ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Although firms are faced by a large number of market introduction failures, research into a major driver of these failures, customer resistance to innovation, is surprisingly scarce. While most authors have investigated positive adoption decisions, this paper focuses instead on consumer resistance to innovation. The current study presents a conceptual framework which explicates the major components of consumer resistance: (1) rejection, (2) postponement, and (3) opposition, and discusses two main groups of antecedents to consumer resistance: (1) degree of change required and (2) conflicts with the consumer’s prior belief structure. This framework is explored with both a literature review and a qualitative focus group study. These joint efforts result in the formulation of a model of consumer resistance. Finally, the authors discuss several relevant theoretical and strategic implications, and point out directions for future research.
Content may be subject to copyright.
An exploration of consumer resistance to innovation and its antecedents
Mirella Kleijnen
a,*
, Nick Lee
b
, Martin Wetzels
c
a
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Faculty of Economics and Business Administration, Boelelaan 1105, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
b
Aston University, Birmingham B4 7ET, UK
c
P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Received 7 April 2008
Received in revised form 19 February 2009
Accepted 19 February 2009
Available online 9 March 2009
JEL classification:
M31
APA classification:
3920
PsycINFO classification:
1973
11500
11470
40647
Keywords:
Innovation
Consumer resistance
Focus groups
a b s t r a c t
Although firms are faced by a large number of market introduction failures, research into a
major driver of these failures, customer resistance to innovation, is surprisingly scarce.
While most authors have investigated positive adoption decisions, this paper focuses
instead on consumer resistance to innovation. The current study presents a conceptual
framework which explicates the major components of consumer resistance: (1) rejection,
(2) postponement, and (3) opposition, and discusses two main groups of antecedents to
consumer resistance: (1) degree of change required and (2) conflicts with the consumer’s
prior belief structure. This framework is explored with both a literature review and a qual-
itative focus group study. These joint efforts result in the formulation of a model of con-
sumer resistance. Finally, the authors discuss several relevant theoretical and strategic
implications, and point out directions for future research.
Ó2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Despite company efforts to adopt consumer-oriented innovation development processes focused on delivering added va-
lue to the consumer (Danneels, 2003), most commercial companies are faced with high rates of innovation failures (Moore,
2002). This is puzzling, as innovation adoption research has stressed that relative advantage is a dominant driver of con-
sumer adoption. Nevertheless, many innovations still meet resistance (Garcia & Atkin, 2002; Molesworth & Suortti, 2002).
The reasons for this resistance vary and have not as yet received a significant amount of study, but examples illustrate
the diversity of innovations which meet resistance. For example, consumers expressed moral objections against genetically
modified food, and actively campaigned against the introduction of these innovations (Bredahl, 2001; Fortin & Renton, 2003).
0167-4870/$ - see front matter Ó2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.02.004
*Corresponding author. Tel.: +31 20 5989861; fax: +31 20 5986005.
E-mail addresses: mkleijnen@feweb.vu.nl (M. Kleijnen), n.j.lee@aston.ac.uk (N. Lee), m.wetzels@mw.unimaas.nl (M. Wetzels).
Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Economic Psychology
j o u r n a l h o m e pa g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / j o e p
Consumer resistance also appears in the case of simpler innovations. For example, many wine drinkers steadfastly refused to
accept the screw cap as an acceptable replacement for the traditional cork on wine bottles (Garcia & Atkin, 2002).
While numerous studies explore factors that contribute to the ‘‘positive” decision to adopt such innovations (e.g., Lennon
et al., 2007; Lin, Hsin-Yu, & Sher, 2007), understanding why customers resist adoption is at least as important (Midgley &
Dowling, 1993; Szmigin & Foxall, 1998). While fundamental innovation research (Rogers, 2003) has always at least tacitly
recognized this importance, empirical research in this field has been less active in investigating the nature and drivers of
resistance compared to adoption. In particular, it is rarely the case that studies explicitly differentiate adoption from resis-
tance; instead implicitly considering resistance as simply non-adoption (Nabih, Bloem, & Poiesz, 1997). However, it is not
appropriate to conclude that consumer resistance is simply the obverse of adoption (Gatignon & Robertson, 1989; Herbig
& Day, 1992; Ram & Sheth, 1989).
The objective of this paper is to develop insight into this relatively underdeveloped area in the innovation literature. The
nature of the contribution provided is thus twofold. First, a detailed theoretical conceptualization of consumer resistance is
developed. Ram and Sheth (1989) have provided an initial investigation of this concept, where they recognize that consumer
resistance reveals itself in different forms of behavior, being rejection, postponement, and opposition, which appear similar
to the ideas put forward by Coetsee (1999). Nevertheless, as suggested by Nabih et al. (1997), these concepts tend to lack
standardized conceptual and operational definitions, and resistance itself appears to have been confused with the simple,
and more passive, notion of ‘non-adoption’ (Peñaloza & Price, 1993). Following from this, with few noteworthy exceptions
(e.g., Ram & Sheth, 1989), little research has been done on the antecedents which may create consumer resistance to inno-
vations (Lapointe & Rivard, 2005). This article reports the results of a combined theory-driven and theory-generating study
conducted according to the aforementioned objectives. First, an integrative overview of relevant literature is presented.
Additionally, the methods and results of a qualitative focus-group study are discussed. Finally, limitations and directions
for future research are detailed.
2. Literature review
While several authors have supported the notion of consumer resistance (Gatignon & Robertson, 1989; Ram, 1987; Sheth,
1981) and implicitly or explicitly acknowledged the importance of ‘negative’ or ‘anti’ consumption (e.g., Bredahl, 2001; Gar-
rett, 1987; Herrmann, 1993; Kozinets & Handelman, 1998; Saba, Rosati, & Vassallo, 2000), there is little attention devoted to
the thorough conceptualization of the concept of individual consumer resistance (Lapointe & Rivard, 2005; Peñaloza & Price,
1993). Moreover, that research which does exist is predominantly theoretical with little effort devoted to the empirical expli-
cation and validation of consumer resistance. Therefore, a more in-depth discussion of the concept of consumer resistance is
needed.
Innovation resistance is a response based on a conscious choice (Szmigin & Foxall, 1998), defined by Ram and Seth (1989,
p. 6) as ‘‘the resistance offered by consumers to an innovation, either because it poses potential changes from a satisfactory status
quo or because it conflicts with their belief structure.” Nevertheless, this broad definition of innovation resistance is not partic-
ularly illuminating, being as it essentially defines innovation resistance as ‘resistance to innovation’. One key issue of concern
is that resistance includes not trying the innovation (Nabih et al., 1997; Ram & Sheth, 1989; Szmigin & Foxall, 1998). This is
problematic because Rogers (2003) points out that initial objections toward an innovation can sometimes be overcome by
offering consumers the opportunity to try the innovation for a certain period of time. Moreover, existing literature (e.g., Ram
& Sheth, 1989; Szmigin & Foxall, 1998) suggests that innovation resistance can be further delineated from the basic ‘not-try-
ing’ of the innovation into three distinct types of consumer behavior: rejection, postponement, and opposition.
Rejection: Rejection is not driven by a simple lack of awareness or ignorance about the innovation on the consumer’s part.
Rather, this form of resistance implies an active evaluation on the part of the consumer, which results in a strong disincli-
nation to adopt the innovation (Rogers, 2003). Lee and Clark (1996–1997) recognize that this reluctance is often induced by a
suspicion of new and unproven innovations. In addition, Hirschheim and Newman (1988) contend that rejection is inter-
twined with an innate conservatism, i.e., a reluctance to change the status quo. An example of a food innovation that was
rejected by the American market is McDonalds’ ‘Arch Deluxe’ burger, which had the slogan the ‘‘Burger with the Grown-
up Taste”. While McDonalds positioned this new burger as a more sophisticated food product for adults, consumers did
not really consider McDonalds as a provider of sophistication, but of convenience (Haig, 2003).
Postponement: Although consumers find an innovation acceptable in principle, they may decide not to adopt it at that
point in time, for example until the circumstances are more suitable. In this case the decision is not final, and thus this def-
inition is similar to Greenleaf and Lehmann’s (1995) ‘‘delay”, as aform of consumer resistance. For example, many consum-
ers are waiting for voice over internet protocol (VoIP) technology to become more mainstream before considering adopting
this technology. Currently, software and hardware for VoIP is readily available (e.g., Skype) and is in fact very easy to use.
However, it is not regarded as a standard yet, and most consumers are still suspicious of what they regard as ‘‘unproven tech-
nology” (D’Errico, 2005).
Opposition: Consumers may be convinced that the innovation is unsuitable and decide to launch an attack – for example
negative word-of-mouth – against its launch. Davidson and Walley (1985) describe this as innovation sabotage, where con-
sumers actively engage in strategies to prevent the innovation’s success. Such forms of opposition, also referred to as ‘‘active
rebellion”, are most likely to affect market mechanisms (Fournier, 1998). There are ample examples of consumer innovations
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 345
that have met opposition. A recent example is called ‘‘The Golden Cage”, an innovative TV program that was broadcast on
Dutch television. This program showed a group of adults living in a house together, striving to be the last one left in the
house by pestering the other candidates out. The program met significant opposition from various schools, parents, and even
government spokespeople, that did not condone the promotion of pestering as a form of entertainment. This eventually led
to low ratings and the premature cancelation of the show (Dekker, 2007).
2.1. Drivers of consumer resistance
Gatignon and Robertson (1989) explicitly call for research that investigates consumer resistance to innovation as a spe-
cific form of behavior, conceptually separate from innovation adoption. Several researchers claim that the characteristics de-
fined in adoption research are not typically the factors which lead to active innovation resistance, and that resistance might
prevail despite the presence of many ‘adoption-related’ characteristics (Garcia & Atkin, 2002; Ram, 1987). However, other
researchers stress that studies on innovation characteristics such as Rogers’ diffusion theory may offer useful insights,
and should therefore not be completely ignored (Herbig & Day, 1992; Molesworth & Suortti, 2002; Ram, 1987). Conse-
quently, the current discussion complements such theoretical reflections on consumer resistance theory with Rogers’ per-
ceived innovation characteristics, in order to create a broader perspective on this important issue.
The factors that drive consumer resistance can be split into two main types (e.g., Gatignon & Robertson, 1989; Herbig &
Day, 1992; Martinko, Henry, & Zmud, 1996; Ram & Sheth, 1989). First, innovations which require a change in consumers’
established behavioral patterns, norms, habits and traditions are likely to be resisted. Second, innovations which in some
way cause a psychological conflict or problem for consumers are likely to be resisted. Similarly, Tornatzky and Klein
(1982) conclude that compatibility, defined as the degree to which an innovation is perceived as consistent with existing
values, habits and past experiences of the potential adopter, is one of the few factors of Rogers’ theory that consistently re-
lates to adoption. However, the operationalization of this variable is inconsistent across studies, including conflicts with val-
ues (e.g., Cho & Kim, 2001–2002), societal norms (e.g., Taylor & Todd, 1995), daily routines and habits (e.g., Tornatzky & Klein,
1982), and consumer lifestyle (e.g., Kleijnen, de Ruyter, & Wetzels, 2004). This variety has lead to confusion with regard to
the construct of compatibility, and a recent study by Karahanna, Agarwal, and Angst (2006) confirms the importance of
disaggregating the construct with regard to the specific compatibility issues that might arise. Thus, in line with theory on
consumer resistance, an explicit distinction between conflicts with traditions and norms, which relate to a societally-rele-
vant context, and conflicts with existing usage patterns, which refer to the personal routines and habits of individual con-
sumers, seems appropriate.
Considering traditions and norms, any behavior that is contrary to group norms, or societal and family values, creates a
barrier (Herbig & Day, 1992). As suggested by Bredahl (2001), innovations that can potentially transform future life tend
to generate high levels of social involvement. This does not only imply the disruption of values related to this particular inno-
vation, but also the extent to which the acceptance of such an innovation might lead to highly undesirable (unforeseeable)
consequences for society (Saba et al., 2000). For example, consumers may worry that by accepting genetically modified food
they are slowly adapting societal norms to become more open to genetic manipulation of the DNA of humans, an issue that
currently is considered unacceptable by society as a whole.
Secondly, several researchers (Foxall, 1993; Foxall, 1994; Ram, 1987; Sheth, 1981) argue that when consumers are sat-
isfied with their current situation, they have no desire or reason to change. Sheth (1981) points out that resistance is some-
times a consequence of habits. These habits are formed when a customer uses a product frequently over a long period of
time. Thus, it is not surprising that innovations which conflict with the usage patterns of competing and well-established
products (e.g., a piece of software not compatible with Microsoft Windows), or that contradict well-established workflows,
practices, or habits, will face resistance (Hurter & Rubenstein, 1978; Oreg, 2003). In this situation, other routine behaviors
must change before the innovation achieves acceptance.
In terms of psychological variables, theory suggests that the perceived product image of an innovation should have an
influence on resistance. Literature on innovation adoption has often implicitly conflated image with relative advantage (Rog-
ers, 2003). However, several researchers call for a more explicit investigation of this variable, and empirical studies support
such pleas (Chau, 1996; Kleijnen, de Ruyter, & Andreassen, 2005; Tornatzky & Klein, 1982). In particular, when studying
innovation resistance, image serves as an extrinsic cue that consumers use as a signal to base their decisions on. This is line
with Bearden and Shimp (1982), who argue that extrinsic product cues are important for consumers to assess new products.
As the actual product characteristics and functioning of an innovation may be hard to observe, the image is likely to be de-
rived from stereotypes, rumor or other indirect, non-experiential, sources (Ram & Sheth, 1989). Additionally, a certain iden-
tity might be obtained from the innovation’s origins, for example the product class or industry which it belongs to, or the
country of origin. Recent research has also suggested that negative media coverage can induce negative image perceptions
of innovations (Fortin & Renton, 2003) that lead to resistance (Ram &Sheth, 1989).
Innovation research points out complexity as another important impediment to adoption (Rogers, 2003; Tornatzky &
Klein, 1982). This complexity relates to the extent to which an innovation is difficult to use and understand. The cognitive
effort related to innovation adoption is increasingly receiving scholarly attention (e.g., Kleijnen, de Ruyter, & Wetzels, 2007),
and is stressed as a contributor to innovation resistance (Oreg, 2006; Ram, 1989). More particularly, information overload is
recognized as a factor which is growing in importance as choices of consumers are increasing. Information overload can be
defined as a response to the ever-increasing rate of information, knowledge, and innovations that emerge (Herbig & Kramer,
346 M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
1994). This makes it difficult for the consumer to organize and evaluate all the information and make comparisons between
the available alternatives (Herbig & Day, 1992; Hirschman, 1987). Similarly, Malhotra (1984) argues that consumers’ pro-
cessing capacity can become overloaded if they try to process too much information in a limited time, leading to dysfunc-
tional consequences such as cognitive strain (Herbig & Day, 1992). This is supported by Keller and Staelin (1987) who argue
that too much information damages the effectiveness of any single piece of information.
Resistance towards innovations is also influenced by consumers’ awareness of the perceived risk of adopting an innovation
(Shoemaker & Shoaf, 1975). Consumers often experience many uncertainties about the adoption of innovations, especially
with regard to performance (Garcia & Atkin, 2002), and consequently assume the likely outcome of innovation usage to
be negative (Martinko et al., 1996). It is consumers’ evaluation of the likelihood of these negative outcomes which consti-
tutes their perceived risk. Literature has defined several forms of risks, of which physical, economic, functional, and social
risk have been mentioned in relation to consumer resistance (Bredahl, 2001; Ram & Sheth, 1989; Saba et al., 2000). Physical
risk concerns consumer perceptions of the potential damage to persons or property which may be caused by the innovation
(Klerck & Sweeney, 2007). Economic risk is related to the cost (in a general sense) of an innovation. For example, Noussair,
Robin, and Ruffieux (2004) find that, contrary to popular belief, a substantial amount of their sample was willing to buy
genetically modified food if it was sufficiently inexpensive. Functional risk is concerned with uncertainty about performance
of the innovation. Finally, social risk refers to whether or not consumers feel that their social environment (e.g., reference
groups) will accept or support their adoption. The latter is strongly related to observability as defined by Rogers (2003). Peer
observation is an important factor in the consumer decision process, and lack of social support could potentially cause users
of a socially-unaccepted innovation to isolate themselves from their social system.
Despite the previous work on the different types of consumer resistance and its many antecedents, there is a dearth of
empirical evidence regarding whether different forms of resistance have distinct antecedents. Table 1 summarizes the schol-
arly research on consumer resistance. The table illustrates that empirical evidence specifically focusing on resistance is scat-
tered, and that a systematic approach to investigating the differences in drivers of various resistance forms is lacking.
Particularly conspicuous is the absence of evidence regarding opposition. This is striking, as an initial investigation of the
literature seemed to reveal a plethora of studies on outright opposition and consumer actions against innovations. However,
this type of research has mostly focused on collective actions such as boycotts and protests, whereby the antecedents are
often outcome related, such as the expected success of the action itself (e.g., Garrett, 1987; Sen, Gurhan-Canli, & Morwitz,
2001). Nonetheless, antecedents related to the innovation itself have received little study.
Despite the lack of empirical attention, there is some anecdotal evidence to suggest that it should not be assumed that
each antecedent has an equal impact on consumer resistance, either as a whole or on each separate type. For example, Szm-
igin and Foxall (1998) suggest that postponement is more likely to be driven primarily by situational factors. These patterns
are often witnessed with technology-related innovations, where consumers often worry that by investing too early in such
innovations, they are at risk of rapid introductions of new and improved versions leaving them with obsolete equipment
(Dhebar, 1996). This was exactly the situation in the 1970s, when early buyers of Sony Betamax found themselves in a quan-
dary when VHS entered the market and quickly became dominant. Additionally, consumers often postpone their innovation
decisions until they have been able to process more information about the innovation (Szmigin & Foxall, 1998). Rejection
however seems be driven by inertia to a large degree, implying that usage patterns might be more influential for this form
of resistance (Ganiere, Chern, Hahn, & Chiang, 2004). Woodside and Biemans (2005) add that rejecters favor the status quo
because they are highly concerned about financial, performance and personal risks, and are therefore more comfortable with
sticking with what they know best. Finally, opposition seems to be strongly driven by tradition and norms. A contemporary
example is RFID (radio frequency identification) technology. Consumers are concerned about the impact of this technology
on society and believe that the constant monitoring of their behavior is in conflict with societal ethics (Thiesse, 2007). While
these are incidental cases, they do suggest the presence of differences in the drivers underlying the different forms of resis-
tances. In turn this illuminates a number of quandaries in our present understanding of the underlying forms of consumer
resistance to innovations, as well as their antecedents. Hence, a more in-depth perspective on the drivers of the various resis-
tance types is called for. We considered that a qualitative research method was ideally suited to this task.
3. Research method
Given the need for more detailed insights into both consumer resistance itself as well as its antecedents, a qualitative
focus-group study was conducted. Focus groups are particularly appropriate for our purposes, since they allow insight into
the social aspects of innovation resistance, which previous research has argued may be a crucial factor. This technique af-
fords us the opportunity to observe the collective sense-making of individuals with regard to innovation resistance decisions
(such as participants probing and challenging others’ opinions), and also to generate a wider range of views, which is impor-
tant for theory-generation (Bryman, 2004). The research objectives are clearly subsumed within Fern’s (2001) theory appli-
cations approach, by using focus groups to develop and (in part) confirm theory. Eight focus group discussions (three each on
the subject of ‘rejection’ and ‘opposition’, and two for ‘postponement’) were conducted, a number broadly consistent with
similar research across various fields (e.g., Morgan & Spanish, 1985; Pavia & Costa, 1993).
About 58 subjects (34 males and 24 females) were approached on the streets of the central shopping precinct within a
major city in the Netherlands according to the criteria that they appeared to be below 30, and to be active consumers. As
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 347
Table 1
Literature overview: drivers of consumer resistance.
Driver Description Source Nature of the study Relevant findings
Traditions and
norms
Inherited body of customs and
beliefs within a relevant social
context
Herbig and Day
(1992)
Conceptual Suggest that culture and society create so-
called diffusion thresholds, when
innovations go beyond this threshold they
will be resisted
Hirschman (1987) Conceptual Suggests that innovations that are closer to
traditional norms are more acceptable, where
as innovations that deviate from
these ideas (i.e., innovative day care centres)
are resisted at first
Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
societal disapproval and resistance. Expected
to be particularly present for eating customs,
as they are strongly culturally embedded
Existing usage
patterns
Habitual behavior formed when
using a product frequently over a
long period of time
Foxall (1993) Quantitative – survey Finds that adaptors, who have a high level of
commitment to a coherent behavior pattern,
are more likely to resist food innovations
Herbig and Day
(1992)
Conceptual Suggest that incompatibility of the
innovation with existing workflows
creates a consumer acceptance barrier
Oreg (2003) Quantitative – survey Finds that routine seeking is an important
contributor to resistance to change in a
variety of contexts
Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
changes in daily routine and resistance
Szmigin and Foxall
(1998)
Qualitative – interviews Consumers reject innovations that are
offered via a medium that is incompatible
with their existing usage patterns
Perceived image Unique set of associations within
the minds of customers, based
on e.g., the product category the
innovation belongs to, the
manufacturer that produces it,
or the country where it is
produced
Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
unfavorable image and resistance.
Molesworth and
Suortti (2002)
Qualitative – interviews Finds a negative relationship between
unfavorable image and resistance
Information
overload
A user’s response to the ever-
increasing rate of information,
knowledge, and innovations
Herbig and Day
(1992)
Conceptual Suggest that when consumers are overwhel-
med with information and cannot accurately
compare existing alternatives to the
innovation, this creates a situation where he/
she becomes impervious to further change
Herbig and
Kramer (1994)
Conceptual Suggests that information overload can have
an adverse effect on consumer decision
making when this overload exceeds
consumers’ processing capabilities
Hirschman (1987) Conceptual Suggests that dealing with overwhelming
amounts of information with regard to
innovations and alternatives complicates
decision making and generally leads to
resistance toward novelty
Oreg (2006) Quantitative – survey Finds that more information has a negative
impact on resistance
Physical risk Concern that the innovation
might be harmful, unhealthy of
cause injury
Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
physical risk and resistance. Fear for
innovations is especially recognized for
chemical products and processed foods
Bredahl (2001) Quantitative – survey Perceived risks with regard to harmful, health
related effects of genetically modified food
negatively affect consumer evaluations of
these food innovations
Saba et al. (2000) Quantitative – survey Perceived risks with regard to harmful, health
related effects of genetically modified food
negatively affects consumer behavior with
regard to these food innovations
Ganiere et al.
(2004)
Quantitative – survey Perceived risks with regard to harmful, health
related effects of genetically modified food
causes consumers to oppose these innovations
348 M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
younger consumers are more likely to have experience with innovative products (Parasuraman, 2000), this demographic
group is the primary focus. Specifically, studying consumers who are more open to innovation adoption increases the
chances that they have considered adopting innovations which they decided against, which was important to the study. Con-
sequently, subjects were asked a screening question to ensure that they had in the recent past made a conscious decision not
to adopt a new product or service. Those who passed through this screening (approximately 90% of those who had agreed to
be questioned) were then allocated into focus groups specific to the three different forms of resistance. Each focus group con-
sisted of between 6 and 9 individuals (Fern, 2001, p. 161). However, actual group size on the day varied due to practical con-
straints such as respondent availability. Table 2 shows the demographic characteristics of each focus group.
All focus groups lasted between 1.5 and 2 h. Data were collected through written notes and audio tape recording of all
groups. Each group was moderated by a single member of the research team, and observed by another, who provided a use-
ful cross-check and additional note-taking facility. Each focus group dealt with a single type of resistance as defined earlier,
allowing the depth necessary to develop theory. The groups began with a friendly welcome and explanation, followed by
various exercises and tasks to generate the type of permissive and non-judgmental environment necessary for unguarded
disclosure (Krueger, 1988). The moderator began by collaboratively discussing the ‘type’ of resistance that the focus group
was to explore (either rejection, postponement, or opposition), both to investigate the content of that form, and also to en-
sure that participants should only discuss resistance activities that fitted that definition. Subsequently, participants were
asked to explain a situation where they recently resisted an innovative product, which was used as a starting point for
Table 1 (continued)
Driver Description Source Nature of the study Relevant findings
Economic risk Concern that the innovation will
be a waste of economic
resources
Dhebar (1996) Conceptual Suggest that particularly high-tech
innovations often require high investments
which makes consumers worry reluctant to
spend such amounts of money as they
worried about how well spend this money
really is on a long-term basis
Noussair et al.
(2004)
Quantitative – experiment Find that consumers’ reluctance to buy
genetically modified food decreases when
price decreases
Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
economical risk and resistance. This type of
risk is expected to be especially relevant for
technology-related innovation. Consumers
will speculate about future lower prices,
which lead to postponement
Szmigin and Foxall
(1998)
Qualitative – interviews Consumers postpone adoption until they feel
they can afford the innovation
Woodside and
Biemans (2005)
Conceptual Suggest that financial risk leads to rejection.
Functional risk Performance uncertainty Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
performance risk and resistance
Szmigin and Foxall
(1998)
Qualitative – interviews Consumers postpone adoption until they are
assured that the innovation functions
properly
Woodside and
Biemans (2005)
Conceptual Suggest that functional risk leads to
rejection
Social risk Concern that the purchase of the
innovation will not be approved
of by relevant others (direct
social circle)
Ram and Sheth
(1989)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
social risk and resistance in various contexts
Fain and Roberts
(1997)
Conceptual Suggests a negative relationship between
social risk and resistance, particularly for
high-tech innovations
Table 2
Focus group descriptives.
Group no. and topic Males Females Ages Average age
1. Rejection 3 4 25, 21, 22, 23, 20, 21, 23 22.1
2. Rejection 6 2 19, 23, 22, 21, 20, 21, 19, 23 21
3. Rejection 6 3 21, 20, 25, 21, 25, 22, 19, 24, 24 22.3
4. Postponement 2 4 19, 22, 21, 24, 23, 23 22
5. Postponement 5 2 26, 23, 24, 20, 22, 21, 22 22.6
6. Opposition 4 4 25, 20, 21, 25, 26, 21, 22, 25 23.1
7. Opposition 4 2 25, 22, 23, 20, 21, 19 21.7
8. Opposition 4 3 24, 23, 20, 21, 19, 23, 25 22.1
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 349
the rest of the discussion. The moderator used a discussion guide, which was mainly to structure the topics to cover (e.g.
defining resistance, generating and discussing different drivers, etc.), rather than specifically ‘forcing’ respondents to discuss
drivers which had emerged from prior theory. In this way, the structure of the discussion was kept fluid, and participants
were able to direct the conversation along their own lines (critical to later analysis). During the interaction, the moderator
was extremely careful to encourage views of all different types, and also to probe carefully into respondents’ opinions, atti-
tudes and their associated reasoning and motivation. In doing so it was hoped that any major problems with respondent
‘impression management’ could be avoided or at least recognized and incorporated into the analysis.
In keeping with the mainly exploratory goals of the research, little direction was pre-specified for each group. Participants
were first taken through a brainstorming exercise where they generated examples of innovative products that they had re-
sisted in the recent past. This exercise was used to help participants generate ideas through a process of ‘snowballing’ and
comparison. While there were many different specific innovations named, for each focus group the examples generated by
participants tended to fall into three different product categories; food, domestic appliances, and electronics (although a very
small minority fell outside these categories). The moderator then used the examples to drive the group discussion. Further-
more, as the participants themselves defined the products to discuss at this early stage, the discussion was strongly centred
on those products and categories. The overall design therefore allowed a good amount of consistency in terms of the overall
product categories discussed over the eight groups, although for each group the specific examples were different.
The data were analyzed using a combination of inductive and theory-based approaches. Specifically, literature-based re-
search had suggested a set of concepts which seemed likely to have an influence on resistance. However, particular care was
taken to avoid prejudging either the non-existence of any additional concepts, or the specific relationships of any concept to
the various types of resistance. In specific terms, the analysis was structured by a process of meaning condensation and cat-
egorization, driven by a hermeneutic process of iterative overall reading and detailed coding and analysis (e.g., Holbrook &
O’Shaughnessy, 1988). Thus, the focus groups transcripts were treated as ‘documents’, and an initial protocol for coding and
analysis of the full data set was developed through theoretical research and initial holistic readings of a subset of the data.
This follows the qualitative document analysis approach of Altheide (1996), as well as in part the approaches taken to inter-
view analysis in previous economic psychology research (e.g., Dittmar & Drury, 2000; Luomala & Laaksonen, 1999). The pro-
tocol was then subject to examination by a team of three academic experts who were not part of the author group.
The detailed coding process used a content analysis method which drew from both qualitative media analysis (e.g., Althe-
ide, 1996), and the traditional content analysis approach more familiar to many researchers (e.g., Kassarjian, 1977; Kolbe &
Burnett, 1991). The unit of analysis was the theme, or ‘‘single assertion about a subject” (Kassarjian, 1977, p. 10), rather than
a simple count of specific words or phrases. Statements by individual respondents were broken into discrete themes before
categorization (Kassarjian, 1977), and these utterances were categorized according to their closeness to the various catego-
ries in the protocol (Altheide, 1996). To enhance objectivity a multi-researcher strategy was used. Specifically, a researcher
not part of the author group was trained in the use of the protocol, and then conducted the content analysis. The coding of
the entire data set was then cross-validated by two independent coders, also not part of the author group. Rust and Cooil’s
(1994) proportional reduction in loss (PRL) measure was utilized to assess reliability. There were a total number of 1925
units extracted, each of which could be coded into one of eight possible categories, and three independent judges. The pro-
portion of coding agreement between the three judges over the data set was 0.95. Using tables provided by Rust and Cooil
(1994) this gives a PRL reliability of 100, which is the maximum possible. However, Rust and Cooil (1994) only provide tables
for up to five categories. Even so, Perreault and Leigh (1989, pp. 141–142) argue that a greater number of categories will lead
to higher reliability, and explicitly show how their measure of reliability exhibits this behavior up to nine categories. As the
Perreault and Leigh (1989) measure of reliability is simply a special case of the PRL measure (Rust & Cooil, 1994, p. 6), the PRL
of 100 reported here can be taken as a useful indication of reliability.
4. Results
In looking at the results as a whole, some interesting patterns became clear. Even though the moderator made strenuous
efforts to let the conversation evolve naturally concerning the causes of resistance, virtually none of the data provided from
the focus groups fell outside the general scope of antecedents which had been previously discussed in the literature. This is
surprising, since most of the existing work on the antecedents to resistance is purely theoretical, with little empirical inves-
tigation (e.g., Bagozzi & Lee, 1999; Peñaloza & Price, 1993; Ram, 1987; Ram & Sheth, 1989). However, the primary objective
was concerned with the potential variation in importance of these different antecedents, and, in this area, the findings pro-
vide a considerable contribution. Most strikingly, under 2% of all the quotes from all the groups related to the ‘information
overload’ antecedent, leading to the removal of information overload from the set of antecedents to resistance. This obser-
vation runs counter to existing theoretical work, which conceptualizes overload as an important antecedent to resistance. In
particular, we expected postponement to be influenced by information overload (Mitchell & Papavassiliou, 1997; Szmigin &
Foxall, 1998). However, the respondents in our research did not perceive information overload as an important issue. By con-
trast, the other antecedents were all discussed at length in the focus groups. Product usage patterns were the subject of 24%
of quotes, perceived image 21%, and traditions/norms 12%. By far the most common antecedent discussed was risk, with 41%
of quotes relating to this antecedent. The most commonly-mentioned type of risk was functional, with economic risk also
popular. By contrast, physical and social risks were discussed only sporadically. Moreover, there were interesting patterns
350 M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
evident in the different types of antecedents which were relevant to the three different resistance types. Fig. 1 provides a
graphical representation of the focus group findings, and how the different antecedents were related to the three different
types of resistance.
Of particular interest in Fig. 1 is the pattern of relevance of the antecedents to each type of resistance. In order to gain a
basic idea of how relevant each antecedent was to the different resistance types, three different ways at the comparative
percentage of times each antecedent was mentioned were utilized (see Fern, 2001). The first comparison has already been
discussed, between the different antecedents across all focus groups (the bold numbers in Fig. 1). Subsequently, two other
comparisons were of interest. First was the relative percentage of times each antecedent was mentioned within the focus
groups regarding a single resistance type (the italicized numbers in Fig. 1). For example, out of all the times antecedents were
mentioned in the postponement groups, 42% were concerning usage patterns. Secondly, the underlined numbers in Fig. 1
represent the percentage of the comments about an antecedent across all focus groups, which occurred in relation to each
type of resistance (e.g., out of all the times physical risk was mentioned, 79% were in the opposition groups). For each type of
resistance, an antecedent was considered relevant if this number was large (over 30%). These three different proportions are
considered to be valid indications of the relative importance of the antecedents for a number of reasons. Firstly, the moder-
ator was conscious of letting each group develop naturally, rather than forcing groups to discuss all antecedents. Thus, the
amount of times each antecedent was discussed should not be biased by the moderator, and instead should indicate the
importance of that antecedent to the respondents. Secondly, while group size varied between 6 and 9, the average size of
the groups for each type of resistance is very similar (8 for rejection, 7 for opposition, and 6.5 for postponement). Therefore,
the numbers should be good indications of the relative importance of the antecedents in general (in bold), and within each
type of resistance (in italic). However, the decision to only use two postponement groups may have influenced the relative
frequency across resistance forms for each antecedent (the underlined numbers in Fig. 1). This is a possibility, but it is un-
likely to have made a major difference to the current findings, as it is only liable to have increased the relative frequencies for
each antecedent within the postponement groups by a small amount. For example considering ‘social risk’, the underlined
numbers show that over 90% of the times it was mentioned occurred in the rejection and opposition groups. The situation is
similar for the other antecedents which currently are not considered relevant to postponement. Thus, there is little scope for
a significant change in proportions (i.e., large enough to influence the current findings) to occur with an extra postponement
group, especially given the cutoff of 30% relative frequency.
This emphasis on the relative frequency of categories is consistent with a thematic content analysis method, as it is the
patterns in the data rather than the absolute numbers of quotes which provide content analysis with its meaning (see Franz-
osi, 2004). However, it is important to reiterate that the division of large sentences into theme units, and the allocation of
these units into antecedent categories was based on a rigorous qualitative content analysis methodology (detailed earlier),
rather than simplistic criteria such as whether a unit included a specific word (such as ‘risk’). Thus, the numerical data in
Fig. 1 should be interpreted as another way of shedding light on the complex qualitative picture of consumer resistance,
and is only truly insightful in combination with the discussion of the data.
Looking at the emergent patterns (Fig. 1), the data seems to suggest a somewhat hierarchical pattern among the three dif-
ferent types of resistance, moving from postponement, to rejection, to opposition, depending on both the amount and type of
antecedents present. More specifically, if an innovation is likely to change existing usage patterns and has an economic risk,
(9) (79)
Bold numbers refer to the percentage per antecedent of the total number of quotes for all focus groups
Italicized numbers refer to the percentage of quotes within each set of focus groups (e.g. the rejection groups)
Underlined numbers refer to the
p
ercenta
g
e of the total number of
q
uotes re
g
ardin
g
that antecedent within all focus
g
rou
p
s (the bold number)
Physical (4)
Economic (14)
Functional (19)
Social (4)
Risk
(41)
Traditions and
Norms (12)
Usage Patterns
(24)
Perceived Image
(21)
Postponement
Opposition
(30) (45)
Rejection
(18) (57)
(22) (44)
(11) (32)
(20) (43)
(24) (41)
(26) (48)
Resistance Hierarchy
(5) (48)
(42) (39)
(5) (46)
(25) (45)
Fig. 1. The resistance hierarchy.
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 351
then consumers are likely to resist by postponing adoption (the weakest form of resistance). However, when these two fac-
tors are combined with a functional risk, a social risk, and a poor image, consumers will resist by rejecting that innovation.
Finally, when functional and social risks are combined with poor perceived image, a conflict with existing traditions and
norms, and the perceived risk of physical harm, consumer resistance is likely to be expressed by active opposition (the stron-
gest form of resistance).
The hierarchical pattern suggests that not only do the resistance forms differ in terms of the number of antecedents, but
also in their general nature. Comparing postponement to rejection, the emphasis seems to move from more basic, practical
concerns in the case of postponement, to more societal concerns such as tradition and norms where rejection is concerned.
Such issues become even more prevalent, and also include physical harm, as the resistance form becomes more pro-active.
This is in line with ideas presented by Peñaloza and Price (1993). It seems that opposition to innovation is somehow bound
up with the idea of the ‘‘citizen” within the consumer, where consumers feel the need to be pro-active to the extent that they
feel their society and associated values and norms are threatened. Research has documented such demonstrations of system-
atic action of the market against particular innovations (Moisio & Askegaard, 2002; Ritson & Dobscha, 1999). Conversely,
innovations that are met by postponement appear to face some less fundamental concerns and confronted with barriers that
– in part at least – might be less difficult to overcome for the individual consumer or the marketer (Szmigin & Foxall, 1998).
In the following sections, the individual results for each concept in the model are discussed.
Postponement. Beginning with postponement, findings from the groups which focused on this type of resistance suggested
that there were two main factors that – if present – would lead consumers to postpone rather than adopt an innovation. After
discussion, the definition of postponement itself was agreed to concern an active decision to not adopt an innovation at that
moment in time. This decision seemed to be most influenced by the risk consumers saw in the adoption of the product. How-
ever, unlike the other types of resistance discussed below, by far the most influential type of risk was economic. One key
component of this risk was concerned with the fact that consumers felt that in their current situation they were unable
to afford an innovation, such as ‘‘I can not afford it because I do not have a job right now”, and ‘‘I first need to have the pos-
sibility to afford one”. However, consumers also referred commonly to the increased long-term expenditure of buying or
using an innovation, for example: ‘‘It is more expensive per brush as you will use more toothpaste on it”, or ‘‘I will not
pay for a newspaper which I could get for free”. A similar but distinct set of comments referred to an expectation that price
may come down in the future, such as ‘‘it has not come on special offer yet” and ‘‘a DVD-recorder is too expensive yet”. It is
important to note however that economic risk as conceptualized here is not only to do with the ability to afford the inno-
vation now or in the future, which can be seen in comments such as ‘‘I have the money, but I’m not prepared to spend it on
this product”. Rather economic risk also concerns the issue of whether or not purchasing the innovation is really a good
investment in the long run. These concerns are illustrated by quotes such as, ‘‘I’m not sure what is still coming, so it might
be a waste of money”, and ‘‘it’s nonsense to spend money on this”.
A conflict with existing usage patterns was the other primary driver of postponement, with 42% of quotes in the post-
ponement focus groups referring to this (nearly 40% of all quotes regarding usage patterns in the entire sample). Many of
these comments referred to how existing patterns conflicted with an innovation, such as ‘‘if I had a car, I would buy one”,
‘‘I will wait for it to be integrated into the mobile phone”, and ‘‘as a student, you are not really a fanatical ironer”. Essentially,
consumers seemed to be unwilling to break with their existing routines to use an innovation.
Rejection. Moving to rejection, findings from the three groups which concentrated on this type of resistance indicate that a
number of key characteristics can result in outright rejection of an innovation rather than postponement. All focus groups
agreed that ‘rejection’ referred to the active decision to not at all take up an innovation which had been introduced to market.
Like postponement, economic risk and conflict with existing usage patterns played a prominent role in consumers’ decision
to reject an innovation. Comments here were similar to those for postponement above, with typical comments relating to
economic risk being ‘‘you must buy expensive throw-away mops to use it”, while conflicts with existing usage patterns were
exemplified by comments such as ‘‘when I need the internet, I use my laptop at home” Thus, it can be seen that there were
overlaps with the kinds of opinions raised in the focus groups regarding postponement.
It seems that the difference between the postponement of innovation adoption, and the complete rejection of it, lies in the
additional factors which were raised by the respondents in these groups. The level of risk was a key factor. Economic risk has
already been mentioned, but rejection was distinguished from postponement by the prominence of other types of risk in the
consumers’ mind. Of major importance was functional risk. Over half the quotes concerning risk referred to functional risk,
and typical comments about various innovations were; ‘‘[it’s] quasi-cleaning”, and ‘‘the jam tastes like plastic!” Consumers
thus appear to reject innovations which they feel just do not work the way they should. Fewer comments were evident con-
cerning the other types of risk, but it is of particular note that social risks appear to be a strong antecedent to rejection. While
only a small percentage of all comments in the 8 groups concerned social risks, 48% of them occurred in the rejection groups.
The comments regarding social risk were also very evocative, such as ‘‘it’s a little bit weird to order” and ‘‘if I did decide to
use it, I would do it in a corner where nobody could see me”.
Similarly, the perceived image of the innovation was a commonly-discussed antecedent to rejection, with 26% of quotes
referring to it. These quotes as expected referred in general to negative extrinsic cues such as ‘‘reminds me of ugly mobile
phones”, and ‘‘everybody has one, it is not special anymore”. Furthermore, many of these comments referred implicitly to
negative stereotypes such as; ‘‘only for kids”, ‘‘home cleaning products are just not cool”, and ‘‘in your unconscious, it has
to do with something you do on the toilet each day”. Cues related to the product’s origins were also common, such as ‘‘it
is just not a producer that should sell bread”, and ‘‘it should not be in a bottle”.
352 M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
Opposition. Opposition – the strongest form of resistance – was agreed upon in the focus groups to refer to actual active
behavior directed in some way towards opposing the introduction of an innovation. Such behaviors ranged from complaint let-
ters, negative word of mouth, online activities, through to taking protest action against the introduction of a product (such as
genetically-modified crops). There are some clear differences in the pattern of antecedents to opposition to the other types of
resistance. Firstly, none of the antecedents to opposition are shared with postponement, which suggests that even innova-
tions which are considered to be low in economic risk, and to fit well with existing routines, may still be actively opposed by
consumers.
Opposition does however share a number of antecedents with rejection. Like rejection, functional and social risk are of
importance, and in fact both forms of resistance account for similar proportions of comments here. The content of the com-
ments was broadly similar to those in the rejection groups, such as ‘‘it is too big, it takes up a lot of room” for functional risk,
and ‘‘you would look like a wally” for social risk. Perceived image was also a key antecedent to opposition, seemingly with
about the same importance as to rejection. Yet here, comments relating to perceived image seemed a lot stronger than those
occurring in the rejection groups, with things such as ‘‘[the packaging] is probably meant to mislead children”, and ‘‘it doesn’t
matter what type it is, Coke makes you fat” being typical. These quotes clearly illustrate the signaling function of the inno-
vation’s image.
Apart from the increased strength of feeling evident in the comments regarding perceived image, the difference between
simply rejecting and actively opposing an innovation may be driven by two main factors which are present in addition to
those previously mentioned. Firstly is the level of physical risk which the consumer feels is inherent to the innovation. While
physical risk was mentioned very infrequently in general, the three groups focused on opposition contained almost every
occasion of it being mentioned. Participants were also very evocative in their comments, such as ‘‘Dangerous! If you get
in an accident on the highway with it, you are dead”, ‘‘I always think there is mold and little pieces in it”, and ‘‘this thing
does not protect your eyes”. Second and more commonly mentioned was a conflict with the existing traditions and norms
of society. Comments regarding this were exemplified by such quotes as; ‘‘Dutch people prefer to pay with coins”, ‘‘vegeta-
bles do not belong in a bottle” and ‘‘milk is milk and nothing else”. Perhaps the most resonant example of this antecedent is
the comment by one consumer that ‘‘I am against it because of my principles”.
5. Discussion and implications
The conceptualization presented in this study has aspects of a ‘hierarchical’ pattern, as the collection of antecedents is
different in nature for each form of resistance. As such our findings support prior conceptualizations of resistance, and pro-
vide an integrative structure to the rather piecemeal approach of prior research on resistance. In particular, Fournier (1998)
proposed a resistance ‘continuum’, leading from avoidance to active ‘rebellion’ – although this model appears to focus more
on resistance to consumer society as a whole than to individual innovations. Bagozzi and Lee (1999) also seem to classify
resistance according to its active or passive nature. The resistance model proposed herein has the function of integrating
and clarifying aspects of prior conceptualizations. In particular, the model could be considered to run from less intense/ac-
tive forms of resistance (postponement) to more intense/active (opposition). In this way, the model is primarily a conceptual
tool to structure how consumers think about resistance, not a process model where consumers are considered to ‘move
through’ the various forms of resistance.
With regard to the drivers some interesting conclusions can be drawn. While the drivers differ for the various resistance
forms, there are some general observations that can be made. Throughout the various types of resistance, risk plays an
important role. This implies that risk reduction strategies will play a key role in diminishing consumer resistance toward
innovations. Interestingly, a common consumer risk reduction strategy for consumers is the pursuit of information to in-
crease knowledge about these risks and their solutions (Dowling & Staelin, 1994; Locander & Hermann, 1979). Previous
resistance research (e.g., Herbig & Kramer, 1994) has cautioned companies about elaborate informational campaigns though,
due to the discovery of information overload as an important influence on resistance. However, our results indicate that
information overload may not be a relevant factor for any of the resistance types. This could be explained by the possibility
that the young sample we studied used more new media channels to gather information for themselves, rather than pas-
sively accepting it from companies, as may have been the case in previous research. This suggests that, rather than pushing
information about new innovations towards the consumer, companies should simply make sure the necessary information is
available to the consumer at the moment it is requested or needed, and aid proactive consumers in acquiring the relevant
information (Murray, 1991). The information provided should be tailored to pre-empt consumers’ concerns about the issues
addressed in this study in order to overcome consumers’ resistance. For example, new media can be particularly helpful in
providing demonstrations of how new products work in virtual environments, illustrating how products can be incorporated
in existing habits and living situations.
Additionally, the results suggest that scholars and practitioners should be careful about the simplistic conclusion that
decreasing resistance calls for similar approaches to those used in increasing adoption. Innovation adoption research gener-
ally focuses on emphasizing benefits of the innovation. With resistance, consumers appear to focus on the adverse conse-
quences as essential factors in their consumer decision making. All the factors are stated as unfavorable attributes of the
innovation. Mizerski (1982) finds that such unfavorable attributes have a far stronger impact than favorable. Strategies to
diminish this impact should be carefully contemplated, as resistance cannot simply be offset by offering additional product
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 353
benefits (Fortin & Renton, 2003). Rather than simply maximizing expected gain, companies should also strategize to mini-
mize expected losses (Peter & Tarpey, 1975). Additionally, the varying importance of the drivers for the different resistance
types suggests that companies should develop specific approaches to deal with each particular type of resistance. Each of
these implications is discussed below.
5.1. Strategic implications: Postponement
Concerning postponement, economic risk and usage patterns are the main drivers. This is in line with Szmigin and Foxall
(1998) who state that postponement, the weakest form of resistance, is often driven by situational barriers that can be tem-
porary in nature. Companies should consider the implications of this finding carefully, as many companies apply price-skim-
ming strategies when introducing innovations, to benefit from their (temporary) monopolistic position. However, this seems
be an ill-advised strategy, as economic risk is one of the major drivers for postponement. Such strategies may cause consum-
ers to adopt at a later stage, which does not only imply individual resistance, but might also create loss of the contagions
effects necessary for successful diffusion (van den Bulte & Stremersch, 2004).
Conflicts with existing usage patterns may cause consumers to postpone their innovation decision. However, often such
usage patterns develop over time without conscious reflection about how effective these habits still are (Wansink & Ray,
1996). Therefore, it could be beneficial to communicate to consumers how these innovations in fact do fit within their cur-
rent lifestyle. One way is to design marketing communications that illustrate before and after situations. For example, in re-
cent times Toyota has focused much of its marketing communications on showing how the Prius is still able to deliver a
driving experience consistent with current usage habits (e.g., ability to cover long distances without refueling). It may also
be beneficial to bundle innovations with products that are already part of consumers’ usage patterns (Harris & Blair, 2006).
Such marketing communications aim to illustrate an innovation’s fit with current behaviors, and help in diminishing con-
sumers risk perceptions of innovation (Reinders, Dabholkar, & Frambach, 2008). Of course, companies must be careful with
such strategies, as Microsoft found out to their cost with their bundling of Internet Explorer with Windows in the 1990s.
5.2. Strategic implications: Rejection
As the number and variety of risk dimensions increases, consumers appear to be more likely to outright reject the inno-
vation, rather than postpone adoption. More specifically, in addition to economic risk, functional and social risks are impor-
tant drivers of rejection. Perceived image also plays a part in to rejection. Specifically, image serves a signaling function to
make up for a lack of knowledge; the image is a set of associations related to the innovation, which in the case of resistance
serves as a negative extrinsic cue. While it can be beneficial to counter-attack these negative associations with positive
external cues (Shimp & Bearden, 1982), this is not always sufficient to offset consumers’ negative perceptions. Moreover,
positive signals offered by the innovation literature such as word-of-mouth (e.g., Bansal & Voyer, 2000), while highly effec-
tive, are difficult to induce by the company, especially when the general level of rejection towards the innovation is high
(Midgley & Dowling, 1993). In order to reduce risk, consumers often engage in information-seeking activities (Dholakia,
2001; Locander & Hermann, 1979). Companies can aid consumers in these risk-handling activities in various ways such
as extensive labeling information, or increasing the traceability of certain ingredients/components of the innovation (Yeung
& Morris, 2001). Concerning functional risk, warranties and quality assurances are often mentioned as an important tool to
reduce risk perceptions (Shimp & Bearden, 1982; Yeung & Morris, 2001). Social risk perhaps implies that educating the con-
sumer’s environment (rather than the actual consumer) is most important. Diminishing social risk can be accomplished by
increasing consumer confidence, which might prove to be a difficult task, or by taking a more peripheral route and changing
the perceptions of the environment. Eliciting endorsements and testimonials of celebrities is a commonly suggested strategy
in this regard (Roselius, 1971; Yeung & Morris, 2001).
5.3. Strategic implications: Opposition
Opposition seems to be driven by factors strongly embedded in the consumers’ personal and societal environment. While
the results were fairly consistent throughout the product categories, such factors are more likely to occur in radical than
incremental innovations (Danneels, 2004). Conflicts with usage patterns and perceived image are still relevant, and in addi-
tion to social and functional risk, physical risk comes into play. It is especially striking that economic risk is no longer a sig-
nificant motivational factor. This suggests that monetary incentives are not suitable to prevent opposition. Thus, the general
contention advanced by innovation research that lower price increases adoption likelihood (e.g., Liao & Cheung, 2001), does
not transfer to this particular resistance form. This notion is consistent with the finding that traditions and norms drive only
this resistance type. Additionally, it provides support for the earlier conceptual delineation of Rogers’ (2003) compatibility
construct. An issue that companies should consider in this regard is that it might be extremely difficult to change a societal
context, and indeed is sometimes impossible. Consequently, companies should consider investigating the flexibility of the
innovation, even after introduction (Ram & Sheth, 1989). For example, while instant cake-mix formulas are perfectly accept-
able in today’s kitchen, Betty Crocker’s early introduction in the 1950s of instant mixes was strongly opposed by traditional
housewives. This type of product was not considered ‘genuine cooking’ according to society’s standards at that time. How-
ever, Betty Crocker famously modified the product formula, requiring housewives simply to crack an egg into the mix. This
354 M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
product adaptation made the product a big success, as it appealed more to the traditional cooking experience and gave the
users the sense that they were providing their families with nutritious food as was expected of them.
With regard to physical risk, it is important to note that this type of risk is probably most difficult to assess beforehand
(especially if the potential damage is long-term). Consequently, consumers are strongly inclined to exhibit avoidance behav-
ior with regard to this type of risk (Yeung & Morris, 2001). While innovation literature has often offered word-of-mouth as a
successful way to diminish risk perceptions of consumers (e.g., Midgley & Dowling, 1993), research indicates that this is not
beneficial in the case of expected physical harm (Mitchell & Greatorex, 1990; Roselius, 1971). Consumers ascribe the exper-
tise necessary to evaluate this type of risk only to experts and independent sources (Becker, 2000; Grabner-Kraeuter, 2002).
Therefore, testing of innovations by independent institutes (e.g., by government, or independent standards agencies) is far
more effective (Roselius, 1971; Yeung & Morris, 2001).
6. Limitations and directions for future research
As with any research approach, focus groups are not without limitations (Frey & Fontana, 1993; Greenbaum, 1997). These
limitations are closely related to the interviewing process and the generalizability of the findings (Crabtree, Yanoshik, Miller,
& O’Connor, 1993; Greenbaum, 1997). As far as the process is concerned, group interaction might lead to conformity with the
prevailing opinions in the group and this might even be compounded by the presence of a number of dominant participants.
As a result, some participants might not be willing or able to share their views, or may engage in ‘impression management’.
We detailed the strategies we used to minimize the chance of such issues earlier, yet the possibility must still be taken into
account when interpreting the findings. Focus groups necessarily consist of a small, non-representative sample of the larger
population and as such researchers must be cautious when generalizing these findings (Greenbaum, 1997). In this particular
case the respondents were relatively young and well educated. However, research (e.g., Meuter, Bitner, Ostrom, & Brown,
2005) has stipulated that such socio-demographic characteristics are not particularly effective in explaining consumer deci-
sions with regard to innovations. Even so, future research looking at different socio-demographic samples will likely prove
fruitful in this regard.
The research presented here provides a solid foundation for future work regarding the resistance to innovation. There are
a number of important avenues where further research efforts should be directed. The objective of our work was to deter-
mine a joint framework that recognizes the different forces driving the various forms of resistance. Nevertheless, in doing so
we have devoted less attention to more complex interactive relationships between the antecedent factors in our framework.
While in our data there was no evidence of such interactions, further research with a specific focus on such relationships
could be very insightful. For example, past research has indeed suggested relationships between various types of risk
(e.g., Campbell & Goodstein, 2001). Hence, more detailed insights into these relationships, trade-offs, and the possible
thresholds that consumers hold in that regard, could be very useful in understanding how to overcome consumer resistance.
While the current findings suggest key general antecedents to the different forms of resistance, there are a number of
other factors which need investigation. For example, while a range of product types was discussed, further research should
investigate whether our framework holds for a wider variety of products. More specifically, while the results were fairly con-
sistent throughout the different categories, only product, rather than service, innovations were discussed. Several research-
ers have pointed out that service innovations need to be investigated separately due to their unique characteristics (Hennig-
Thurau, Walsh, & Wruck, 2001; Zeithaml, Parasuraman, & Berry, 1985). Additionally, one wonders about the effects of the
product type with regard to the level of radicalness. It may be the case that radical innovations (or what have been called
‘really new products’ or RNPs) face more difficulties than incremental innovations, as they are more likely to general the
resistance drivers discussed in this study.
Furthermore, our study has focused on cognitive processes, and to a lesser extent on the role of sociological and emotional
elements in explaining resistance. Our results already indicate that social risk is a key factor, but the various dimensions of
sociological effects have been left unexplored. For example, recent literature about individual versus group behavior illus-
trates that not only innovators, but also consumers that resist innovations, use their behavior as a strategy to stand out from
the group (Berger & Heath, 2007). Such consumer behaviors are not considered in the current study and provide an avenue of
research in its own right.
In addition, understanding emotional reactions toward product innovations might further develop our insights on resis-
tance. Duhachek and Oakley (2007) illustrate that one way of coping with negative emotions toward products is to ignore
them, while other emotions trigger behaviors that might aid in overcoming resistance. When and how such emotions surface
and how they interact with the antecedents we uncovered deserves further attention. Nevertheless, while additional re-
search is necessary in order to be more conclusive with regard to the issues presented above, the model presented here pro-
vides an important step forwards in the conceptualization of resistance as a key component of consumer psychology which
warrants detailed investigation in its own right.
Acknowledgements
We thank George Zinkhan and Machiel Reinders for providing us with valuable comments on earlier drafts of the paper,
and Berne van Leeuwen for his data collection efforts.
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 355
References
Altheide, D. L. (1996). Qualitative media analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Bagozzi, R. P., & Lee, K.-H. (1999). Consumer resistance to, and acceptance of, innovations. Advances in Consumer Research, 26(1), 218–225.
Bansal, H. S., & Voyer, P. A. (2000). Word-of-mouth processes within a services purchase decision context. Journal of Service Research, 3(2), 166–177.
Bearden, W. O., & Shimp, T. A. (1982). The use of extrinsic cues to facilitate product adoption. Journal of Marketing Research, 19(2), 229–239.
Becker, T. (2000). Consumer perception of fresh meat quality: A framework for analysis. British Food Journal, 102(3), 158–176.
Berger, J., & Heath, C. (2007). Where consumers diverge from others: Identity signaling and product domains. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(August),
121–134.
Bredahl, L. (2001). Determinants of consumer attitudes and purchase intentions with regard to genetically modified foods – Results of a cross-national
survey. Journal of Consumer Policy, 24, 23–61.
Bryman, A. (2004). Social research methods (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Campbell, M. C., & Goodstein, R. C. (2001). The moderating effect of perceived risk on consumers’ evaluations of product incongruity: Preference for the
norm. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 439–449.
Chau, P. Y. K. (1996). An empirical assessment of a modified technology acceptance model. Journal of Management Information Systems, 13(2), 185–204.
Cho, I., & Kim, Y. (2001–2002). Critical factors for assimilation of object-orientated programming languages. Journal of Management Information Systems,
18(3), 125–156.
Coetsee, L. (1999). From resistance to commitment. Public Administration Quarterly, 23(2), 204–222.
Crabtree, B. F., Yanoshik, M. K., Miller, W. L., & O’Connor, P. J. (1993). Selecting individual or group interviews. In D. L. Morgan (Ed.), Successful focus groups
(pp. 137–152). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
D’Errico, R. A. (2005). Harris interactive poll finds mixed news for VoIP adoption. The Business Review, March 15, <http://albany.bizjournals.com/>.
Danneels, E. (2003). Tight-loose coupling with customers: The enactment of customer orientation. Strategic Management Journal, 24(66), 559–576.
Danneels, E. (2004). Disruptive technology reconsidered: A critique and research agenda. Journal of Product Innovation Management, 21, 246–258.
Davidson, R. S., & Walley, P. B. (1985). Computer fear and addiction: Analysis, prevention and possible modification. Journal of Organizational Behavior
Management, 6, 37–51.
Dekker, W. (2007). Gouden Kooi dit jaar van de buis. Volkskrant, August 23, <http://www.volkskrant.nl/binnenland/article454703.ece/
Gouden_Kooi_dit_jaar_van_de_buis>.
Dhebar, A. (1996). Speeding high-tech producer, meeting the balking customer. Sloan Management Review, 37(2), 37–49.
Dholakia, U. M. (2001). A motivational process model of product involvement and consumer risk perception. European Journal of Marketing, 35(11/12),
1340–1360.
Dittmar, H., & Drury, J. (2000). Self-image – Is it in the bag? A qualitative comparison between ‘ordinary’ and ‘excessive’ consumers. Journal of Economic
Psychology, 21, 109–142.
Dowling, G. R., & Staelin, R. (1994). A model of perceived risk and intended risk-handling activity. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(1), 119–135.
Duhachek, A., & Oakley, J. L. (2007). Mapping the hierarchical structure of coping: Unifying empirical and theoretical perspectives. Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 17(3), 218–233.
Fain, D., & Roberts, M. L. (1997). Technology vs. consumer behavior: The battle for the financial services customer. Journal of Direct Marketing, 11(1), 44–54.
Fern, E. E. (2001). Advanced focus group research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Fortin, D. R., & Renton, M. S. (2003). Consumer acceptance of genetically modified foods in New Zealand. British Food Journal, 105(1/2), 42–58.
Fournier, S. (1998). Special session summary, consumer resistance. Societal motivations, consumer manifestations, and implications in the marketing
domain. Advances in Consumer Research, 25, 88–90.
Foxall, G. R. (1993). The influence of cognitive style on consumers’ variety seeking among food innovations. British Food Journal, 95, 32–36.
Foxall, G. R. (1994). Consumer initiators: Both adopters and innovators. In M. J. Kirton (Ed.), Adopters and innovators (pp. 236–259). London: Routledge.
Franzosi, R. (2004). Content analysis. In M. Hardy & A. Bryman (Eds.), Handbook of data analysis (pp. 547–565). London: Sage Publications.
Frey, J. H., & Fontana, A. (1993). The group interview in social research. In D. L. Morgan (Ed.), Successful focus groups (pp. 20–34). Newbury Park: Sage
Publications.
Ganiere, P., Chern, W., Hahn, D., & Chiang, F.-S. (2004). Consumer attitudes towards genetically modified foods in emerging markets: The impact of labeling
in Taiwan. International Food and Agribusiness Management Review, 7(3), 1–20.
Garcia, R., & Atkin, T. (2002). Coo-petition for the diffusion of resistant innovations: A case study in the global wine industry. Institute for Global Innovation
Management Working Paper, 05-002 (pp. 1–22).
Garrett, D. E. (1987). The effectiveness of marketing policy boycotts: Environmental opposition to marketing. Journal of Marketing, 51(April), 46–57.
Gatignon, H., & Robertson, T. S. (1989). Technology diffusion: An empirical test of competitive effects. Journal of Marketing, 53(1), 35–49.
Grabner-Kraeuter, S. (2002). The role of consumers’ trust in online-shopping. Journal of Business Ethics, 39, 43–50.
Greenbaum, T. L. (1997). The handbook for focus group research. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
Greenleaf, E. A., & Lehmann, D. R. (1995). Reasons for substantial delay in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 22(2), 186–199.
Haig, M. (2003). Brand failures. London, UK: Kogan Page.
Harris, J., & Blair, E. A. (2006). Consumer preference for product bundles: The role of reduced search costs. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 34(4),
506–513.
Hennig-Thurau, T., Walsh, G., & Wruck, O. (2001). An investigation into the factors determining the success of service innovations – The case of motion
pictures. Academy of Marketing Science Review, 1(6)<http://www.amsreview.org/articles/hennig06-2001.pdf>.
Herbig, P. A., & Day, R. L. (1992). Customer acceptance. The key to successful introductions of innovations. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 10(1), 4–15.
Herbig, P. A., & Kramer, H. (1994). The effect of information overload on the innovation choice process: Innovation overload. Journal of Consumer Marketing,
11(2), 45–54.
Herrmann, R. O. (1993). The tactics of consumer resistance. Group action and the marketplace exit. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 130–134.
Hirschheim, R., & Newman, M. (1988). Information systems and user resistance. theory and practice. The Computer Journal, 31(5), 398–408.
Hirschman, E. C. (1987). Adoption of an incredibly complex innovation: Propositions from a humanistic viewpoint. Advances in Consumer Research, 14,
376–377.
Holbrook, M. B., & O’Shaughnessy, J. (1988). On the scientific status of consumer research and the need for an interpretive approach to studying
consumption behaviour. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3), 398–402.
Hurter, A. P., & Rubenstein, A. H. (1978). Market penetration by new innovations, technological literature. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 197,
221.
Karahanna, E., Agarwal, R., & Angst, C. M. (2006). Reconceptualizing compatibility beliefs in technology acceptance research. MIS Quarterly, 30(4), 781–804.
Kassarjian, H. H. (1977). Content analysis in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 4(June), 8–18.
Keller, K. L., & Staelin, R. (1987). Effects of quality and quantity of information on decision effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Research, 2, 200–213.
Kleijnen, M. H. P., de Ruyter, K., & Andreassen, T. W. (2005). Image congruence and the adoption of service innovations. Journal of Service Research, 7(4),
343–359.
Kleijnen, M. H. P., de Ruyter, K., & Wetzels, M. G. M. (2004). Consumer adoption of wireless services: Discovering the rules, while playing the game. Journal of
Interactive Marketing, 18(2), 51–61.
Kleijnen, M. H. P., de Ruyter, K., & Wetzels, M. G. M. (2007). An assessment of value creation in mobile service delivery and the moderating role of time
consciousness. Journal of Retailing, 83(1), 33–46.
356 M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357
Klerck, D., & Sweeney, J. C. (2007). The effect of knowledge types on consumer-perceived risk and adoption of genetically modified foods. Psychology &
Marketing, 24(2), 171–193.
Kolbe, R. H., & Burnett, M. S. (1991). Content-analysis research: An examination of applications with directives for improving research reliability and
objectivity. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(2), 243–250.
Kozinets, R. V., & Handelman, J. M. (1998). Ensouling consumption: A netnographic of the meaning of boycotting behavior. Advances in Consumer Research,
25, 475–480.
Krueger, R. A. (1988). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research. London: Sage publications.
Lapointe, L., & Rivard, S. (2005). A multilevel model of resistance to information technology implementation. MIS Quarterly, 29(3), 461–491.
Lee, H. G., & Clark, T. H. (1996–1997). Market process reengineering through electronic market systems: Opportunities and challenges. Journal of
Management Information Systems, 13(3), 113–137.
Lennon, S. J., Kim, M., Johnson, K. K. P., Jolly, L. D., Damhorst, M. L., & Jasper, C. R. (2007). A longitudinal look at rural consumer adoption of online shopping.
Psychology & Marketing, 24(4), 375–401.
Liao, Z., & Cheung, M. T. (2001). Internet-based e-shopping and consumer attitudes: An empirical study. Information & Management, 38, 299–306.
Lin, C.-H., Hsin-Yu, S., & Sher, P. J. (2007). Integrating technology readiness into technology acceptance. The TRAM model. Psychology & Marketing, 24(7),
641–657.
Locander, W. B., & Hermann, P. W. (1979). The effect of self-confidence and anxiety on information seeking in consumer risk reduction. Journal of Marketing
Research, 16(May), 268–274.
Luomala, H. T., & Laaksonen, M. (1999). A qualitative exploration of mood-regulatory self-gift behaviors. Journal of Economic Psychology, 20, 147–182.
Malhotra, N. K. (1984). Reflections on the information overload paradigm in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 436–437.
Martinko, M. J., Henry, J. W., & Zmud, R. W. (1996). An attributional explanation of individual resistance to the introduction of information technologies in
the workplace. Behaviour & Information Technology, 15(5), 313–330.
Meuter, M. L., Bitner, M. J., Ostrom, A. L., & Brown, S. W. (2005). Choosing among alternative service delivery modes: An investigation of customer trial of
self-service technologies. Journal of Marketing, 26(April), 61–83.
Midgley, D. F., & Dowling, G. R. (1993). A longitudinal study of product form innovation: The interaction between predispositions and social messages.
Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 611–625.
Mitchell, V.-W., & Greatorex, M. (1990). Consumer perceived risk in the UK food market. British Food Journal, 92(2), 16–22.
Mitchell, V.-W., & Papavassiliou, V. (1997). Exploring consumer confusion in the watch market. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 15(4), 164–172.
Mizerski, R. W. (1982). An attribute explanation of the disproportionate influence of unfavorable information. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(December),
301–310.
Moisio, R. J., & Askegaard, S. (2002). ‘‘Fighting Culture” – Mobile phone consumption practices as means of consumer resistance. In T. Ping & R. Zwick (Eds.),
Asia pacific advances in consumer research (5th ed., pp. 24–29). Valdosta, GA: Association for Consumer Research.
Molesworth, M., & Suortti, J.-P. (2002). Buying cars online: The adoption of the web for high-involvement, high cost purchases. Journal of Consumer
Behaviour, 2(2), 155–168.
Moore, G. A. (2002). Crossing the chasm: Marketing and selling high-tech products to mainstream consumers. New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers.
Morgan, D. L., & Spanish, M. T. (1985). Social interaction and the cognitive organisation of health-relevant knowledge. Sociology of Health & Illness, 7(3),
401–422.
Murray, K. B. (1991). A test of services marketing theory: Consumer information acquisition activities. Journal of Marketing, 55(January), 10–25.
Nabih, M. I., Bloem, J. G., & Poiesz, T. B. C. (1997). Conceptual issues in the study of innovation adoption behaviour. Advances in Consumer Research, 24,
190–196.
Noussair, C., Robin, S., & Ruffieux, B. (2004). Do consumers really refuse to buy genetically modified food? Economic Journal, 114(492), 102–120.
Oreg, S. (2003). Resistance to change: Developing and individual differences measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(4), 680–693.
Oreg, S. (2006). Personality, context, and resistance to organizational change. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(1), 73–101.
Parasuraman, A. (2000). Technology Readiness Index (TRI): A multiple-item scale to measure readiness to embrace new technologies. Journal of Service
Research, 2(4), 307–320.
Pavia, T., &Costa, J. A. (1993). The winning number: Consumer perceptions of alpha-numeric brand names. Journal of Marketing, 57(July), 85–98.
Peñaloza, L., & Price, L. L. (1993). Consumer resistance. A conceptual overview. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 123–128.
Perreault, W. D., Jr., & Leigh, L. E. (1989). Reliability of nominal data based on qualitative judgments. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(2), 135–148.
Peter, J. P., & Tarpey, L. X. Sr., (1975). A comparative analysis of three consumer decision strategies. Journal of Consumer Research, 2(1), 29–37.
Ram, S. (1987). A model of innovation resistance. Advances in Consumer Research, 14(1), 208–211.
Ram, S. (1989). Successful innovation using strategies to reduce consumer resistance. Journal of Product Innovation Management, 6(1), 20–34.
Ram, S., & Sheth, J. N. (1989). Consumer resistance to innovations: The marketing problem and its solutions. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 6(2), 5–14.
Reinders, M. J., Dabholkar, P. A., & Frambach, R. T. (2008). Consequenses of forcing consumers to use technology-based self-service. Journal of Service
Research, 11(2), 107–123.
Ritson, M., & Dobscha, S. (1999). Marketing heretics: Resistance is/is not futile. Advances in Consumer Research, 26, 159.
Rogers, E. M. (2003). Diffusion of innovations (5 ed.). New York: Free press.
Roselius, T. (1971). Consumer rankings of risk reduction methods. Journal of Marketing, 35(January), 56–61.
Rust, R. T., & Cooil, B. (1994). Reliability measures for qualitative data: Theory and implications. Journal of Marketing Research, 31(1), 1–14.
Saba, A., Rosati, S., & Vassallo, M. (2000). Biotechnology in agriculture: Perceived risk, benefits and attitudes in Italy. British Food Journal, 102(2), 114–122.
Sen, S., Gurhan-Canli, Z., & Morwitz, V. (2001). Withholding consumption: A social dilemma perspective on consumer boycotts. Journal of Consumer
Research, 28(December), 399–417.
Sheth, J. N. (1981). Psychology of innovation resistance: The less developed concept in diffusion research. Research in Marketing, 4, 273–282.
Shimp, T. A., & Bearden, W. O. (1982). Warranty and other extrinsic cue effects on consumers’ risk perceptions. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 38–46.
Shoemaker, R. W., & Shoaf, F. R. (1975). Behavioural changes in the trail of new products. Journal of Consumer Research, 104, 109.
Szmigin, I., & Foxall, G. (1998). Three forms of innovation resistance: The case of retail payment methods. Technovation, 18(6/7), 459–468.
Taylor, S., & Todd, P. (1995). Decomposition and crossover effects in the theory of planned behavior: A study of consumer adoption intentions. International
Journal of Research in Marketing, 12(2), 137–155.
Thiesse, F. (2007). RFID, privacy and the perception of risk: A strategic framework. Auto-ID Labs White Paper, WP-BIZAPP-031 (pp. 1–24).
Tornatzky, L. G., & Klein, K. J. (1982). Innovation characteristics and innovation adoption-implementation: A meta-analysis of findings. IEEE Transactions on
Engineering Management, 29(1), 28–45.
van den Bulte, C., & Stremersch, S. (2004). Social contagion and income heterogeneity in new product diffusion: A meta-analytic test. Marketing Science,
23(4), 530–544.
Wansink, B., & Ray, M. L. (1996). Advertising strategies to increase usage frequency. Journal of Marketing, 60(January), 31–46.
Woodside, A. G., & Biemans, W. G. (2005). Modeling innovation, manufacturing, diffusion and adoption/rejection processes. Journal of Business & Industrial
Marketing, 20(7), 380–393.
Yeung, R. M. W., & Morris, J. (2001). Food safety risk: Consumer perception and purchase behaviour. British Food Journal, 103(3), 170–186.
Zeithaml, V. A., Parasuraman, A., & Berry, L. L. (1985). Problems and strategies in services marketing. Journal of Marketing, 49(Spring), 33–46.
M. Kleijnen et al. / Journal of Economic Psychology 30 (2009) 344–357 357
... middle-east countries, have progressed fast in digitalising Islamic banking, but Indonesian Islamic banking is not catching up with this pace (Leong et al., 2017). The possible reasons for this phenomenon can be linked to the common tendency to resist change (Kaur et al., 2020;Kleijnen et al., 2009), disbelief among consumers about the capability of Islamic banks to offer robust technological solutions (Bonheure and de Gantès, 2021), or it can also be linked to the digitalisation ecosystem present in Indonesia (Bonheure and de Gantès, 2021;Zouari and Abdelhedi, 2021). These factors may be preventing Indonesian consumers from diverting from conventional banking ...
... According to the consumer resistance theory (CRT), when an alternate solution is made available to consumers, there is a tendency to resist it, irrespective of whether it is better than the available solution or not (Kaur et al., 2020;Kleijnen et al., 2009). This resistance can be due to value, usage, or risk. ...
... Although digital banking is the need of the hour and its utility is proven in a COVID-19-like situation, respondents have still hesitated to adopt it in a significant way. This is in line with the consumer resistance theory (CRT), as explained in Section 2, that even better solutions are resisted (Kaur et al., 2020;Kleijnen et al., 2009). In this study, the diverting effects have proven to trigger consumer resistance (hypothesis H1). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose – Use of digital Islamic banking services is not increasing in Indonesia, despite the country’s sizable Islamic population. This non-adoption might have resulted from consumer resistance brought on by attempts to divert them, or it might have been a direct consequence of the diverting effect. This study aims to examine the direct relationship between the diverting effects and the decision to not adopt digital Islamic banking, as well as the mediating role of consumer resistance. Design/methodology/approach – The partial least squares-based structural equation modelling (PLS-SEM) technique is used to analyse relationships among diverting effects, consumer resistance and non-adoption decisions. A total of 517 middle- and upper-class Indonesian consumers were involved as a sample, with several criteria such as being a conventional bank customer for at least 5 years and using digital services with a minimum of 5 transactions per month but not yet being an Islamic bank customer. Findings – It is found that the direct relationship between the diverting effects and the non-adoption decisions is not statistically significant (β = 0.045, p = 0.15). However, it is found that the diverting effects trigger consumer resistance in a statistically significant manner (β = 0.192, p < 0.1). This consumer resistance is found to be influencing non-adoption decisions directly (β = 0.694, p < 0.1), as well as a mediating factor between diverting effects and the non-adoption decisions (β = 0.133, p < 0.1). Originality/value – The paper deals with an emerging industry and offers actionable insights for its growth. In practice, this study suggests several possible strategies for the Islamic banking industry to grow under circumstances where digital banking is considered sine-qua-non of modern banking. Several related theories are also evaluated in this study.
... TB are also a cause of consumer resistance to mobile commerce applications (Hew et al., 2023), and negative image perceptions can ultimately lead to the rejection of innovations (Kleijnen et al., 2009). In the sports domain, Uhrich (2022) investigated the impact of fan resistance to experiential applications specifically driven by declining atmosphere (UB), distraction from the game (TB), and perceived social risks coupled with data security concerns (RB)-on usage attitudes and adoption intentions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Digital technology has greatly influenced the way the public engages in sports activities. However, the behavioral decision-making process associated with the adoption of digital technology for sports participation remains unclear. This study employs the Behavioral Reasoning Theory to elucidate the cognitive processes underlying participants’ behavioral intentions by examining the reasons for and against the adoption, as well as the influence of self-efficacy. The model presented is a theoretical expansion of the current innovation frameworks within sports management and marketing. The findings indicate that the reasons against adoption by respondents exert a more substantial impact on their attitudes and behavioral intentions than the reasons for adoption. Perceived enjoyment and perceived barriers to use are identified as the leading factors for reasons in favor of and against adoption, respectively. The study also highlights the pivotal role of participants’ self-efficacy in the cognitive decision-making process concerning the adoption of digital technology for sports participation. To amplify the impact of digital technology within the sports domain, it delineates the reasons for and against adoption and to account for the influence of self-efficacy in the decision-making process, thereby ensuring that the integration of digital technology with sports more effectively addresses the practical needs of participants.
... Perceived risk was primarily related to the uncertainty that comes with the acceptance or adoption of a new technology. Overall, perceived risk is the evaluation of the possible negative outcomes that come from the adoption of a technology [38,39]. Two other studies defined multiple types of perceived risk, those being the possible loss of money, possible poor performance of the technology, possible social disapproval, possible physical harm, possible anxiety, and possible loss of time related to the acceptance or adoption of the technology [40,41]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Artificial intelligence (AI) technology has become one of the most frequently discussed subjects in the development of technology in recent years. Due to its incredible pattern recognition, it can help humans complete work much faster than before with little to no monetary cost. Despite the widespread impact that AI technologies have on various fields, acceptance and adoption of AI lag behind because of a wide range of factors among users. This paper outlines the results of a large literature review that attempts to tease out some of these factors by examining individual differences that may impact the acceptance and adoption of AI. This goal was achieved through an exploration of individual differences that play a role in the acceptance and adoption of new technologies more broadly, as well as AI technologies, to gain a more holistic understanding of the factors contributing to the lack of acceptance and adoption of AI. The main goal of this literature review was to find the individual differences (IDs) associated with the acceptance and adoption of AI technology and general technology. A secondary goal was to create a model based on the acceptance of general technology that could assist in future AI technology research, development, and implementation. This paper identifies several IDs that were found to play a role in the adoption and acceptance of AI technology, as well as 15 specific IDs that were commonly shown to play a role in the adoption and acceptance of general technology. Because of the rapid development of AI technologies in recent years, there is a lack of research examining the acceptance and adoption of AI technologies; however, there is a great deal of research examining the broader acceptance and adoption of technology, and there is significant overlap between the studies that examined general technology acceptance and adoption and those that examined AI-specific technology acceptance and adoption. Because of this, we believe that the research on general technology acceptance and adoption can be used as a foundation and inspiration for future research on AI technology in this area.
... TB are also a cause of consumer resistance to mobile commerce applications (Hew et al., 2023), and negative image perceptions can ultimately lead to the rejection of innovations (Kleijnen et al., 2009). In the sports domain, Uhrich (2022) investigated the impact of fan resistance to experiential applications specifically driven by declining atmosphere (UB), distraction from the game (TB), and perceived social risks coupled with data security concerns (RB)-on usage attitudes and adoption intentions. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Digital technology has greatly influenced the way the public engages in sports activities. However, the behavioral decision-making process associated with the adoption of digital technology for sports participation remains unclear. This study employs the Behavioral Reasoning Theory to elucidate the cognitive processes underlying participants' behavioral intentions by examining the reasons for and against the adoption, as well as the influence of self-efficacy. The model presented is a theoretical expansion of the current innovation frameworks within sports management and marketing. Findings indicate that the reasons against adoption by respondents exert a more substantial impact on their attitudes and behavioral intentions than the reasons for adoption. Perceived enjoyment and perceived barriers to use are identified as the leading factors for reasons in favor of and against adoption, respectively. The study also highlights the pivotal role of participants' self-efficacy in the cognitive decision-making process concerning the adoption of digital technology for sports participation. To amplify the impact of digital technology within the sports domain, it delineates the reasons for and against adoption and to account for the influence of self-efficacy in the decision-making process, thereby ensuring that the integration of digital technology with sports more effectively addresses the practical needs of participants.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Digital transformation is a complex and continuous process that presents significant challenges for companies and employees. Employee resistance, a factor often cited for the failure of digital initiatives, hinders these initiatives and contributes to stress, affecting employee well-being – a connection not yet fully explored in extant literature. Therefore, this study aimed to determine the factors that increase employee resistance to digital transformation and how it can be mitigated. Design/methodology/approach This study adopts a multiple case study approach to analyze qualitative data from ten industrial companies in Finland. Findings Factors influencing resistance at the individual, organizational, and technological levels were identified, and three key components of the resistance mitigation strategy from the case companies were identified: learning, communication and participation. The findings, supported by relevant literature, link resistance to employee well-being and propose that the identified mitigation strategy reduces resistance and supports employee well-being during digital transformation. Practical implications This study provides practical implications and suggests avenues for future studies to examine this unsaturated yet important research area. Originality/value This study is among the first to empirically examine employee resistance in digital transformation and link it to employee well-being.
Article
Purpose Despite a sizeable body of innovation adoption scholarship, innovation failure rates are estimated to be in the range of 40% to 80%. This study aims to better explain the complex consumer decision-making journey by leveraging behavioral reasoning theory (BRT) and integrating the dichotomous influences of reasons for (RF) and reasons against (RA), and the role of moderators in a unified behavioral framework. Design/methodology/approach First, an exploratory reason elicitation study was conducted ( n = 44) to propose a theoretical model with contextual reasons for and against constructs. Second, a quantitative study was conducted ( n = 400) to validate the hypothesized structural relationships and the role of moderators. Findings Reasons for and against adoption are discrete constructs and significantly impact consumer attitudes and intentions. Reasons for adoption emerge as the strongest influencing factor in shaping consumer attitudes. Values influence intentions directly and via reasons but do not influence attitudes. Consumer innovativeness and external stressors moderate consumers’ resistance toward adopting innovations. Practical implications This study presents a novel approach for managers to generate deeper insights into consumer decision-making by integrating contextual reasons for and against, as well as the role of moderators in a unified behavioral framework. This approach enables managers to address both drivers and inhibitors of adoption in the specific radical innovation context they are working on. Originality/value This study integrates reasons for and against in a unified behavioral framework and applies it to radical technological innovations in an emerging market context. This study introduces novel constructs to BRT and examines the role of moderators in the BRT framework.
Chapter
This research paper aims to understand the factors influencing consumers' adoption of sustainable fashion like upcycled products using the Behavioural Reasoning Theory (BRT). The BRT was used to investigate the relationship between the value of openness to change, Attitude, and reasons for and against to buy upcycled fashion products. A total of 323 completed questionnaires were collected, and a response rate of 63.3% was recorded. The results of the CFA showed that the proposed measurement model appears to be satisfactory and provides an excellent fit. The structural equation modelling (SEM) was utilised in the research in order to prove the proposed hypothesis. The findings of this study that environment concern, pride and knowledge about sustainable option can positively contribute to the fashion industry's sustainability goals. Study will also assist in recognizing the reasons against upcycle buying in identify barriers to purchase, such as cost, accessibility, or lack of awareness.
Article
Full-text available
Theoretical background: Innovation is essential for enterprise development and economic growth. Therefore, knowing and characterising the determinants of innovation activity in enterprises is crucial. Determining the factors that shape the innovativeness of enterprises will enable entrepreneurs to skillfully and flexibly adapt to both the current and future market situation. Innovation is a process that involves significant expenses, a long payback period, specific risks and uncertainty, as companies are exposed to various obstacles, both internal and external, throughout the innovation process. Factors limiting innovation implementation are perceived differently by companies of different sizes, ages or depending on the scale of their operations. The literature provides many classifications and divisions of barriers to innovation activity, butthere is still little research on the factors influencing their perception. Purpose of the article: The study aims to identify the factors hindering the decision to start or carry out innovative activity and the determinants of a firms’ perception of barriers to innovation activity in enterprises from the Lubelskie Voivodship in 2017–2019. Research methods: A questionnaire survey on a research sample of 104 enterprises from the Lubelskie Voivodship was conducted, and exploratory factor analysis was employed. Main findings: The paper identifies the most important factors hindering the decision to start or carry out innovative activity in enterprises from the Lubelskie Voivodship in 2017–2019. On the basis of the results of surveys and exploratory factor analysis, it was found that an important barrier to carrying out innovative activity of entities in the Lubelskie Voivodship is the financial barrier related to the lack of capital, difficult access to it and high cost of its service, as well as high costs of innovative activity. In the opinion of entrepreneurs, the market situation, i.e. uncertain demand for new ideas and too much competition in the market and internal processes, are important factors for the development of innovation. The analysis also demonstrates that the sector and scale of activity impact the perception of barriers to innovation. For companies operating in the service sector, the constraints to innovation processes related to the financial, market and internal processes are lower than in the group of industrial and construction companies. The results also indicate a negative relation between the scale of activity and innovation obstacles related to internal processes.
Article
Full-text available
Chapter 1. Plugged in Research Chapter 2. Ethnographic Content Analysis Chapter 3. Process of Qualitative Document Analysis Chapter 4. Newspapers, Magazines, and Electronic Documents Chapter 5. Electronic Reality I Chapter 6. Electronic Reality II Chapter 7. Tracking Discourse Chapter 8. Field Notes and Other Data
Article
Consumer researchers have become increasingly interested in the study of coping. This research contributes to this emerging paradigm by investigating structural theories of coping using a hierarchical modeling approach to better understand the basic dimensional properties of this multifaceted construct. This research makes gains along several fronts. First, the authors empirically examine a new lower order theory of coping based on a comprehensive synthesis of the literature, reconciling nomenclatural and conceptual redundancies found in the structure of coping literature. Analyses across 2 studies using different methods offer support for a 2‐dimensional higher order model of coping. Finally, a third study using an experimental approach validates these findings by showing unique effects relating an approach‐avoidance based higher order model to theoretically predicted emotional antecedents. By examining the mapping of lower order coping strategies onto several higher order coping theories, this research addresses the extant disjuncture between coping theory and measurement found in the literature and offers reconciliation between these 2 perspectives. The findings suggest the need for future coping research to focus on other 2‐dimensional theories beyond problem‐focused versus emotion‐focused coping. The findings build on the synergies among the many coping perspectives, and the implications of a better understanding of coping's hierarchical structure for consumer researchers is discussed.
Article
When a buyer perceives risk in a purchase he can pursue different strategies of risk resolution. This article presents research findings which indicate that consumers have preferences for different methods of risk reduction associated with various types of loss.