and positively related to prosocial behaviors during peer interactions (Hughes et al.,
311B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
2000). Hughes and colleagues consistently report that inhibitory control is the most important skill predicting young children's
behavior.
Others have examined EF, broadly dened, in children with elevated problem behaviors (Cole, Usher, & Cargo, 1993; Kusche,
Cook, & Greenberg, 1993; Seguin & Zelazo, 2005; Speltz, DeKlyen, Calderon, Greenberg, & Fisher, 1999). The majority of these
studies created composite EF scores by aggregating across various tasks, thereby overlooking the signicant role of inhibitory
control. They found that children exhibiting behavior problems had poorer EF skills in general compared to normally developing
children. Within an intervention context, Riggs, Greenberg, Kuche, & Pentz (2006) found that children exposed to a social
emotional intervention had fewer problem behaviors at one year post-intervention as compared to control children and that this
intervention effect was mediated by improvements in inhibitory control.
Although the aforementioned research, largely conducted on middle class samples, suggests that inhibitory control is central to
the development of socialemotional competence, it is important to investigate whether this relationship holds for low-income
children as well in order to understand the generalizability of these ndings. Very few reports have examined inhibitory control in
samples of disadvantaged children (Blair, 2003; Blair & Peters, 2003). In a sample of Head Start children, Blair (2003) and Blair and
Peters (2003), reported a positive association between teachers' reports of children's on task behavior in the classroom and high
levels of inhibitory control as assessed by a composite score of the Peg Tapping and Day/Night tasks. Although this provides some
support for the generalization of this relationship to disadvantaged population, the present study aims to provide more evidence
by examining these relationships in a larger and more ethnically diverse sample of Head Start children.
1.3. Predictors of socialemotional competence in preschool
The above ndings suggest that children who are better able to inhibit their behavior have better social skills and fewer
behavior problems. However, there has been little research that has integrated the assessment of inhibitory control inpreschoolers
with the examination of other factors that have also been shown to predict social and emotional competence. Children's sex
(Lavigne, Gibbons, Christoffel, Arend, & Rosenbaum, 1995; Spieker, Larson, Lewis, Keller, & Gilchrist, 1999), academic skills (Malecki
& Elliott, 2002), emotional knowledge (Arsenio, Cooperman, & Lover, 2000; Denham, Blair, DeMulderr, Levitas, & Sawyer, 2003;
Fine, Izard, Mostow, Trentacosta, & Ackerman, 2003) and attention skills (Belsky, Friedman, & Hsieh, 2001; Eisenberg, Fabes,
Nyman, Bernzweig, & Pinuelas, 1994; Raver, Blackburn, Bancroft, & Torp, 1999) have been identied as important predictors of
socialemotional competence. Caregivers' education and employment status have also been shown to predict children's social
emotional skills (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). There has been little examination of how inhibitory control may interact with these
predictors or how it may relate to social competence and behavior problems in young children above and beyond the impact of the
aforementioned factors.
1.4. The present study
This study contributes to the eld in three ways. First, it examined the relationship between inhibitory control and social
emotional competence in a disadvantaged sample of ethnically-diverse, urbanpreschoolers. Second, by using multi-level modeling
it examined whether classroom-level effects inuence relations between factors and accurately accounted for the effect of
classroom-level effects on individual-level relations. Third, the present study controlled for factors already shown to inuence
socialemotional competence in order to isolate the impact of inhibitory control. To this aim, two primary research questions were
addressed:
1) What is the role of inhibitory control in predicting children's socialemotional competence, controlling for factors that have
already been shown to predict social skills and behavior problems (maternal education and employment status, children's sex,
ethnicity, age, receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge, attention)?
2) Does the relationship between inhibitory control and socialemotional competence vary by maternal education and
employment status, children's sex, ethnicity, receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge and/or attention skills?
It is hypothesized that children with better inhibitory control would have higher social skills and lower externalizing and
internalizing ratings, above and beyond the impact of the aforementioned factors. Because of the limited research on what factors
might moderate the relationship between inhibitory control and socialemotional competence, we did not pose any a priori
hypotheses regarding the moderation analyses.
2. Method
2.1. Design
The present study examined behavior of a subsetof preschool children from a randomized clinical trial of the PATHS (Promoting
Alternative THinking Strategies) preschool program, a curriculum developed to promote social and emotional competence
(Domitrovich, Greenberg, Cortes, & Kusche, 2005). Time 1 data for both control and intervention children were used in the present
analyses. Time 1 assessments were conducted at the beginning of the school year during the fall of 2000. As these data were
collected at the pretest/Time 1, no intervention had been initiated (see Domitrovich, Cortes, & Greenberg, 2007 for results from the
impact study).
312 B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
2.2. Participants
There were 246 children ranging in age from2.5 to 5 years old in the PATHS program at Time 1. However, onlychildren four and
older were included because of the need for valid assessment of the present study's constructs. This study therefore included 146
children who were 45 years old. See Table 1 for a description of the sample participants and their caregivers.
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Receptive vocabulary
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) is a well-established measure of children's
receptive vocabulary. The interviewer presented the child with four pictures and asked him/her to identify the picture of the word
that was orally presented to him/her. The measure consists of 175 items. The PPVT-R shows good convergent validity, and the split-
half reliability coefcient for the PPVT-R is .71 for children 48 to 53 months of age. The median internal consistency across age
groups is r= . 95 (Dunn & Dunn, 1981).
2.3.2. Emotional knowledge
The Kusche Emotional Inventory (KEI, Kusche, 1984) was developed to assess children's recognition of various emotion labels
(e.g., happy, sad, mad, scared, love). A version of the Recognition of Emotion Concepts subtest, adapted for 4-year olds, was used in
this study (Speltz et al., 1999). Children were presented with a page divided into four sections with four cartoon faces, each with a
particular emotional expression. Out of the four cartoons, there was one target picture and three distracter pictures. Children were
presented with 30 stimuli pages assessing 15 different emotions and asked to identify the cartoon picture that best matched the
emotion stated. For each trial, children received two points for the correct response and zero for an incorrect response. If children
chose a picture that depicted an emotional expression of the same valence (e.g., child answered sad for a mad expression) as the
target feeling word, they received one point. The highest possible score was 60. For the original version of this subtest, Kusche (in
press) reported a split-half reliability of .89 and testretest reliability of .85. In the present sample, there was adequate internal
consistency with a Cronbach's alpha coefcient of .73. Total percent correct was used as the predictor variable in the present
analyses.
2.3.3. Attention skills
The Leiter-Revised Attention Sustained Subtest (Leiter-R AS; Roid & Miller, 1997) assessed children's ability to sustain attention
to detail in a repetitive task. Children were shown a target gure (e.g., ower) located at the top of the stimulus and were instructed
to scan an array of gures and cross out all of the target gures as quickly as they can. Children were administered a series of four
boxes, each with a different target gure located in an increasingly more complicated array of gures. For each box, they were given
up to 30 s to complete the task. An adjusted total correct score was created by subtracting the total number of errors from the total
number of correct. This score was converted into an adjusted total scaled score where the average score is represented by a scaled
score of 10 (SD = 3). The Leiter-R AS has been shown to have good internal consistency with a Cronbach's alpha coefcient of .83 for
the 45 year old version and good testretest reliability of (r= .85).
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for participants and caregivers (N= 146).
Mean (SD) or percent
Participant characteristic
Age 54 months (3.6)
Female 54%
Ethnicity
African-American 49.3%
Anglo-American 38.4%
Hispanic 6.8%
Multi-racial or other 5.5%
Caregiver characteristic
Age 32 years (9.8)
Income $6642 (6320)
Relationship to child
Biological mother 81.3%
Relative, foster parent or guardian 18.3%
Employment
Full or part-time employed 60.3%
Unemployed 39.7%
Education
Some high school 22.8%
GED or high school diploma 50.4%
Some college 12.3%
Associates or bachelor degree 14.2%
313B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
2.3.4. Inhibitory control
Two tasks were used to assess children's inhibitory control skills. One measure was a Stroop-like task called Day/Night (D/N;
Gerstadt et al., 1994). This measure assesses children's ability to inhibit a natural tendency to give a verbal response while
remembering the rule for the correct response. Children were presented with a series of two types of cards. One was a black card
with stars and one was a white card with a bright sun. Children were instructed to say dayto the black cards and nightto the
white cards. Children were given the opportunity to practice each action following the experimenter's instructions. Then, children
were given up to three practice trials and praised for correct responses and corrected for incorrect responses. If children got any
trials correct during the practice trials, they were included as their rst two trials. In all, children were administered 16 trials in
which eight of each type of card was included in a counterbalanced sequence. The internal consistency was good within the
present sample with a KuderRichardson (KR-20) coefcient of .89.
The other measure of inhibitory control was Luria's (1966) tapping task called Peg Tapping (PT; Diamond & Taylor, 1996). This
measure was administered to assess children's ability to inhibit a natural tendency to mimic an action while remembering the rule
for the correct action. The administration procedures described by Diamond and her colleagues were followed. Children were
instructed to tap a wooden dowel (15-cm length ×1-cm diameter) twice when the experimenter tapped once and to tap once when
the experimenter tapped twice. The procedures for administration of the PT task were similar to those described above for the D/N
task. In all, children were administered 16 trials in which eight of each action (one-tap and two-tap) were included in a
counterbalanced sequence. The internal consistency was good within the present sample with a KR-20 of .88. Although both tasks
are well-established measures of inhibitory control, it is important to recognize that neither is solely a measure of inhibitory
control, but also requires working memory. For the purpose of the present study, however, these tasks will be referred to as
inhibitory control tasks.
Similar to previous work with these two measures, the scores on D/N and PT were moderately correlated (r=. 42, pb. 001).
Although previous studies have created a single indicator of inhibitory control by averaging the total percent correct on both D/N
and PT (e.g., Blair, 2003; Blair & Peters, 2003), the present study analyzed scores for each measure separately in order to examine
their independent contributions in the prediction of children's socialemotional competence.
2.3.5. Socialemotional competence
The Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales (PKBS; Merrell, 1996), a teacher report measure, was designed to assess social
skills and problem behaviors during the past month in 36 year old children. The Social Skills scale consisted of 34 items that
describe adaptive or positive behaviors associated with positive personal and social outcomes (e.g., is cooperative, follows
directions, shows self-control). These items were designed to reect both peer-related and adult-related forms of social
adjustment. The Problem Behavior scale consists of 42 items that were designed to reect common preschool and kindergarten
children behavior problems (e.g., has tantrums, will not share, is physically aggressive). The two subscales, externalizing and
internalizing behaviors, were used for the present study. For all items, responses were based on a four-point scale: 0 = never true,
1 = rarely true, 2 = sometimes true, and 3 = often true. The subscales showed good internal consistency, with Cronbach's alpha
coefcients ranging from .84 to .97 in the present sample. The present study used mean scores for each scale as the outcome
variables.
2.4. Procedure
Children were tested outside the classroom and completed the battery of measures in a quiet area of the Head Start center
during two separate, 30-minute sessions within a two-month period. The rst session included the Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Task-Revised and the Kusche Emotional Inventory. The second session included the Leiter-R Sustained Attention Task, Day/Night
Task and Peg Tapping. Teacher ratings of children's behaviors were collected at this time as well.
Demographic information was collected from the primary caregiver at the rst home visit with the child's teacher. It included
questions about the caregiver's and child's ethnicity, age, and the caregiver's relationship to the child, his or her education,
employment status, current income, and marital status.
3. Results
3.1. Missing data treatment
Out of the total sample (N= 146), 25 children were missingat least one child-based assessment: one child was missing only the
PPVT-R; three children were missing only the Leiter-R AS; ve children were missing only the D/N measure; six children were
missing only the PT measure; one child was missing both the KEI and the Leiter-R AS measures; one child was missing both the
Leiter-R AS and the PT measures; and eight childrenwere missing both the D/N and PT measures. Also, two children did not receive
teacher ratings at Time 1. Reasons for missing data include interviewer error, child refusal to complete the measure, and child's
inability to understand the instructions. To effectively use all available data, multiple imputation was used to calculate the
parameter estimates. For the present analyses, ve datasets (Schafer & Olsen, 1998) were imputed using PROC MI in SAS v9.1. PROC
MI Analyze was used to combine parameter estimates from the imputed datasets (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Although the
parameter estimates were very similar using the multiple imputation procedure compared to listwise deletion, the multiple
imputation analyses increases power to detect signicant differences and corrects for any potential selection bias for children who
314 B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
completed the measures compared to those who did not. Therefore, the values obtained using PROC MI Analyze will be reported in
the following sections.
3.2. Preliminary analyses
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics for the child and teacher report variables. In this low-income sample, children scored
close to one standard deviation below the national norm for receptive vocabulary. However, they demonstrated average sustained
attention with scores comparable with the national norm. On average, they achieved a somewhat greater percent of items correct
on the Day/Night measure of inhibitory control compared to scores on the Peg Tapping measure. In general, the children in this
sample were rated by teachers as having good social skills and low levels of externalizing and internalizing behaviors at the
beginning of the school year.
Table 3 presents the correlations among the child variables. Receptive vocabulary was strongly related to emotional knowledge
and moderately related to sustained attention. There was a signicant positive relation between the inhibitory control, as
measured by PT, and both emotional knowledge and sustained attention. Emotional knowledge was moderately, positively
correlated with sustained attention.
Table 3 also presents correlations between the child constructs and the three teacher-rated subscales. There were no signicant
relationships between sustained attention and social skills or problem behaviors. Inhibitory control, as measured by both D/N and
PT, was positively correlated with social skills. However, only PT was signicantly (negatively) correlated with externalizing and
internalizing problems. Both receptive vocabulary and emotional knowledge were related to children's social skills, indicating
these scores should be covaried in order to examine the role of inhibitory control.
3.3. Analytic method for the multi-level models
Because children in this sample were nested within classrooms, multilevel models with child-level variables at level 1 and
classroom membership at level 2 were used to examine the relationship between inhibitory control and teacher-rated social
emotional competence. By specifying a 2-level model we were able to control for any interdependence in teacher ratings due to
children being in the same classroom and having the same rater. The intraclass correlation (ICC) from the empty model (i.e., no
predictors included) was .16 for social skills, .23 for externalizing behavior, and .37 for internalizing behaviors. These numbers
represent the correlation of teacher ratings for children in the same classroom.
For the 2-level models predicting each of the three outcomes, child-level variables were created as predictors. The child-level
variables were grand-mean centered and represent between-person differences. For instance, the child-level PPVT-R variable (i.e.,
receptive vocabulary) represents the deviation between the overall mean PPVT-R score (90) and the child's individual PPVT-R
score. SAS PROC MIXED was used to estimate linear mixed models to assess the between-person differences in maternal education
and employment status, children's sex, ethnicity, age, receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge, attention skills, and inhibitory
Table 2
Mean (and SD) child measure scores and teacher-rated outcomes.
Measure Mean (SD)
PPVT standard score 89.61 (12.81)
KEI % correct 65.11 (13.05)
Leiter-R AS adjusted correct scaled score 10.75 (2.90)
D/N % correct 78.38 (24.88)
PT % correct 68.21 (29.78)
PKBS social skills 2.35 (.48)
PKBS externalizing .75 (.60)
PKBS internalizing .64 (.60)
Table 3
Concurrent correlations among child constructs and between child constructs and socialemotional competence.
Receptive vocabulary Emotional knowledge Sustained attention IC: day/night IC: peg tapping
Receptive vocabulary
Emotional knowledge .52***
Sustained attention .25** .21*
IC: day night .03 .14 .01
IC: peg tapping .15 .29** .26** .42***
Social skills .38*** .35*** .13 .19* .34**
Externalizing .19* .11 .13 .08 .22*
Internalizing .16 .05 .10 .15 .35***
*pb.05, **pb.01, ***pb.001.
315B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
control in the prediction of social skills, externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. For each model, xed effects of the
predictor variables were added to the empty 2-level model. Model parameters are reported in Table 4.
3.4. The role of inhibitory control in prediction of socialemotional competence
3.4.1. Social skills
Parameter estimates for this model can be found on the left panel of Table 4. The xed effects of receptive vocabulary (pb. 05)
and inhibitory control (as measured by PT; pb. 01) were signicant. The parameter estimates suggest that children with greater
receptive vocabulary and greater inhibitory control skills are more socially competent.
3.4.2. Externalizing behaviors
Parameter estimates for this model can be found in the middle panel of Table 4. Only the xed effect of receptive vocabulary (pb
. 05) was signicant. The parameter estimate suggests that children with greater receptive vocabulary have fewer externalizing
behaviors.
3.4.3. Internalizing behaviors
Parameter estimates for this model can be found on the right panel of Table 4. The xed effects of receptive vocabulary (pb.01)
and inhibitory control (as measured by PT; pb. 01) were signicant. The parameter estimates suggest that children with greater
receptive vocabulary and greater inhibitory control skills have fewer internalizing problems.
3.4.4. Moderation effects
The two-way interactions between inhibitory control and maternal education, maternal employment status, children's sex,
ethnicity (Anglo-American vs. non Anglo-American), receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge, and attention skills were each
added separately to each of the above 2-level models to determine if the relationships between inhibitory control and social
emotional competence varied by these factors. None of the two-way interactions were signicant in any of the three models. These
ndings suggested that the relationship between inhibitory control and socialemotional competence was similar for children
with mothers of varying levels of education and employment status. It was also similar for boys and girls, Anglo-American and
non-Anglo-American children and children with different levels of receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge and attentionskills.
4. Discussion
This study extends previous research by more closely examining the relationships between inhibitory control and social
emotional competence in low-income preschoolers. As hypothesized, the study found that children with better inhibitory control
(as measured by PT) showed better behavioral adjustment compared to those with poorer inhibitory control skills. The ndings
indicated that after controlling for other variables known to be related to children's socialemotional competence, inhibitory
control (as measured by PT) was a signicant, unique predictor of social skills and internalizing problems. Children who had
greater inhibitory control were more likely to be rated higher in social skills and lower in internalizing behavior. Although
inhibitory control was not signicant at the pb.05 level in the model predicting externalizing behavior, the parameter estimate
indicated a relationship in the negative direction, which is consistent with the hypotheses. The exploratory moderation analyses
indicated that these relationships did not vary by maternal education, maternal employment status or children's sex, ethnicity,
receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge or attention skills.
These ndings provide further evidence for the central role of inhibitory control in distinguishing children with adjustment
problems (Blair, 2003; Blair & Peters, 2003; Hughes et al., 1998, 2000). Research has consistently shown that inhibitory control is
crucial in the development of social skills (Berlin & Bohlin, 2002; Brophy, Taylor, & Hughes, 2002; Hughes et al., 1998; Eisenberg,
Spinrad, Fabes, Reiser, Cumberland, 2004) and may help explain the association between cognitive decits and disruptive behavior
Table 4
Parameter estimates for multi-level model predicting social skills, externalizing problems and internalizing problems.
Fixed effects Social skills Externalizing problems Internalizing problems
Estimate SE p value Estimate SE p value Estimate SE p value
Intercept 2.274 0.083 b.0001 0.699 0.138 b.0001 0.588 0.115 b.0001
Maternal education 0.042 0.056 0.4487 0.096 0.082 0.2457 0.007 0.059 0.9106
Maternal employment status 0.013 0.045 0.7799 0.087 0.065 0.1847 0.041 0.048 0.3901
Sex 0.077 0.073 0.2897 0.028 0.104 0.7884 0.053 0.077 0.4950
Ethnicity 0.125 0.083 0.1306 0.027 0.122 0.8235 0.037 0.092 0.6847
Age 0.035 0.129 0.7842 0.011 0.183 0.9525 0.014 0.136 0.9158
Receptive vocabulary 0.009 0.003 0.0106 0.010 0.005 0.0361 0.010 0.004 0.0101
Emotional knowledge 0.005 0.003 0.1667 0.002 0.005 0.6676 0.003 0.004 0.4405
Sustained attention 0.009 0.015 0.5469 0.002 0.020 0.9311 0.014 0.014 0.3141
Inhibitory control: D/N 0.001 0.002 0.4156 0.000 0.003 0.8960 0.000 0.002 0.9695
Inhibitory control: PT 0.005 0.002 0.0240 0.004 0.003 0.2321 0.006 0.002 0.0008
Note. Bolded parameter estimates are signicant at pb.05.
316 B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
(Charman, Carroll, & Sturge, 2001; Quay, 1997; Rubia, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, Brandeis, & Leeuwen, 1998; Songuga-Barke, Dalen,
Daley, & Remmington, 2002). In all, these ndings suggest children's ability to inhibit prepotent responses is a signicant predictor
of social skills and problem behaviors above and beyond the impact of receptive vocabulary.
These conclusions must be tempered, however, by two considerations. First, like other measures of EF, PT and D/N are not pure
measures of one individual component of EF. Instead, they measure inhibitory control in the contextof a working memory demand.
Although the results suggest that children's inhibitory control plays an important role in predicting children's socialemotional
competence above and beyond maternal education and employment status, and children's sex, ethnicity, age, receptive vocabulary,
emotional knowledge, and attention, the individual contribution of inhibitory control cannot be disentangled from working
memory.
Second, the present study demonstrated that all inhibitory control tasks are not created equal. In the present analyses two
commonly used measures of inhibitory control for young children, PT and D/N were only moderated correlated and were
differentially related to socialemotional competence. Previous studies that utilized both of these measures created a composite
inhibitory control score by averaging the PT and D/N scores (Blair, 2003; Blair & Peters, 2003), which masked the individual
contributions of the two measures. Our analyses suggest that compared to D/N, PT is a more robust predictor of child behaviors. It
may be that D/N is not as sensitive to individual differences in inhibitory control for disadvantaged preschool children or that the
ability to inhibit a verbal response (as compared to a motor response) is less predictive of children's behavior. PT is designed to
assess children's ability to inhibit a motor response, which particularly during early childhood when aggression is at its peak, may
be more predictive of their socialemotional competence in the classroom.
4.1. Prevention implications
Numerous preschool prevention programs are aimed at improving children's intellectual ability (e.g., Ramey & Ramey, 2004;
Weikart & Schweinhart, 1997). However, the results from the present study would suggest that inhibitory control skills are
essential to children's socialemotional competence. Preschool children who have difculties with inhibitory control skills are
clearly at risk for lower socialemotional competence. Children who exhibit increased problem behaviors during early childhood
are at risk for numerous poor developmental outcomes later in life including antisocial behavior, mental health problems, school
failure, poor peer relationships, and unemployment (Broidy, Nagin, Tremblay, Bates, Brame, 2003; Caspi, Moftt, Wright, & Silva,
1998; Henry, Caspi, Moftt, & Silva,1996; Jimerson, Egeland, Sroufe, & Carlson, 2000; Moftt, 1993; Moftt, Caspi, Dickson, Silva, &
Stanton, 1996; Robins & Price, 1991; Tremblay, Phil, Vitaro, & Dobkin, 1994). Therefore, early identication of inhibitory control
difculties may be benecial for targeting children at-risk for these maladaptive outcomes. Research highlights the role of
environmental experience in the development of inhibitory control skills, suggesting there are many opportunities to intervene
during early childhood (Dowsett & Livesey, 2000).
Although there are very few programs that directly target executive functions such as inhibitory control (Diamond, Barnett,
Thomas, & Munro, 2007; Klingberg, Fernell, Olesen, Johnson, & Gustafsson, 2005; Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, &
Posner, 2005), researchers are increasingly recognizing the importance of incorporating theoretical models of neurocognitive
functioning in the development and evaluation of prevention programs aimed at reducing problem behaviors and enhancing social
competence (Greenberg, Riggs, & Blair, 2007; Riggs et al., 2006). Numerous evidence-based prevention programs whose goals are
to promote self-regulation and reduce problem behaviors in preschoolers already currently exist (Domitrovich et al., 2007; Lynch,
Geller, & Schmidt, 2004; Serna, Nielsen, Lambros & Forness, 2000; Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2003). Although these programs were
not designed to enhance inhibitory control skills directly, it is possible that they inuence them indirectly by targeting self-control
and problem-solving skills.
4.2. Limitations
There are a number of limitations to the present study that should be considered wheninterpreting the results. First, due to the
correlational and cross-sectional nature of our analyses it is not possible to determine the directionality of the associations
between inhibitory control and children's socialemotional competence. A second limitation was the use of teacher ratings as the
only assessment of socialemotional competence. Although teachers are an excellent source of information about children's
behavior due to the extended periods of time they spend with them, biases may interfere with an accurate representation of their
socialemotional competence. Future studies should include multiple measures from multiple sources in order to more accurately
assess children's socialemotional competence.
A third concern regards the measurement issues that surround the assessment of inhibitory control and attention in young
children (Blair, Zelazo, & Greenberg, 2005). No single, pure measure of inhibitory control in young children currently exists. As
previously mentioned, PT and D/N areboth tasks that require an ability to hold information in mind and therefore reect children's
working memory abilities in addition to their inhibitory control abilities. Inhibitory control is only one component of the important
multi-dimensional construct of EF. Research has shown that other components of EF, including working memory and planning, are
also important in the prediction of children's behavior (Espy, McDiarmid, Cwik, Stalets, Hamby, & Senn, 2004; Hughes et al., 1998;
Hughes, Cutting, & Dunn, 2001; Moses, Carlson, & Sabbagh, 2005). Future studies should assess all aspects of EF to understand how
different components of EF jointly or uniquely predict children's behavior. Similarly, this study used the Leiter-R AS subtest as our
measure of attention. While it is a measure with strong psychometric properties, there are no studies that we are aware of that
compare the Leiter-R AS, which requires a motor response to other measures of attention that do not.
317B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
Finally, one must be cautious when interpreting the lack of signicant interaction effects as an indication that the relationship
between inhibitory control and socialemotional competence did not vary by maternal education or employment status or
children's sex, ethnicity, receptive vocabulary, emotional knowledge or attention skills. Although the interactions were entered one
at a time to limit the effect of power issues, the sample size in the present study may have limited the ability to detect signicant
interactions. Future studies should replicate these moderation analyses with larger sample sizes to eliminate lack of power as the
explanation for these ndings.
4.3. Contributions and future directions
The present study makes signicant contributions to the growing literature examining the relationship between
neurocognitive skills and socialemotional competencies in young children. First, the majority of past research examining the
link between inhibitory control and children's behavior has been conducted with relatively small, homogenous, middle-class
samples. However, research suggests that inhibitory control skills are especially important sources of resilience for children raised
in stressful environments (Blair, 2002). Therefore it is crucial to understand how inhibitory control operates in a more
disadvantaged sample like the one in the present study. Second, unlike other studies that ignore the nested structure of their data,
the present study used multi-level modeling in order to accurately account for classroom-level effects on individual-level relations
between inhibitory control and teacher-rated socialemotional competence. Finally, few studies have isolated the specic impact
of inhibitory control (in the context of a working memory demand) by covarying factors already known to be associated with
socialemotional competence. The present study makes novel contributions by showing how children's performance on inhibitory
control tasks are signicantly associated with teacher ratings of classroom behaviors in a relatively large sample of high risk,
ethnically diverse, low income preschoolers.
Despite the contributions made by the present study, there remain a number of areas of research that deserve furtherattention.
The present study provided evidence of the signicant role of inhibitory control to socialemotional competence, but future
research should focus on examining the relationship between inhibitory control and other important developmental tasks in early
childhood, including school readiness and academic achievement (Blair, 2002). Researchers have already begun to examine the
associations between executive functions and academic skills. For instance, Espy et al. (2004) found that both working memory
and inhibitory control contribute to the prediction of early math skills after controlling for child's age, maternal education and
receptive vocabulary. Presumably, the development of these early neurocognitive abilities will lay the groundwork for the future
self-regulatory and problem-solving skills needed to succeed both socially and academically throughout the life course.
References
Arsenio, W. F., Cooperman, S., & Lover, A. (2000). Affective predictors of preschoolers' aggression and peer acceptance: Direct and indirect effects. Developmental
Psychology,36, 438448.
Belsky, J., Friedman, S., & Hsieh, K. (20 01). Testing core emotion-regulation prediction: Does early attentional persistence moderate the effect of infant negative
emotionality on later development. Child Development,72,123133.
Berlin, L., & Bohlin, G. (2002). Response inhibition, hyperactivity and conduct problems among preschool children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,31, 242251.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness, integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American
Psychologist,57, 111127.
Blair, C. (2003). Behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in young children: Relations with self-regulation and adaptation to preschool in children attending
Head Start. Developmental Psychobiology,43,301311.
Blair, C., & Peters, R. (2003). Physiological and neurocognitive correlates of adaptive behavior in preschool among children in Head Start. Developmental
Neuropsychology,24,479497.
Blair, C., Zelazo, P. D., & Greeberg, M. T. (2005). Introduction to the special issue on the measurement of executive function in early childhood. Developmental
Neuropsychology,28,561571.
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology,53,371399.
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A., et al. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and
adolescent delinquency: A six site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology,39, 222245.
Brophy, M., Taylor, E., & Hughes, C. (2002). To go or not to go: Inhibitory control in Hard to managechildren. Infant and Child Development,11,125140.
Campbell, S. B., Pierce, E. W., March, C. L., Ewing, L. J., & Szumowski, E. K. (1994). Hard to manage preschool boys: Symptomatic behavior across context and time.
Child Development,65, 836851.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (20 02). How specic is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and
working memory. Infant and Child Development,11,7392.
Caspi, A., Moftt, T., Wright, B., & Silva, P. (1998). Early failure in the labor market: Childhood and adolescent predictors of unemployment in the transition to
adulthood. American Sociological Review,63, 424451.
Charman, T., Carroll, F., & Sturge, C. (2001). Theory of mind, executive function and social competence in boys with ADHD. Emotional and Behavioural Difculties,6,
3149.
Cole, P. M., Usher, B. A., & Cargo, A. P. (1993). Cognitive risk and its association with risk for disruptive behavior disorder in preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology,22,154164 .
Dempster, F. N., & Corkhill, A. J. (1999). Interference and inhibition in cognition and behavior: Unifying themes for education psychology. Educational Psychology
Review,11,188.
Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulderr, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., et al. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence.
Child Development,74, 238256.
Denham, S. A., & Weissberg, R. P. (2004). Socialemotional learning in early childhood: What we know and where to go from here.In E. Chesebrough, T. P.King, T. P.
Gullotta, & M. Bloom (Eds.), A blueprint for the promotion of prosocial behavior in early childhood (pp. 1350). New York: Kluwer/Academic Publishers.
Diamond, A. (1991). Frontal lobe involvement in cognitive changes during the rst year of life. In K. R. Gibson & A.C. Petersen (Eds.), Brain maturation and cognitive
development: Comparative and cross-cultural perspectives. Foundations of human behavior (pp. 127180). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy and biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R.T.
Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466503). New York: Oxford University Press.
318 B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
Diamond, A., Barnett, S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool improves cognitive control. Science,318(5855), 13871388.
Diamond, A., Kirkham, N., & Amso, D. (2002). Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Developmental
Psychology,38, 352362.
Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to do as I say, not as I do.
Developmental Psychobiology,29,315334.
Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (2007). Improving young children's social and emotional competence: A randomized trial of the Preschool PATHS
Curriculum. Journal of Primary Prevention,28,6791.
Domitrovich, C. E., Greenberg, M. T., Cortes, R., & Kusche, C. A. (2005). The preschool PATHS curriculum. South Deereld, MA: Channing-Bete.
Dowsett, S. M., & Livesey, D. J. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of executive skillstraining. Developmental
Psychobiology,36,161174.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised: Manual for forms L and M. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Nyman, M., Bernzweig, J., & Pinuelas, A. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to children's anger-related reactions. Child
Development,65,109128.
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children's
resiliency and adjustment. Child Development,75,2546.
Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. D., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in
preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology,26, 465486.
Fine, S. E., Izard, C. E., Mostow, A. J., Trentacosta, C. J., & Ackerman,B. P. (2003). First grade emotion knowledge as a predictor of fth grade self-reported internalizing
behaviors in children from economically disadvantaged families. Development and Psychopathology,15,331342.
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y., & Diamond, A. (1994).The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/27 years old on a Stroop-like daynight
test. Cognition,53,129153.
Greenberg, M. T., Riggs, N., & Blair, C. (2007). The role of preventive interventions in enhancing neurocognitive functioning and promoting competence in
adolescence. In E. F. Walker & D. Romer (Eds.), Adolescent psychopathology and the developing brain: Integrating brain and prevention science (pp. 4 41462). New
York: Oxford.
Greenberg, M. T., Weissberg, R. P., O'Brien, M. U., Zins, J. E., Fredericks, L., Resnik, H., et al. (2003). Enhancing school-based prevention through coordinated social,
emotional and academic learning. American Psychologist,58,466474.
Henry, B., Caspi, A., Moftt, T., & Silva, P. (1996). Temperamental and familial predictors of violent and nonviolent criminal convictions. Developmental Psychology,
32,614623.
Howse, R. B., Calkins, S. D., Anastopoulos, A. D., Keane, S. P.,& Shelton, T. L. (2003). Regulatory contributors to children's kindergarten achievement. Early Education
and Development,14,10111 9.
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,16, 233253.
Hughes, C., Cuttting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (2001). Acting nasty in the face of failure? Longitudinal observations of hard-to-managechildren playing a rigged
competitive game with a friend. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,29, 403416.
Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in hard-to-managepreschoolers.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,39, 981994.
Hughes, C., White, A., Sharpen, J., & Dunn, J. (20 00). Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: Hard-to-managepreschoolers' peer problems and possible cognitive
inuences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,41,169179.
Jimerson, S. R., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Carlson, B. (2000). A prospective longitudinal study of high school dropouts: Examining multiple predictors across
development. Journal of School Psychology,38, 525549.
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in childrenwith ADHDA
randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,44,177186.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the rst four years of life. Child Development,72, 10911111.
Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates and consequences. Journal of
Personality,71, 10871112.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social
development. Developmental Psychology,36, 220232.
Kopp, C. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology,25, 343354.
Kusche, C.A. (1984). The understanding of emotion concepts by deaf children: An assessment of an affective curriculum. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Washington, Seattle.
Kusche, C. A., Cook, E. T., & Greenberg, M. T. (1993). Neuropsychological and cognitive functioning in children with anxiety, externalizing and comorbid
psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,22,172195.
Lavigne, J. V., Gibbons, R. D., Christoffel, K. K., Arend, R., Rosenbaum, D., Binns, H., et al. (1995). Prevalence rates and correlates of psychiatric disorders among
preschool children. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,35,204214.
Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.
Lynch, K. B., Geller, S. R., & Schmidt, M. G. (2004). Multi-year evaluation of the effectiveness of a resilience-based prevention program for young children. The
Journal of Primary Prevention,24, 335353.
Malecki, C. K., & Elliott, S. N. (2002). Children's social behaviors as predictors of academic achievement: A longitudinal analysis. School Psychology Quarterly,17,
123.
Merrell, K. W. (1996). Socioemotional assessment in early childhood: The preschool and kindergarten behavior scales. Journal of Early Intervention,20,132145.
Moftt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review,100,674701.
Moftt, T. E., Caspi, A., Dickson, N., Silva, P., & Stanton, W. (1996). Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial conduct problems in males: Natural history
from ages 3 to 18 years. Development and Psychopathology,8, 399424.
Moses, L. J., Carlson, S. M., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2005). On the specicity of the relation between executive function and children's theories of mind. In W. Schneider, R.
Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children's cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability
and theory of mind (pp. 131145). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Olson, S. L. (1989). Assessment of impulsivity in preschoolers: Cross-measure convergences, longitudinal stability, and relevance to social competence. Journal of
Clinical Child Psychology,18,176183.
Olson, S. L., & Hoza, B. (1993). Preschool developmental antecedents of conduct problems in children beginning school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,22,
6067.
Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L., & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children:The role of
effortful control. Development and Psychopathology,17,2545.
Olson, S. L., Schilling, E. M., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Measurement of impulsivity: Construct coherence, longitudinal stability, and relationship with externalizing
problems in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,27,151165.
Pennington, B. F. (1997). Dimensions of executive functions in normal and abnormal development. In N. A. Krasnegar, G. R. Lyon & P.S. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.),
Development of the prefrontal cortex: Evolution, neurobiology and behavior (pp. 265281). Baltimore: Paulth Brookes Publishing.
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,3,337344.
Perner, J., Winfried, K., & Barchfeld, P. (2002). Executive control and higher-order theory of mind in children at risk of ADHD. Infant and Child Development,11,
141158.
Quay, H. C. (1997). Inhibition and attention decit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,25,713.
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (2004). Early learning and school readiness: Can early intervention make a difference. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,50,471491.
319B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
Raver,C. C., Blackburn, E. K., Bancroft, M., & Torp, N. (1999). Relations between effective emotional self-regulation, attentional control and low-income preschoolers'
social competence with peers. Early Education and Development,10, 333350.
Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Pentz, M. A. (2006). The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a socialemotional
prevention program in elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS Curriculum. Prevention Science,7,91102.
Robins, L. N., & Price, R. K. (1991). Adult disorders predicted by childhood conduct problems: Results from the NIMH epidemiological catchment area project.
Psychiatry,54,116132.
Roid, G., & Miller, L. (1997). Examiner's manual: Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co.
Rothbart, M. (1989). Temperament and development. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates & M. Rothbarts (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 187247). New York: Wiley.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,40,2139.
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3 Social, emotional and personality
development (pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.
Rubia, K., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., Brandeis, D., & Leeuwen, T. (1998). Inhibitory dysfunction in hyperactive boys. Behavioural Brain Research,94,2532.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic inuences on the development of executive
attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Scie nces,102,1493114936.
Rueda, R. M., Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental
Neuropsychology,28,573594.
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods,7,147177.
Schafer, J. L., & Olsen, M. K. (1998). Multiple imputation for multivariate missing data problems: A data analyst's perspective. Multivariate Behavioral Research,33,
545571.
Seguin, J. R. , & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Executive f unction in early aggression . In R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression
(pp. 307329). New York: Guilford Press.
Serna, L., Nielsen, E., Lambros, K., & Forness, S. (2000). Primary prevention with children at risk for emotional and behavioral disorders: Data on a universal
intervention for Head Start classrooms. Behavioral Disorders,26,7084.
Shields, A., Dickstein, S., Seifer, R., Giusti, L., Magee, K. D., & Spritz, B. (2001). Emotional competence and early school adjustment: A study of preschoolers at risk.
Early Education and Development,12,7396.
Songuga-Barke, E. J. S., Dalen, L., Daley, D., & Remmington, B. (2002). Are planning, working memory, and inhibition associated with individual differences in
preschool ADHD symptoms. Developmental Neuropsychology,,21, 255272.
Speltz, M. L., DeKlyen, M., Calderon, R., Greenberg, M. T., & Fisher, P. A. (1999). Neuropsychological characteristics and test behaviors of boys with early onset
conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,108,315325.
Spieker, S. J., Larson, N. C., Lewis, S. M., Keller, T. E., & Gilchrist, L. (1999). Developmental trajectories of disruptive behavior problems in preschool children of
adolescent mothers. Child Development,70,443458.
Tremblay, R. E., Phil, R. O., Vitaro, F., & Dobkin, P.L. (1994). Predicting early onset of male antisocial behavior from preschool behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry,
51,732739.
Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (2003). Treating conduct problems and strengthening social and emotional competence in young children: The Dina Dinosaur
Treatment Program. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders,11,130143.
Weikert, D. P., & Schweinhart, L. J. (1997). High/Scope Perry Preschool Program.In G. Albee & T.P. Gullotta (Eds.), Primary prevention works (pp.146166). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Welsh, M. C., & Pennington, B. F. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Development Neuropsychology,4,
199 230.
Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normal-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children.
Developmental Neuropsychology,7,131149 .
320 B.L. Rhoades et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30 (2009) 310320
... Among them, inhibitory control refers to the ability of young children to inhibit irrelevant stimuli, including emotional control, behavioral control, and temptation control (ElAdl, & Eissa, 2019). Rhoades et al. (2009) selected 146 young children (M age = 54 months; SD = 3.6 months) to explore the relationship between children's inhibitory control and their social and emotional abilities. The results showed that 4-5 year old children with better inhibitory control had higher emotional management ability and lower social behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
The impact of executive function on individual emotional competence directly may affects individual emotional competence. It may indirectly affects individual emotional competence through language. This study investigates the mediating effect of preschool children's language in the relationship between their executive function and emotional competence. A total of 210 preschool children from four kindergarten in Rafha city were randomly selected. The Pearson’s Moment Product Correlation Coefficient Technique was used to determine the relations between study variables. The mediating role of language in the relationship between Executive Function and Emotional Competence was examined with basic mediation analysis using Model 6 in PROCESS macro. The mediation effect results show that language plays a partial mediating role between executive function and the emotional competency of preschool children. The results of mediation analysis show that language plays a partial mediating role in the mediation model of "executive function-language-emotional competence".
... A growing body of evidence suggests that motor health behaviors not only contribute to improved physical health but can also have a significant, positive impact on children's psychology (Herring et al., 2019;Lewis et al., 2021). Social-emotional competence is a key developmental task in early childhood that significantly predicts educational and occupational achievement, health, and well-being (Rhoades et al., 2009;Schoon, 2021) and contributes to children's social success (Denham et al., 2001). The environment is an important contributor to children's motor health development (Kracht et al., 2021;Armstrong et al., 2019), and better living and learning environments give preschoolers a vital source of physical activity, leading to more physical and mental health benefits as well as better overall competence (Christian et al., 2022;Ng M et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background. Sports health and the future of preschool children are urgent problems. However, a comprehensive and scientific evaluation index system (EIS) for assessing their sports health is lacking. Objectives. The study aimed to elaborate an EIS for preschool children’s motor health based on interdisciplinary subjects to solve the growth dilemma faced by children during their development. Materials and methods. Sixteen interdisciplinary experts who have been engaged in sports and health education for more than 15 years participated in a Delphi study. They used a 5-point Likert scale twice to assess the importance of preset indicators and provided feedback to modify and allocate items to the EIS. Results. After two rounds of expert consultation, a consensus was reached on the EIS, which included 6 primary and 26 secondary indicators. The coefficients for experts’ familiarity (As), judgment (Ai), and authority (Aa) were all >0.7. The coefficients for opinion coordination (ω) were >0.7. The arithmetic mean (Mean) of the recognition and operability scores was ≧4, and the coefficients of variation (CV) were all <0.25. The harmonization coefficient (ω) for the primary indicators was 0.803, and the harmonization coefficient (ω) for the secondary indicators was 0.758 (all > 0.75). Conclusions. Accurate, reliable and scientific data were collected to develop an EIS in order to address the challenges of assessing children’s motor health. The findings can serve as a reference for future instruction on preschoolers’ motor health.
... Language skills are frequently found to be linked to internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children without NF1 [54]. Abundant evidence shows that language use or skills play an important role in regulating emotions and behaviors [55][56][57], which is then associated with the degree of internalizing and externalizing symptoms across developmental periods [58,59]. The inability to communicate efficiently and the associated low self-concept and poor social skills might also directly affect individuals' internalizing symptoms [55]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Individuals with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) frequently report psychosocial problems, among which internalizing and externalizing symptoms are the most poorly understood due to limited research and inconsistent evidence. This hinders the overall attendance of their psychosocial needs and has a major impact on their quality of life. Thus, this systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to synthesize existing findings on the degree to which individuals with NF1 experience internalizing and externalizing symptoms, compared with the unaffected population, and explore moderators of the group disparities. Methods Scopus, PsycINFO, Web of Science, PubMed, and ProQuest were searched from inception to March 26th, 2024, which identified 59 eligible studies (N of NF1 = 3182, mean ages 2.38 to 46.4 years). Hedges’ g was calculated for differences in internalizing and externalizing symptoms between the NF1 group and the unaffected controls. Study effect sizes were pooled using robust variance estimation and random-effects models. Moderators of group differences were tested using meta-regression. Results Random-effects meta-analyses indicated that compared with unaffected controls, individuals with NF1 showed more severe depressive (k = 21; g = 0.43; 95% CI [0.21, 0.65]), anxiety (k = 24; g = 0.27; 95% CI [0.01, 0.54]), somatic (k = 27; g = 0.56; 95% CI [0.30, 0.83]), total internalizing (k = 75; g = 0.50; 95% CI [0.33, 0.67]), aggression (k = 33; g = 0.33; 95% CI [0.08, 0.58]), delinquency, (k = 37; g = 0.43; 95% CI [0.26, 0.60]), and total externalizing symptoms (k = 47; g = 0.24; 95% CI [0.13, 0.35]). Studies that included more participants with NF1 who had ADHD or a lower verbal IQ reported greater group disparities in total internalizing symptoms or aggression. Conclusions Findings highlight the importance of promptly recognizing internalizing and externalizing symptoms in individuals with NF1 for timely interventions. Future research should identify predictors of internalizing and externalizing symptoms within the NF1 population to inform our knowledge and intervention development. Other implications for future research were also discussed. Systematic review registration The study protocol of this meta-analysis was registered at PROSPERO (CRD42023478258).
... Inhibitory control (IC) is defined as the ability to suppress an impulsive response in favour of one that is more situationally appropriate (Diamond, 2013). High levels of IC during early childhood are associated with better social competence (Rhoades et al., 2009), higher math and reading achievement (Whedon et al., 2020), and adaptive emotion regulation skills (Carlson & Wang, 2007). IC emerges in infancy and develops rapidly during early childhood under a biosocial lens (Olson et al., 2002), highlighting the importance of examining early life predictors of IC. ...
Article
Measures of parasympathetic regulation, such as respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), predict executive function outcomes, including inhibitory control, across childhood. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia augmentation tends to be associated with more maladaptive outcomes, compared to RSA suppression, but the literature regarding RSA profiles and inhibitory control development across infancy and early childhood is contradictory. The goal of our current study was to examine the longitudinal and interactive effects of infant RSA during a frustrating task with negative maternal characteristics on inhibitory control during early childhood. Participants included 410 children (209 girls, 77.6% White, 92.9% non‐Hispanic) and their mothers. With small to moderate effect sizes ( r ² range from 0.124 to 0.143), we demonstrated that maternal negative affect when children were 36 months old moderated the association between 10‐month‐old RSA during a frustrating task and 48‐month‐old inhibitory control, such that RSA suppression predicted higher levels of inhibitory control, but only when mothers exhibited high levels of negative affect. Our results highlight the biosocial framework under which child inhibitory control develops.
... Items capturing 15 primary emotions (i.e., angry, ashamed, confused, disappointed, embarrassed, excited, frustrated, happy, love, proud, sad, scared, surprised, tired, and worried) were retained in favor of those tapping five more ambiguous emotions (i.e., hate, fine, sure, lonely, and safe). These retained emotions are most frequently examined in research due to their basic and universally understood nature (Berzenski & Yates, 2017;Rhoades et al., 2009). Emotion recognition was assessed by asking the child to select a target emotion from four gendermatched line drawings of children expressing emotions (e.g., "Which boy/girl feels happy? ...
Article
Full-text available
Children’s emotion knowledge encompasses abilities to recognize and label emotions in the service of positive adaptation. Drawing on a sociodemographically diverse sample of 250 children (50% female sex assigned at birth; Mage_W1 = 49.02 months, SD = 2.99) and their maternal caregivers (55.6% Latina; 37.6% poverty), this study evaluated a multiple mediation model to integrate heretofore distinct bodies of research examining (a) parenting effects on the development of emotion knowledge and (b) emotion knowledge effects on socioemotional adaptation. Observations of maternal supportive presence at age 4 predicted increases in children’s emotion recognition and labeling from ages 4 to 8. However, only emotion labeling skills explained children’s behavioral adjustment outcomes with a significant pathway from supportive parenting at age 4 to fewer externalizing and internalizing behavior problems at age 10 via improved emotion labeling skills. These findings suggest that emotion knowledge, particularly labeling skills, partially explains the protective impact of supportive parenting on behavioral adaptation across childhood. Prevention and intervention efforts should target both supportive parenting practices and emotion knowledge skill development to support children’s socioemotional functioning and reduce behavior problems.
Article
Full-text available
Both successful academic performance and social functioning are correlated with social competence. On all teacher training programs, instruction in children's social development and social skills is not a must. However, recognizing social competency abilities and elements that support its development in kids might be helpful for pre-service teachers. Children who are socially competent are better able to form bonds with their peers, and strengthening these bonds can boost social competence, which in turn improves academic performance. According to the study, both positive and bad trends in a child's social and emotional development can be predicted by their teacher interactions. Children's social skills can be predicted by their educational environment, and direct instruction can predict skills and frequently enhance social skills in areas where children may be deficient. Early interactions between teachers and students are important for the development of healthy teenagers. The development of a positive teacher-student relationship is crucial for young children. Beginning on the first day of school, teachers can establish a positive rapport with the students by, for example, smiling, warmly welcoming them, speaking in a soft voice, and assisting them in feeling at ease in social situations. The setting of the classroom is crucial for learning. It has the potential to either engage kids and reduce problem behaviors, or it has the potential to cause chaos and increase the likelihood of such behaviors. The end result of "deep-level learning" is social competence. By fostering a learning environment where students can engage in experiences addressing capacities to understand feelings in both themselves and others, teachers can help children's basic social worldviews shift as they grow. The study concluded that the social competency of the instructors is the most crucial factor in establishing a communication between teacher and administrator, between teacher and teacher, between teacher and student, between teacher and pupil's between a parent, a teacher, and the community.
Article
This study investigated whether parental socialization of negative emotions moderated the relationship between adolescents’ low executive function or high impulsivity and their current or subsequent emotion dysregulation. Emotion dysregulation, characterized by difficulties in managing the intensity and duration of emotions, is a transdiagnostic factor linked to adverse outcomes. Youth with poor executive functioning and/or high impulsivity are at risk for emotion dysregulation; however, the role of parenting in influencing this trajectory warrants exploration. Self-report and parent-report measures were collected from 116 adolescents and their parents, assessing adolescent executive function (BRIEF), adolescent impulsivity (S-UPP-S), adolescents’ perceptions of their parent’s responses to negative emotions (CCNES-AP), and adolescent emotion regulation difficulties (DERS). Moderation analyses revealed that high levels of unsupportive parental responses significantly moderated the effect of high impulsivity on emotion dysregulation ( b = 10.27, p = .031) cross-sectionally.
Article
In the last few decades, there has been growing interest in the association between executive functions (EFs) and social cognition in the childhood years, but it is not fully understood what aspects of EF are linked to social cognition. Nor is the direction of these associations clear. This systematic review aimed to organize and clarify the existing knowledge about the links between EF and social cognition in typically developing children and provide directions for future research. Following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines for empirical studies (Moher et al., 2015), we identified 133 concurrent, longitudinal, and intervention studies (reported in 125 papers) that met our criteria and were published between 1995 and 2024. There were four main findings. First, the core EFs were correlated with both cognitive and affective social cognition. Second, most studies examined the associations between EF and theory of mind. Third, relativity few studies examined the association between hot EF and social cognition. Fourth, limited longitudinal and intervention research has been conducted in this field, and while work generally tends to support the impact of EF on social cognition, there are some contradictory findings on the temporal order of effects between these constructs.
Article
Full-text available
The day-night task requires saying “night” to a picture of the sun and “day” to a picture of the moon. In this investigation of why young children fail at this task, systematic variations of the task were administered to 96 children, half 4 years old and half 4½ years old. Training children on the strategy of chunking the 2 rules into 1 (“say the opposite”), thus reducing memory load, did not help their performance. What helped was reducing the inhibitory demand by instructing them to say “dog” and “pig” (not “night” and “day”) even though memory of 2 rules and inhibiting saying what the pictures represented were still required. Here the response to be activated and the response to be inhibited were unrelated. When the correct response was semantically related to, and the direct opposite of, the to-be-inhibited response, children performed poorly. Inserting a delay between stimulus and response helped even though that delay was filled with distraction. Young children apparently need several seconds to compute the answer on this task. Often they do not take the needed time; when forced to do so, they do well.
Article
Full-text available
The course, antecedents, and implications for social development of effortful control were examined in this comprehensive longitudinal study. Behavioral multitask batteries and parental ratings assessed effortful control at 22 and 33 months (N = 106). Effortful control functions encompassed delaying, slowing down motor activity, suppressing/initiating activity to signal, effortful attention, and lowering voice. Between 22 and 33 months, effortful control improved considerably, its coherence increased, it was stable, and it was higher for girls. Behavioral and parent-rated measures converged. Children's focused attention at 9 months, mothers' responsiveness at 22 months, and mothers' self-reported socialization level all predicted children's greater effortful control. Effortful control had implications for concurrent social development. Greater effortful control at 22 months was linked to more regulated anger, and at 33 months, to more regulated anger and joy and to stronger restraint.
Article
Full-text available
Observational assessments were made of 51 preschoolers' (mean age = 53.25 months) peer aggression and emotional displays outside of (baseline) and during aggressive interactions, and their emotion knowledge and peer acceptance were also assessed. Results indicated that the connections between children's affective dispositions and their aggression and peer acceptance varied as a function of both the emotion context (baseline vs. aggression related) and the particular emotion involved (happiness vs. anger). Emotion knowledge and affective dispositions overlapped little with each other, and both made independent contributions to peer acceptance and aggression. Mediation analyses revealed, however, that the significant connections between children's emotional dispositions and knowledge and their peer acceptance were mostly mediated by aggression.
Article
Full-text available
School-age children and adolescents with conduct problems typically exhibit deficits in verbal IQ, language abilities, and executive functions. This study examined the extent to which this pattern was evident in a clinic group of preschool boys with early onset conduct problems who met criteria for oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). A 2nd question focused on the strength of relation between clinic boys’ uncooperative or inattentive test behaviors and their test performance. As expected, the clinic boys showed a neuropsychological profile highly similar to the one found in older conduct problem populations. Verbal tests distinguished clinic from matched comparison boys even after controlling for observers’ ratings of disruptive behavior during testing. Clinic boys with ODD and ADHD had lower verbal and executive function scores than clinic boys with ODD alone. After general vocabulary knowledge and test behavior were controlled, clinic boys were found to have poorer vocabularies for describing affective states than comparison group boys.
Chapter
Full-text available
Chapter
Full-text available
Temperament refers to early-appearing dispositions upon which personality is based, and research on temperament offers an important place to begin in the study of human development. Rather than seeing individual differences solely as the result of socialization, temperament research identifies basic dispositions toward affect, arousal, and attention that will be further shaped by experience in development. In this article, temperament is defined, a brief history is presented, current research is reviewed, and directions for the future are outlined.
Article
Full-text available
New research on children's executive functioning and self-regulation has begun to reveal important connections to their developing social understanding (or “theories of mind”) and emotional competence. The exact nature of the relations between these aspects of children's social and emotional development is, however, far from being fully understood. Considerable disagreement has emerged, for instance, over the question of whether executive functioning facilitates social-emotional understanding, or vice versa. Recent studies linking the development of children's social understanding with aspects of their interpersonal relationships also raise concerns about the particular role that social interaction plays in the development of executive function. Three key questions currently drive this debate: Does social interaction play a role in the development of executive function or, more generally, self-regulation? If it does play a role, what forms of social interaction facilitate the development of executive function? Do different patterns of interpersonal experience differentially affect the development of self-regulation and social understanding? In this book, the contributors address these questions and explore other emerging theoretical and empirical links between self-regulation, social interaction, and children's psycho-social competence. It will be a valuable resource for student and professional researchers interested in executive function, emotion, and social development.