ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Narcissism is characterized in part by an acute concern for one’s appearance. Despite this fundamental aspect of narcissism, little is known about whether narcissism is manifested in features of physical appearance. Can you tell if someone is narcissistic just by looking at them? Our results indicate that snap judgments of narcissism based on full-body photographs are at least as accurate as snap judgments of any of the big five personality traits. Narcissists are more likely to wear expensive, flashy clothing, have an organized, neat appearance requiring a lot of preparation, and (in females) wear makeup and show cleavage. Furthermore, observers’ judgments correlate with the presence of these cues, suggesting that they are drawing on the correct information when making their judgments. Finally, observers’ judgments are associated with three of the four facets of narcissism and capture the unique constellation of personality traits typical of narcissists (i.e., high extraversion and low agreeableness). These findings suggest that physical appearance reflects narcissists’ personality, preoccupation with good looks, and desire to be the center of attention, and serves as a vehicle with which to promote their status.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy
Portrait of a narcissist: Manifestations of narcissism in physical
appearance
q
Simine Vazire
a,*
, Laura P. Naumann
b
, Peter J. Rentfrow
c
, Samuel D. Gosling
d
a
Washington University in St. Louis, Department of Psychology, 1 Brookings Drive, Campus Box 1125, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
b
Institute of Personality and Social Research, University of California Berkeley, 4115 Tolaman Hall #5050, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
c
Faculty of Social and Political Sciences, University of Cambridge, Free School Lane, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK
d
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station, A8000, Austin, Texas 78712, USA
article info
Article history:
Available online 24 June 2008
Keywords:
Narcissism
Personality
Personality judgment
Person perception
Brunswik Lens Model
abstract
Narcissism is characterized in part by an acute concern for one’s appearance. Despite this
fundamental aspect of narcissism, little is known about whether narcissism is manifested
in features of physical appearance. Can you tell if someone is narcissistic just by looking at
them? Our results indicate that snap judgments of narcissism based on full-body photo-
graphs are at least as accurate as snap judgments of any of the big five personality traits.
Narcissists are more likely to wear expensive, flashy clothing, have an organized, neat
appearance requiring a lot of preparation, and (in females) wear makeup and show cleav-
age. Furthermore, observers’ judgments correlate with the presence of these cues, suggest-
ing that they are drawing on the correct information when making their judgments.
Finally, observers’ judgments are associated with three of the four facets of narcissism
and capture the unique constellation of personality traits typical of narcissists (i.e., high
extraversion and low agreeableness). These findings suggest that physical appearance
reflects narcissists’ personality, preoccupation with good looks, and desire to be the center
of attention, and serves as a vehicle with which to promote their status.
Ó2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
It is no accident that the myth from which narcissism gets its name has its crucial turning point when the central char-
acter Narcissus catches sight of himself reflected in a pool of water. So mesmerized was Narcissus by his own unattainable
reflection that he exclaimed ‘‘then let me look at you and feed my wretched frenzy on your image” (Ovid, 2004, book III, lines
621–622). This acute concern about one’s appearance is a fundamental aspect of sub-clinical narcissism
1
(Raskin & Terry,
1988; Sedikides, Gregg, Cisek, & Hart, 2007) but has received no empirical attention. The psychological portrait of narcissists
has been well described (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001;Vazire & Funder, 2006) but there is strong reason to believe that narcissism
also has a distinct physical signature. Indeed, there is good theoretical reason to suppose that of all traits, narcissism will be
manifested in the domain of appearance. What are the visible cues that betray narcissism and do observers pick up on them?
Research on the accuracy of snap judgments, or ‘‘zero-acquaintance” judgments, has shown that some traits can be accu-
rately perceived from minimal information. For example, targets’ extraversion and conscientiousness can be perceived accu-
0092-6566/$ - see front matter Ó2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.007
q
Portions of this research were supported by National Science Foundation Grant No. 0422924 to Samuel D. Gosling and a National Science Foundation
fellowship to Laura P. Naumann. We are grateful to Matthias Mehl for his helpful comments on this paper.
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: svazire@artsci.wustl.edu (S. Vazire).
1
Throughout the paper, we use the term ‘‘narcissism” to refer to sub-clinical narcissism.
Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Research in Personality
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp
Author's personal copy
rately after a very brief interaction (Kenny, 1994; Watson, 1989), intelligence can be perceived accurately after watching a
videotape of the targets reading a weather report (Borkenau & Liebler, 1993), and openness to experience can be perceived
accurately after seeing the targets’ bedrooms or websites, or hearing their top-10 favorite songs (Gosling, Ko, Mannarelli, &
Morris, 2002; Rentfrow & Gosling, 2006; Vazire & Gosling, 2004). However, we know very little about how narcissism—the
trait most explicitly connected to appearance—is manifested and perceived in everyday life.
1.1. Manifestations of narcissism
Why should narcissism, in particular, be associated with a distinct physical appearance? Several aspects of narcissism are
directly related to physical appearance, and in each case these aspects are referred to in the Narcissistic Personality Inventory
(NPI, Raskin & Terry, 1988) , the most widely used instrument designed to assess sub-clinical narcissism. First, narcissists are
characterized in part by their high levels of vanity and interest in their own appearance. Several items on the NPI directly
address this tendency (e.g., ‘‘I like looking at myself in the mirror,” ‘‘I like to start new fads and fashions”). Thus, narcissists
may pay special attention to their appearance, leading to observable manifestations such as matching, fashionable clothes,
and a tidy, organized appearance.
Another characteristic of narcissists that might affect their physical appearance is their desire to be the center of atten-
tion. NPI items such as ‘‘I really like to be the center of attention,” ‘‘I like to display my body,” and ‘‘I am apt to show off if I get
the chance” tap into this facet of narcissism. Narcissists’ exhibitionist tendencies might lead them to dress and adorn them-
selves in provocative, attention-grabbing ways. For example, narcissists may try to attract attention by wearing flashy or
revealing clothing or by adorning themselves (e.g., with makeup).
Finally, narcissists’ concern with status could also influence their physical appearance. Indeed, researchers have hypoth-
esized that narcissists are especially likely to use clothes to enhance their status. For example, Sedikides et al. (2007) pro-
posed that narcissists should be more likely than non-narcissists to buy brand-name, expensive, and stylish clothes, and
to wear them as signals to their actual or desired status. Several items on the NPI tap into this facet of narcissism, such
as ‘‘I like having authority over people,” ‘‘I insist upon getting the respect that is due me,” and ‘‘I want to amount to some-
thing in the eyes of the world.”
Despite these theoretical links between narcissism and physical appearance, there is no research on the physical mani-
festations of narcissism or perceivers’ use of appearance cues in forming impressions of narcissism. Does narcissism have
reliable physical cues? If so, can you tell if someone is a narcissist just by looking at them? The present study examines these
questions.
1.2. Present study
The aim of the present study was to examine whether, and how, narcissism is manifested in physical appearance. We
tested three research questions related to this issue.
Question 1: How accurate are snap judgments of narcissism? As stated above, there are many reasons to think that narcis-
sism would be easy to spot. Narcissists care about fashion, they like to be the center of attention, and they like to be re-
spected and praised. It is therefore reasonable to suppose that people with such characteristics would alter their
appearance to stand out. However, it would be surprising if a trait as psychologically complex as narcissism could be easily
detected from just a glance. For example, previous research has shown that the level of accuracy achieved with snap judg-
ments from physical appearance is usually low to moderate (Naumann, Vazire, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2008). Thus, we did not
expect high levels of accuracy; even moderate levels of accuracy would be impressive in this context.
What, then, is a practically significant degree of accuracy? To gauge this, we will compare the accuracy of snap judgments
of narcissism based on physical appearance to the accuracy of judgments of narcissism made by well-acquainted informants
(i.e., close friends). This provides an important benchmark because the degree of accuracy achieved by those who know a
person well represents a reasonable maximum threshold for the level of accuracy possible among strangers. We also com-
pare the degree of accuracy obtained for narcissism to the well-established accuracy correlation obtained for extraversion
judged at zero-acquaintance (i.e., from silent video clips). We chose extraversion because it is the most observable of the
traits that have been examined (John & Robins, 1993) and also the most accurately-judged (Borkenau & Liebler, 1993) , thus
providing a high benchmark. We chose silent video clips as the comparison stimulus because several zero-acquaintance
studies have examined accuracy using this stimulus and because silent video clips closely mirror our photographic stimulus
in that they eliminate information from speech patterns and vocal content. In spite of this, silent video clips are still an espe-
cially strict benchmark to meet because these stimuli afford observers other sources of information (e.g., nonverbal behav-
ior) that might improve accuracy, but that are absent from a static photograph.
Question 2: Which components of narcissism are manifested in physical appearance? Narcissism is typically conceived of as a
constellation of narrower traits, which combine to produce a unique personality construct. It is possible that some facets of
narcissism are better manifested in physical appearance than others. Thus, our second aim was to decompose narcissism into
its component parts and examine which aspects can be detected from physical appearance alone.
What are the traits that make up narcissism? These have been conceptualized in several ways: within the framework of
the Big Five personality traits (Paulhus & Williams, 2002) and as facets that can be captured by creating subscales of the NPI.
The Big Five substrates of narcissism are high levels of agentic traits, especially extraversion, and low levels of communal
1440 S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447
Author's personal copy
traits, especially agreeableness. Although not within the Big Five model of personality, explicit self-esteem is another trait
that has consistently been included as an important component of narcissism (e.g., Raskin, Novacek, & Hogan, 1991; Rho-
dewalt & Morf, 1995).
The NPI-derived facets of narcissism have been conceptualized in two ways. Emmons, 1984; Emmons, 1987) approach
derives four subscales with varying levels of adaptive value: leadership/authority (most adaptive), self-absorption/self-admi-
ration, superiority/arrogance, and exploitativeness/entitlement (most maladaptive). Similarly, Raskin and Terry (1988) iden-
tified seven subscales (authority, self-sufficiency, superiority, exhibitionism, exploitativeness, vanity, and entitlement), and
Raskin and Novacek (1989) showed that authority was the most adaptive facet and exploitativeness and entitlement were
the most maladaptive. Because the four- and seven-facet models yield similar patterns of correlations with adjustment, we
will focus only on the four-factor model because it is more parsimonious and because the reliabilities of its subscales are
higher than those of the seven-factor model.
Thus, to address our second research question, we examined the extent to which observers’ perceptions of narcissism cor-
respond with targets’ levels of extraversion, agreeableness, and self-esteem, as well as Emmons’ four facets of the NPI. By
determining which components of narcissistic personality are captured in observers’ judgments, we can gain a better under-
standing of which aspects of narcissism are expressed in physical appearance. For example, if perceivers’ impressions of nar-
cissism are associated with targets’ actual scores on the exploitativeness/entitlement subscale, it would suggest that this
maladaptive aspect of narcissism may be more easily expressed in physical appearance than other aspects of narcissism.
Question 3: What are the valid cues to narcissism and what cues do observers use to judge narcissism? If observers are able to
accurately judge narcissism based on physical appearance alone, this suggests that physical appearance contains reliable
cues to narcissism. Thus, several questions follow: How is narcissism manifested in physical appearance? And, how do
observers form accurate perceptions of narcissism based on physical appearance? Brunswik’s (1956) lens model offers a
framework in which to examine the links between narcissism and physical appearance and between physical appearance
and perceptions of narcissism. According to this model, physical appearance serves as a lens through which observers form
impressions of underlying constructs. For example, expensive clothes could serve as the lens through which observers per-
ceive a target’s narcissism.
In Brunswik’s lens model, the term ‘‘cue validity” refers to the correspondence between the physical cue (e.g., expensive
clothes) and the target’s actual disposition (e.g., level of narcissism). The term ‘‘cue utilization” refers to the correspondence
between the physical cue (e.g., expensive clothes) and the observer’s perception (e.g., of narcissism). According to the lens
model, accuracy results if observers utilize valid cues and ignore invalid cues to the targets’ narcissism. Thus, our third aim
was to examine what physical appearance cues are associated with actual narcissism (cue validity) and what cues are asso-
ciated with observers’ judgments (cue utilization). Because this is the first study to examine the physical appearance corre-
lates of narcissism, we did not form specific hypotheses about which cues would be valid and utilized. Thus, our analyses are
largely exploratory.
2. Method
We tested our three research questions in one comprehensive study involving three phases. In the first phase, we col-
lected self and informant ratings of the targets’ personalities, and photographed each target. In the second phase, a team
of observers looked at the photographs and formed impressions of the targets’ personalities, including narcissism. In the
third phase, a team of trained coders recorded the physical characteristics of the targets in the photographs (i.e., cues).
2.1. Participants
Participants were 160 students enrolled in introductory psychology at the University of Texas at Austin in the fall of 2002.
The sample was 54% female, and the average age of the participants was 18.7 years (SD = 2.0). According to self-reported
ethnicity, 56% of participants were White, 23% Asian, 12% Hispanic, 3% Black, 3% indicated ‘other’, and 3% did not report their
race. Participants completed the experiment in exchange for partial fulfillment of course requirements. Unless otherwise
noted, there were no significant sex or ethnicity differences in any of the results presented.
2.2. Self ratings
Participants completed a battery of personality measures. Narcissism was measured with the 40-item Narcissistic Person-
ality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988). Specifically, participants read each of 40 pairs of statements (one indicating low
narcissism and one indicating high narcissism) selecting the item that is closer to their own feelings about themselves. A
participant’s score on the NPI is the number of high-narcissism items they endorsed. The mean score on the NPI was
16.56 (SD = 7.56) and the alpha reliability was .87. We also computed facet scores for each of Emmons (1984), Emmons
(1987) four facets of the NPI. The alpha reliabilities for the facets were .79 for leadership/authority, .73 for self-absorp-
tion/self-admiration, .58 for superiority/arrogance, and .60 for exploitativeness/entitlement.
Extraversion and agreeableness were measured with the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008) using a 1
(‘‘Strongly disagree”) to 7 (‘‘Strongly agree”) Likert-type scale. The mean scores were 4.51 (SD = 1.06) for extraversion and
S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447 1441
Author's personal copy
5.19 (SD = .83) for agreeableness. The alpha reliability of self-ratings was .82 across the eight extraversion items and .86
across the nine agreeableness items. Self-esteem was measured using the single-item self Esteem inventory (SISE; Robins,
Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001). Specifically, participants indicated the extent to which they agreed with the statement ‘‘I
have high self-esteem” on a Likert-type scale from 1 (‘‘Disagree strongly”) to 7 (‘‘Agree strongly”). The mean score on the
SISE was 5.01 (SD = 1.51). We could not measure the reliability of this scale because it consists of only one item but previous
research shows that the test-retest reliability of this measure is about .79 (Vazire & Mehl, 2007). Consistent with previous
research (Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Raskin et al., 1991), participants’ NPI scores were related to their self-reported level of
extraversion (r= .49, p< .01), agreeableness (r=.37, p< .01), and self-esteem (r= .37, p< .01).
2.3. Informant ratings
Each participant was asked to nominate three people who knew them well to provide ratings of their personality. Infor-
mants had known the targets an average of 9.6 years (SD = 7.3), and over 94% of informants had known the target for at least
one year. Participants were told that the informants’ ratings would be kept completely confidential, and that they themselves
would never see their informants’ ratings.
Informant-ratings were collected via the Internet following recommendations by Vazire (2006). Informants were con-
tacted by e-mail and asked to complete an online questionnaire about how they see the target participant’s personality.
Informants received a link and unique identifying number in the email. Informants who did not complete the ratings were
sent reminder emails after two weeks, four weeks, and six weeks. Two months after the end of the study, 79% of informants
had completed the ratings, resulting in a total of 381 informant ratings, of which 219 (57%) were from friends, 63 (17%) from
parents, 41 (11%) from romantic partners, 46 (12%) from siblings, and 12 (3%) from others (ex-boyfriends, ex-girlfriends, and
extended family). Participants were compensated at the end of the three weeks, regardless of whether the informants had
completed their ratings. Informants were not compensated for their cooperation.
Informants completed a battery of personality measures about the target, including a 4-item observer-rating measure of
narcissism using a Likert-type scale from 1 (‘‘Not true”) to 7 (‘‘Very true”). The items on this scale are: ‘‘Always wants to be
the center of attention,” ‘‘Tends to brag,” ‘‘Thinks too much of him/herself,” and ‘‘Overestimates his/her abilities.” The infor-
mant ratings were aggregated to form a single, composite measure of well-acquainted others’ perceptions. The reliability of
the four-item narcissism measure for the composite of all three informants was ICC [1, k] = .82, and the average pairwise
informant agreement correlation for the four-item measure of narcissism was .25. The mean aggregated informant rating
on this scale was 2.54 (SD = 1.05). See Nathanson (2003) for details of convergent and discriminant validity for a measure
very similar to this one.
2.4. Observer ratings
Observers’ ratings of narcissism were based purely on photographs of the targets. Standardized photographs were taken
of each participant standing against a white wall in a bare room, containing only a camera on a tripod. The location of the
camera and the participant were fixed so that the bottom of the frame was just below the participant’s feet, ensuring that the
entire body would be captured in the photograph. Participants did not know before coming to the experiment that they
would be photographed.
Seven undergraduate research assistants served as observers. The observers were each given a CD with all targets’ pho-
tographs. They looked at the photographs of the target participants and made judgments about their personalities, including
the same 4-item observer-rating measure of narcissism completed by the informants. The observers were instructed not to
speak to each other about their ratings. The order in which the observers viewed the photographs was varied to reduce the
impact of order effects. The observers were not given any instructions about how to form impressions of the targets. They
were instructed to give their best guess as to the targets’ personalities. The observers agreed substantially in their ratings of
the targets’ narcissism. The inter-observer agreement (ICC[2, k]) for the scale scores was .71, and the average pairwise agree-
ment correlation was .28. The alpha reliability of the composite measure (averaging all four items across all seven observers)
was .89. The mean aggregate observer rating on this scale was 3.28 (SD = .71).
2.5. Coding of cues
The data in this study were part of a larger dataset designed to examine the links between personality and appearance
more generally (see Naumann et al., 2007, for a full description of the study). Thus, over 160 cues were coded from the pho-
tographs including items related to clothing, preparation, stance and emotional expression. All cues were coded by at least
two research assistants, to allow us to measure the reliability of the codings. Many cues had low reliabilities, either due to
low base rates (e.g., shirt tied around waist, hands on hips) or because the cues were hard to see in the photographs (e.g., hair
accessories, wrinkles). Thus, we eliminated more than half of the initially coded cues due to low reliability.
We selected the final set of cues using the following procedure: First, all cues with a reliability (Cronbach’s
a
) of less than
.40 were removed from the list. Second, any cue that did not correlate with either the targets’ NPI scores or the observers’
ratings of narcissism was removed (i.e., cues were kept only if they were either valid or utilized). Third, any redundant cues
with similar patterns of utilization and validity correlations were removed (e.g., ‘‘wearing eye shadow,” ‘‘wearing mascara,”
1442 S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447
Author's personal copy
‘‘wearing lipstick,” ‘‘wearing blush” and ‘‘wearing makeup” all had the same pattern of cue utilization and cue validity, so the
first four were removed and ‘‘wearing makeup” was kept). This procedure resulted in a final list of 16 cues with an average
alpha reliability of .69 (range: .41–1.00).
3. Results
Question 1: How accurate are snap judgments of narcissism? To examine the accuracy of the observers’ ratings of nar-
cissism, we correlated the aggregated observers’ ratings with the targets’ scores on the NPI. This correlation (r= .25,
p< .01) was significant and moderate in size (Hemphill, 2003), demonstrating that observers’ ratings had considerable accu-
racy. To gauge the magnitude of this accuracy correlation, we compared it to the degree of accuracy that knowledgable infor-
mants could achieve; we correlated the informants’ ratings of narcissism with the targets’ scores on the NPI (r= .39, p< .01;
Table 1). This correlation represents a reasonable maximum threshold for the level of accuracy strangers could possibly
achieve. Although the accuracy of snap judgments of narcissism was considerably lower than this benchmark (though the
difference did not reach statistical significance using Hotelling’s T-test with the Williams modification; t= 1.41, p= .16), this
benchmark helps put the accuracy of observers’ ratings into perspective.
Another way to measure the practical significance of this accuracy correlation is to compare it to the level of accuracy
obtained at ‘‘zero-acquaintance” for extraversion, the most observable of the Big Five personality traits (John & Robins,
1993). The accuracy of observers’ ratings of extraversion based on silent video clips is consistently in the range of .20–
.30, with an average of .25 across four studies (Borkenau & Liebler, 1993; Yeagley, Morling, & Nelson, 2007). In light of
the fact that extraversion is the most accurately judged of any Big Five domain at ‘‘zero-acquaintance” and that observers
in these comparison studies had more information (e.g., body movements and dynamic facial expressions) than the observ-
ers in the present study, the accuracy level for snap judgments of narcissism obtained here is particularly impressive.
Question 2: Which components of narcissism are manifested in physical appearance? To examine which components of
narcissism are best represented in physical appearance, we examined the correlation between the observers’ judgments of
narcissism and each of narcissism’s narrower facets. We started by correlating observers’ judgments of narcissism with the
targets’ extraversion, agreeableness, and self-esteem, the most well-documented trait components of narcissism. As shown
in Table 2, observers’ ratings of narcissism were associated with targets’ levels of extraversion (r= .25, p< .01) and agreeable-
ness (r=.25, p< .01), but were not significantly correlated with targets’ levels of explicit self-esteem (r= .14, n.s.).
Another model for breaking down narcissism into its narrower components is Emmons’s NPI-derived set of facets of
narcissism (Emmons, 1984; Emmons, 1987). Thus, we correlated observers’ judgments of narcissism with targets’ scores
on the four facets of the NPI. The results presented in Table 2 show that observers’ judgments captured most of the facets
of narcissism, including both the most adaptive facet, leadership/authority (r= .21, p< .01) and the least adaptive facet,
exploitativeness/entitlement (r= .19, p< .05). Not surprisingly, observers’ judgments were most strongly correlated with
the self-absorption/self-admiration facet of narcissism, the facet most associated with vanity and care for one’s appearance
Table 1
Accuracy of snap judgments of narcissism and benchmark
Accuracy (r)
Observer ratings of narcissism .25
**
Informant ratings of narcissism .39
**
Note. N = 160. Observers’ ratings are the aggregate of seven observers’ ratings based on physical appearance alone. Informant ratings are the aggregate of up
to three well-acquainted informants. Accuracy was measured by correlating judgments with targets’ NPI scores.
**
p < .01, two-tailed.
Table 2
Snap judgments of narcissism correlated with narcissism components
Observers’ judgments (r)
Extraversion .25
**
Agreeableness .25
**
Self-esteem .14
NPI subscales:
Leadership/authority .21
**
Self-absorption/self-admiration .24
**
Superiority/arrogance .12
Exploitativeness/entitlement .19
*
Note. N = 160. Observers’ ratings are the aggregate of seven observers’ ratings based on physical appearance alone. Extraversion, agreeableness, and Self-
esteem were measured using self-reports.
*
p< .05, two-tailed.
**
p< .01, two-tailed.
S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447 1443
Author's personal copy
(r= .24, p< .01). Finally, observers’ ratings of narcissism were not significantly correlated with the superiority/arrogance fa-
cet of narcissism (r= .12, n.s.).
Question 3: What are the valid cues to narcissism and what cues do observers use to judge narcissism? To understand
how observers were able to form accurate impressions of narcissism based on only a photograph, we conducted an explor-
atory Brunswik lens model analysis. Specifically, we correlated the cues with the targets’ NPI scores (to measure cue validity)
and with observers’ ratings of narcissism (to measure cue utilization). To control for mean differences between the sexes on
the cue variables all cues were standardized within sex before computing the cue validity and cue utilization correlations.
We also examined cue validity and cue utilization separately for each sex to examine whether some cues were valid or uti-
lized for only one sex. Of the 16 cues, 10 applied to both sexes, 4 applied only to females, and 2 applied only to males. These
16 cues are presented in Table 3 along with the cue validity and cue utilization correlation for each cue.
As the cue validity column in Table 3 shows, the targets’ NPI scores were associated with expensive and stylish clothes, a
neat and organized appearance, an appearance that seemed to take a lot of preparation, and attractiveness. Among females,
NPI scores were also associated with looking feminine, wearing makeup, having plucked eyebrows, and showing cleavage.
Among males, NPI scores were negatively associated with wearing eyeglasses.
As the cue utilization column in Table 3 shows, the cues associated with observers’ ratings of narcissism are largely the
same as those associated with targets’ NPI scores. However, in addition to utilizing most of the valid cues observers also ap-
peared to draw on a few cues with weak validity correlations. For example, the presence of a fraternity or sorority symbol on
the targets’ clothing was associated with observers’ ratings of narcissism, but was not strongly associated with actual NPI
scores (although there was a weak, marginally significant correlation).
Finally, we conducted exploratory analyses to examine how the cues were related to each component of narcissism exam-
ined in Question 2. Table 4 presents the correlations between each cue and each component of narcissism. The first data col-
umn of Table 4 is identical to the cue validity column in Table 3 (i.e., correlations between cues and total NPI scores). These
results show that, overall, global narcissism was more strongly related to these physical appearance cues than were any of
the individual components of narcissism. However, each component also had distinctive patterns of cue correlations. For
example, the most maladaptive component of narcissism, exploitativeness/entitlement, was the component most strongly
associated with wearing expensive clothes and, in women, showing cleavage, while extraversion and self-esteem, arguably
two of the more adaptive components of narcissism, were the components most strongly associated with cheerfulness and,
along with the leadership/authority facet, attractiveness.
4. Discussion
Our study was based on the premise that narcissism should be manifested in physical appearance. Specifically, we pre-
dicted that narcissists’ concern for their appearance, their desire to be the center of attention, and their pursuit of status
would result in a distinct physical appearance. We also predicted that this distinct physical appearance would result in
observers making accurate snap judgments of narcissism. Our findings support these predictions.
Table 3
Narcissism cue validity and cue utilization
Cue validity (r) Cue (
a
) Cue utilization (r)
Full sample (N = 160)
.16
*
Fashionable clothes (.57) .22
**
.22
**
Stylish clothes (.64) .33
**
.29
**
Expensive clothes (.41) .28
**
.18
*
Plain clothes (.81) .04
.13
Frat/sorority type (1.00) .38
**
.17
*
Organized appearance (.52) .24
**
.17
*
Neat (vs. messy) appearance (.57) .25
**
.13 Cheerful (.88) .23
**
.28
**
Amount of preparation required (.66) .34
**
.23
**
Attractiveness (.82) .44
**
Females only (N = 87)
.26
*
Feminine (vs. masculine) (.45) .40
**
.22
*
Makeup (.83) .55
**
.23
*
Plucked eyebrows (.83) .41
**
.23
*
Cleavage showing (.65) .26
*
Males only (N = 73)
.25
*
Eyeglasses (.92) .28
*
.15 Muscular (.47) .35
**
Note. Cue validity is the correlation between cue and targets’ NPI scores; Cue utilization is the correlation between cue and observers’ ratings of narcissism.
All cue variables were standardized within sex before computing the cue validity and cue utilization correlations. Cue alphas are based on two coders.
p< .10, two-tailed.
*
p< .05, two-tailed.
**
p< .01, two-tailed.
1444 S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447
Author's personal copy
Our results show that narcissism can be judged with some degree of accuracy on the basis of physical appearance alone.
Furthermore, the magnitude of this effect (r= .25) is sizeable considering how little information was available to the observ-
ers. In comparison, well-acquainted informants ratings of narcissism correlated .39 with the criterion in the present study,
and published studies of judgments based on silent video clips tend to yield accuracy correlations around .25 for extraver-
sion, the most accurately-judged of the traits commonly examined (Borkenau & Liebler, 1993; Yeagley et al., 2007). One
study examining the accuracy of personality judgments based on photographs has yielded accuracy correlations ranging
from .15 to .25 across the Big Five personality traits with an average of .19 (Robins, Gosling, & Donahue, 1998) and another
similar study yielded accuracy correlations ranging from .14 to .37 with an average of .24 (Naumann et al., 2008). Thus, it
seems narcissism can be judged at least as accurately and easily from physical appearance as any of the Big Five personality
traits.
Our analyses also show that some of the narrower traits that comprise narcissism were more clearly manifested in phys-
ical appearance than others. Specifically, narcissists’ high levels of extraversion and low levels of agreeableness seemed to
play an important role in observers’ judgments of narcissism, whereas narcissists’ high levels of explicit self-esteem did
not seem to influence observers’ judgments as much. In addition, consistent with our hypothesis that it is narcissists’ vanity
and concern about appearances that would lead to observable manifestations, observers’ judgments of narcissism were most
strongly related to the self-absorption/self-admiration facet of the NPI.
Our analyses also support our predictions about the specific ways in which narcissism would be manifested in physical
appearance. We predicted that narcissists’ concern with their appearance, their desire for attention, and their pursuit of sta-
tus would be associated with specific physical appearance cues. Consistent with this prediction, our exploratory analyses
suggest that narcissism is associated with a neat, organized appearance, flashy, revealing clothing, greater adornment
(e.g., makeup), and expensive, stylish clothes. These results suggest that narcissists do seem to alter their appearance (con-
sciously or unconsciously) in a way that reflects their appearance-oriented motives. In addition, narcissists’ distinct appear-
ance can account for the accuracy of observers’ ratings.
Finally, our findings show that judgments of narcissism based on physical appearance capture many of the distinguishing
characteristics of narcissism. Judgments of narcissism correlate with three of the four facets of the Narcissistic Personality
Inventory, and show the expected pattern of correlations with other personality traits (e.g., extraversion and agreeableness).
Thus, the image that narcissists create appears to capture the unique constellation of personality traits characteristic of
narcissism.
4.1. Implications
Our findings support existing theories of narcissism. Specifically, the field’s current understanding of narcissism entails
that narcissism is associated with vanity, exhibitionism, and status-seeking (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001; Raskin & Terry,
Table 4
Cue validity correlations between physical appearance cues and components of narcissism
NPI Ext Agr SE LA SS SA EE
Full sample (N = 160)
Fashionable clothes (.57) .16
*
.06 .14
.01 .13 .07 .05 .16
*
Stylish clothes (.64) .22
**
.24
**
.17
*
.09 .22
**
.17
*
.11 .21
**
Expensive clothes (.41) .29
**
.17
*
.04 .08 .21
**
.18
*
.17
*
.30
**
Plain clothes (.81) .18
*
.07 .03 .03 .12 .14
.29
**
.07
Frat/Sorority type (1.00) .13
.06 .21
*
.09 .13 .12 .08 .11
Organized appearance (.52) .17
*
.10 .09 .10 .12 .21
**
.02 .21
**
Neat (vs. messy) appearance (.57) .17
*
.09 .11 .14
.16
*
.21
**
.03 .13
Cheerful (.88) .13 .18
*
.03 .19
*
.13 .15
.06 .10
Amount of preparation required (.66) .28
**
.17
*
.07 .12 .22
**
.21
**
.18
*
.21
**
Attractiveness (.82) .23
**
.31
**
.12 .27
**
.30
**
.20
*
.12 .10
Females only (N = 87)
Feminine (vs. masculine) (.45) .26
*
.12 .15 .12 .20
.28
**
.12 .19
Makeup (.83) .22
*
.12 .19
.11 .19
.22
*
.09 .11
Plucked eyebrows (.83) .23
*
.14 .04 .11 .23
*
.23
*
.08 .08
Cleavage showing (.65) .23
*
.02 .30
**
.01 .21
*
.09 .09 .31
**
Males only (N = 73)
Eyeglasses (.92) .25
*
.26
*
.01 .25
*
.16 .18 .26
*
.14
Muscular (.47) .15 .03 .15 .17 .16 .15 .19 .01
Average .21 .13 .12 .12 .19 .18 .12 .15
Note. The first data column is identical to the first data column in Table 3. NPI, Narcissistic Personality Inventory total score; LA, leadership/authority
subscale of NPI; SS, self-absorption/self-admiration subscale of NPI; Ext, extraversion self-ratings on BFI; Agr, agreeableness self-ratings on BFI; SE, single-
item self-esteem self-ratings; SA, superiority/arrogance subscale of NPI; E, exploitativeness/entitlement subscale of NPI. All cue variables were standardized
within sex before computing the correlations.
p< .10, two-tailed.
*
p< .05, two-tailed.
**
p< .01, two-tailed.
S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447 1445
Author's personal copy
1988). Our findings show that these values are reflected in narcissists’ physical appearance—in the clothes they wear, the
effort they put into their appearance, and their less inhibited display of their bodies. These results provide important,
real-world evidence for prominent theories of narcissism.
In addition, our findings are consistent with recent research showing that narcissism is associated with unrestricted
sociosexuality (Foster, Shrira, & Campbell, 2006). Specifically, the cues associated with narcissism in our study are consistent
with less restricted sexual attitudes and behaviors (e.g., showing cleavage). Furthermore, the finding that observers can
judge narcissism with above-chance accuracy on the basis of physical appearance alone suggests that narcissists may use
their clothing and appearance to signal or abet their mating strategies.
Finally, the remarkable accuracy of snap judgments of narcissism likely reflects the importance of this trait in interper-
sonal interactions. Narcissism has important consequences both for narcissists and those who must interact with them. For
those who must interact with narcissists, they face a host of unpleasant or maladaptive behaviors such as self-deception
(even in the face of disconfirming evidence; Robins & John, 1997), anger, hostility, and aggression in the face of ego threat
(Stucke & Sporer, 2002), and self-enhancement and self-serving attributions (Campbell, Reider, Sedikides, & Elliot, 2000; Kurt
& Paulhus, 2008). In fact, narcissists are more likely to be disliked by peers (even after making a fleeting positive impression;
Paulhus, 1998) and often report experiencing negative interactions with friends and family members (Helgeson & Fritz,
1999). Perhaps the ability to judge narcissism quickly and accurately is in the service of avoiding such potentially unpleasant
interactions.
4.2. Limitations and future directions
We have shown that college students are able to develop fairly accurate impressions of their peers’ levels of nar-
cissism. However, we do not know whether these findings generalize to other domains and populations. For example,
how is narcissism manifested in job interviews, online interactions, or dating contexts? How accurate are perceptions
in these domains? Previous research has shown that narcissists can make positive first impressions in face-to-face set-
tings (Paulhus, 1998). Furthermore, prior research has found few valid cues to arrogance (a construct related to nar-
cissism) in verbal and nonverbal behavior (Gifford, 1994; Gifford & Hine, 1994). Together, these findings suggest that
narcissists may be able to hide their arrogance in brief interactions, or even create positive impressions using their
charm and sociability, and that narcissism may thus be easier to detect on the basis of a photograph than a face-
to-face interaction.
Another limitation of our study is that the cue analyses were exploratory in nature and should be interpreted with cau-
tion. Although the cue validity and cue utilization correlations are consistent with existing theories about narcissists’ preoc-
cupation with their appearance, we did not make specific predictions about which cues would be associated with narcissism.
By culling the cues from a broader list of 160, we left ourselves open to Type I error. Thus, it would be fruitful to replicate
these correlations in future research. This is especially true for the correlations between the cues and the individual compo-
nents of narcissism presented in Table 4.
In addition, we could not conclusively establish what cues observers actually used to form their impressions. The lens
model analyses only demonstrate that observers’ judgments were related to appearance cues, but we cannot be sure that
observers actually used those cues to judge narcissism. Similarly, we could not conclusively establish how the valid cues
came to be associated with narcissism. Do narcissists deliberately alter their appearance, and if so, what are their motives
in doing so?
In conclusion, narcissism does have a distinct physical signature, and can be detected from physical appearance alone.
Our understanding of the psychological aspects of narcissism can now be supplemented with a better understanding of
its physical manifestations. Narcissus’s obsession with his reflection was likely due in part to his egotism, but perhaps it also
had something to do with the well-groomed, well-dressed image he saw in the pond.
References
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured
intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 546–553.
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., Sedikides, C., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Narcissism and comparative self-enhancement strategies. Journal of Research in Personality,
34, 329–347.
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 291–300.
Emmons, R. A. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 11–17.
Foster, J. D., Shrira, I., & Campbell, W. K. (2006). Theoretical models of narcissism, sexuality, and relationship commitment. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 23, 367–386.
Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework for understanding the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions in nonverbal behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 398–412.
Gifford, R., & Hine, D. H. (1994). The role of verbal behavior in the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions. Journal of Research in Personality, 28,
114–132.
Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room with a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 82, 379–398.
Helgeson, V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (1999). Unmitigated agency and unmitigated communion: Distinctions from agency and communion. Journal of Research in
Personality, 33, 131–158.
Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. American Psychologist, 58, 78–80.
1446 S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447
Author's personal copy
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O.
P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1993). Determinants of interjudge agreement on personality traits: The Big Five domains, observability, evaluativeness, and the
unique perspective of the self. Journal of Personality, 61, 521–551.
Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis. New York: Guilford Pres.
Kurt, A., & Paulhus, D. L. (2008). Moderators of the adaptiveness of self-enhancement: Operationalization, motivational domain, adjustment facet, and
evaluator. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 839–853.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 177–196.
Naumann, L.P., Vazire, S., Rentfrow, P.J., & Gosling, S.D. (2008). Things often are as they appear: Expression and perception of personality from appearance.
Unpublished data, Institute of Personality and Social Research. Berkeley: University of California.
Nathanson, C. (2003). Validation of subclinical psychopathy via peer ratings and concrete behavior. Unpublished master’s thesis. Vancouver, Canada: University
of British Columbia.
Ovid (2004). Metamorphoses. (C. Martin, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. (Original work published ca. 8B. C.E.).
Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74, 1197–1208.
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36,
556–563.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890–902.
Raskin, R., & Novacek, J. (1989). An MMPI description of the narcissistic personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 66–80.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991). Narcissism, self-esteem, and defensive self-enhancement. Journal of Personality, 59, 19–38.
Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2006). Message in a ballad: The role of music preferences in interpersonal perception. Psychological Science, 17, 236–242.
Rhodewalt, F., & Morf, C. C. (1995). Self and interpersonal correlates of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory: A review and new findings. Journal of Research
in Personality, 29, 1–23.
Robins, R. W., Gosling, S. D., & Donahue, E. M. (1998). Are personality judgments based on physical appearance consensual and accurate? In J. Bermudez,B.
de Raad, A. M. Perez, A. Sanchez-Elvira, & G. Van Heck (Eds.), Recent developments in personality psychology in Europe (pp. 70–75). Tilburg, Netherlands:
Tilburg University Press.
Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and the Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 151–161.
Robins, R. W., & John, O. P. (1997). Effects of visual perspective and narcissism on self-perception: Is seeing believing? Psychological Science, 8, 37–42.
Sedikides, C., Gregg, A. P., Cisek, S., & Hart, C. M. (2007). The I that buys: Narcissists as consumers. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17, 254–257.
Stucke, T. S., & Sporer, S. L. (2002). When a grandiose self-image is threatened: Narcissism and self-concept clarity as predictors of negative emotions and
aggression following ego-threat. Journal of Personality, 70, 509–532.
Vazire, S. (2006). Informant reports: A cheap, fast, and easy method for personality assessment. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 472–481.
Vazire, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2004). e-Perceptions: Personality impressions based on personal websites. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
123–132.
Vazire, S., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Impulsivity and the self-defeating behavior of narcissists. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 154–165.
Vazire, S., & Mehl, M. R. (2007). Unpublished data.
Watson, D. (1989). Strangers’ ratings of the five robust personality factors: Evidence of a surprising convergence with self-report. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 57, 120–128.
Yeagley, E., Morling, B., & Nelson, M. (2007). Non-verbal zero-acquaintance accuracy of self-esteem, social dominance orientation, and satisfaction with life.
Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1099–1106.
S. Vazire et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1439–1447 1447
... While more anxious, emotionally unstable, and defensive women (i.e., with higher vulnerable narcissism or neuroticism) tend to use makeup to hide imperfections, more extraverted, sociable, and assertive women (i.e., with higher grandiose narcissism and extraversion) use it to attract more attention (Korichi et al., 2008). The frequency of makeup usage in women is also positively predicted by narcissism, which can be associated with a general preoccupation with good looks in a narcissistic personality (Vazire et al., 2008;Wagstaff, 2018). Makeup and other appearance modifications can thus be used as self-promotion tactics to attract attention and stand out among others . ...
... In line with this, Wagstaff (2018) found that narcissism along with mating strategies is the best predictor of makeup usage when investigating social processes, mating strategies, and personality. Not surprisingly, narcissistic individuals aim for a good physical appearance, use revealing and expensive clothing, and use more makeup (Vazire et al., 2008). They also have high concern with their appearance (Vazire et al., 2008), which may be reflected in their investment in time spent on and frequency of makeup usage. ...
... Not surprisingly, narcissistic individuals aim for a good physical appearance, use revealing and expensive clothing, and use more makeup (Vazire et al., 2008). They also have high concern with their appearance (Vazire et al., 2008), which may be reflected in their investment in time spent on and frequency of makeup usage. ...
Article
Full-text available
Appearance modification practices are ubiquitous, serving to enhance physical attractiveness and accrue social advantages, including increased desirability as a potential mate. Facial cosmetics are frequently used for appearance modification, yet individual differences in makeup usage remain understudied. While makeup usage in women has been linked to traits like narcissism and extraversion, the broader association with Dark Triad traits and how personality influences makeup application across diverse social contexts were less explored. Here we examined these relationships in 1,410 Brazilian women (Mage = 29.9, SD = 10.35), who completed online Big Five and Dark Triad personality measures, reported their usual makeup usage habits, and detailed their makeup usage across different social scenarios. Results revealed makeup usage was positively associated with narcissism and extraversion, and negatively with psychopathy. Additionally, women used less makeup when alone (at home, exercises) and more in social settings, particularly for the first date. However, women with higher narcissism and neuroticism showed larger differences among low and high makeup usage situations, while individuals with higher psychopathy used makeup in similar frequencies among different situations. Further, (1) overall greater users of makeup scored higher on narcissism, conscientiousness, and agreeableness; (2) average users displayed lower psychopathy; and (3) lesser users reported lower narcissism and higher psychopathy. These findings underscore the nuanced relationship between makeup usage and personality, particularly highlighting the influence of narcissism. These findings contribute to the interplay between personality traits and makeup usage, considering interindividual differences and intraindividual variation in understanding cosmetic behaviors among women.
... Particularly, stable traits are expressed in physical appearance cues (e.g., Back, Schmukle, & Egloff, 2010;Hirschmüller, Egloff, Schmukle, Nestler, & Back, 2015;Vazire, Naumann, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2008). For instance, conscientious individuals tend to show a formal and ordinary appearance (Borkenau & Liebler, 1992;Naumann, Vazire, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2009). ...
... With this study, we provide initial insights into valid contextual virtual meeting cues contributing to the research stream of trait expression across various information bases, such as the physical appearance (e.g., Borkenau & Liebler, 1992;Naumann et al., 2009;Vazire et al., 2008) or personal spaces (e.g., Gosling et al., 2002), including business-related information bases like application photographs (e.g., Fernandez et al., 2017), resumés (e.g., Burns et al., 2014;Cole et al., 2003), and LinkedIn profiles (e.g., Fernandez et al., 2021;Roulin & Levashina, 2019). ...
... Conversely, we found narcissists to prioritize self-illumination, potentially driven by their vanity (e.g., Egan & McCorkindale, 2007) and the external attention they pursue (e.g., Wallace & Baumeister, 2002). They also seem to feature professional technical equipment, exemplified by using external microphones and overear headphones, potentially reflecting their willingness to invest resources in shaping a favorable virtual presencemirroring a broader pattern seen in narcissists' consumer behavior (e.g., Cisek et al., 2014) and efforts to enhance their outer appearance (e.g., Holtzman & Strube, 2013;Vazire et al., 2008). ...
Article
Virtual meetings represent a key communication channel in organizations, introducing a range of contextual cues (e.g., background objects, lighting, positioning). Building on the lens model, we examine whether contextual virtual meeting cues signal traits of the displayer (big five, narcissism, intelligence) and are used to form interpersonal impressions (assertive, trustworthy, calm, competent). We assessed personality and ability tests of 362 participants (mostly business students) engaging in an assessment center group task via Zoom. Based on a set of 26 contextual virtual meeting cues, we identified valid cues signaling each of the traits and utilized cues for forming each of the interpersonal impressions, except for being seen as calm. Whereas contextual virtual meeting cues explained significant variance beyond basic demographics in traits and impressions, cue validities and utilizations were only moderate (r < .19), as were the incremental explained variances (R2 < .12). Overall, there seems to be some substance to the notion that traits can manifest through contextual virtual meeting cues and that these cues may shape impression formation. We contribute to the literature on trait expression and impression formation in virtual business meetings and offer practical implications for organizations and meeting attendees aiming to manage their virtual impressions.
... Particularly, stable traits are expressed in physical appearance cues (e.g., Back, Schmukle, & Egloff, 2010;Hirschmüller, Egloff, Schmukle, Nestler, & Back, 2015;Vazire, Naumann, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2008). For instance, conscientious individuals tend to show a formal and ordinary appearance (Borkenau & Liebler, 1992;Naumann, Vazire, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2009). ...
... With this study, we provide initial insights into valid contextual virtual meeting cues contributing to the research stream of trait expression across various information bases, such as the physical appearance (e.g., Borkenau & Liebler, 1992;Naumann et al., 2009;Vazire et al., 2008) or personal spaces (e.g., Gosling et al., 2002), including business-related information bases like application photographs (e.g., Fernandez et al., 2017), resumés (e.g., Burns et al., 2014;Cole et al., 2003), and LinkedIn profiles (e.g., Fernandez et al., 2021;Roulin & Levashina, 2019). ...
... Conversely, we found narcissists to prioritize self-illumination, potentially driven by their vanity (e.g., Egan & McCorkindale, 2007) and the external attention they pursue (e.g., Wallace & Baumeister, 2002). They also seem to feature professional technical equipment, exemplified by using external microphones and overear headphones, potentially reflecting their willingness to invest resources in shaping a favorable virtual presencemirroring a broader pattern seen in narcissists' consumer behavior (e.g., Cisek et al., 2014) and efforts to enhance their outer appearance (e.g., Holtzman & Strube, 2013;Vazire et al., 2008). ...
Conference Paper
Virtual meetings represent a key communication channel in organizations, introducing a range of contextual cues (e.g., background objects, lighting, positioning). Building on the lens model, we examine whether contextual virtual meeting cues signal traits of the displayer (big five, narcissism, intelligence) and are used to form interpersonal impressions (assertive, trustworthy, calm, competent). We assessed personality and ability tests of 362 participants (mostly business students) engaging in an assessment center group task via Zoom. Based on a set of 26 contextual virtual meeting cues, we identified valid cues signaling each of the traits and utilized cues for forming each of the interpersonal impressions, except for being seen as calm. Whereas contextual virtual meeting cues explained significant variance beyond basic demographics in traits and impressions, cue validities and utilizations were only moderate (r ≤ .18), as were the incremental explained variances (ΔR2 ≤ .11). Overall, there seems to be some substance to the notion that traits can manifest through contextual virtual meeting cues and that these cues may shape impression formation. We contribute to the literature on trait expression and impression formation in virtual business meetings and offer practical implications for organizations and meeting attendees aiming to manage their virtual impressions. ***This paper has been nominated among the three finalists for the Best Paper Award of the Organisational Behaviour General Track – SIG 09 OB & HRM at the 2024 EURAM Conference.***
... More frequent sexual activity would then drive self-inferences of high sex drive. Narcissists are also known to invest in their appearance, thereby promoting actual sexual attractiveness (Holtzman & Strube, 2010;Vazire et al., 2008). The ensuing dating success and accompanying sexual activity would again promote the inference of high sex drive (O'Connor et al., 2020). ...
... For example, narcissism often manifests in physical appearance cues (e.g. professional/formal attire; Vazire et al., 2008) and indicators of online socializing behaviour (e.g. more contacts/frequent posting; Gnambs & Appel, 2018;McCain & Campbell, 2018), but less so on LinkedIn. LinkedIn's professional norms -maintaining a formal appearance, building a professional network, and posting business content -may render these cues less discriminative (Hartwell & Campion, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recruiters routinely use LinkedIn profiles to infer applicants' individual traits like narcissism and intelligence, two key traits in online network and organizational contexts. However, little is known about LinkedIn profiles' predictive potential to accurately infer individual traits. According to Brunswik's lens model, accurate trait inferences depend on (a) the presence of valid cues in LinkedIn profiles containing information about users' individual traits and (b) the sensitive and consistent utilization of valid cues. We assessed narcissism (self-report) and intelligence (aptitude tests) in a sample of 406 LinkedIn users along with 64 LinkedIn cues (coded by three trained coders) that we derived from trait theory and previous empirical findings. We used a transparent, easy-to-interpret machine learning algorithm leveraging practical application potentials (elastic net) and applied state-of-the-art resampling techniques (nested cross-validation) to ensure robust results. Thereby, we uncover LinkedIn profiles' predictive potential: (a) LinkedIn profiles contain valid information about narcissism (e.g. uploading a background picture) and intelligence (e.g. listing many accomplishments), and (b) the elastic nets sensitively and consistently using these valid cues attain prediction accuracy (r = .35/.41 for narcissism/intelligence). The results have practical implications for improving recruiters' accuracy and foreshadow potentials and limitations of automated LinkedIn-based assessments for selection purposes.
Chapter
Full-text available
Spor Bilimleri Alanında Akademik Araştırma ve Değerlendirmeler II başlıklı bu kitap, spor bilimleri alanındaki akademik çalışmaları kapsamlı ve disiplinlerarası bir yaklaşımla bir araya getiren bir koleksiyondur. Kitap, spor bilimlerinin çeşitli yönlerine dair en son araştırmaları ve güncel gelişmeleri içermektedir. Her bölüm, alanında uzman kişiler tarafından titizlikle hazırlanmış olup, spor fizyolojisi, biyomekanik, psikoloji ve spor yönetimi gibi çeşitli konulara dair derinlemesine bilgiler sunmaktadır. Dahil edilen çalışmalar, teori ve pratik arasındaki boşluğu doldurmayı hedefleyerek literatüre değerli katkılarda bulunmakta ve spor bilimleri anlayışımızı ileriye taşımaktadır. Bu kitap aracılığıyla, sadece son gelişmeleri vurgulamakla kalmayıp, aynı zamanda spor bilimleri alanındaki ortaya çıkan zorlukları da ele alan zengin bir bilgi mozaiği sunmayı amaçlıyoruz. Bu koleksiyon, spor bilimlerinin dinamik ve gelişen manzarasını keşfetmek isteyen akademisyenler, araştırmacılar, uygulayıcılar ve öğrenciler için hazırlanmıştır.
Article
Purpose Counterproductive work behavior (CWB) imposes significant costs on organizations, thus antecedents of CWB are of particular interest to both practitioners and academics. The authors examine how one’s own narcissism interacts with co-worker narcissism to influence willingness to engage in retaliatory CWB against a co-worker. Design/methodology/approach The data for this study were obtained from Amazon Mechanical Turk participants and Master of Business Administration students, representing a cross-section of employee representatives. Findings The authors find that employees expect narcissistic co-workers to engage in continuing future CWB and this, in turn, increases employees' willingness to engage in retaliatory CWB. That is, non-narcissistic employees are provoked to engage in organizationally-destructive behaviors by peers perceived as narcissists. This affect is attenuated by the employee’s own narcissism. Relative to non-narcissists, narcissistic employees find a narcissistic co-worker more likeable, which reduces their willingness to engage in retaliatory CWB against the co-worker. Practical implications For corporations and HR managers, this study demonstrates the caution necessary when considering hiring and operational practices. Specifically, non-narcissists demonstrate increased willingness to engage in organizationally-destructive behaviors after interpersonal conflict with a narcissistic co-worker. Originality/value The authors extend prior research about interpersonal drivers of CWB, which primarily considered superior-subordinate dyad, by examining the joint effects of individual and co-worker narcissism in peer-to-peer relationships.
Article
The Dark Triad (DT) is a set of personality traits consisting of subclinical psychopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism. Separately, The Red Pill (TRP) is a seduction community part of an online men's movement that advocates incorporating DT traits and behaviors in romantic or sexual relationships with women. This study investigates the potential presence of DT influences in relationships with TRP‐affiliated men as described by former romantic partners. Using a directed content analysis of six semi‐structured interviews, four thematic categories emerged: relationship development , coaxing , outward appearances , and DT‐associated Internet behaviors . TRP men consistently displayed self‐interest and willingness to use manipulation to meet their needs, portraying themselves to their partners and the world as successful while callously disregarding partners' emotions in their pursuit of sex or social desirability. Participants' experiences of TRP men's behaviors aligned with prior knowledge regarding DT‐associated romantic and sexual relationships. This research may be used to inform future research connecting DT personalities and maladaptive social groups, as well as generating prevention strategies among relationship‐based practitioners.
Article
Full-text available
Although the relationship between social media and narcissismin adolescents has been widely studied, the direction of this linkremains unclear to date. This is striking given the potentialdeconstructive consequences of narcissism for adolescents’peer relationships. Therefore, this three-wave panel study(NW1 = 1032) aimed to examine whether increases in adoles-cents’ narcissism levels are (reciprocally) related to increases inposting positive self-presentations on the one hand, and todecreases in peer attachment on the other hand. Althoughnarcissism was significantly and positively related to both post-ing positive self-presentations and peer attachment at thebetween-person level, these findings were not replicated atthe within-person level. At the latter level, only the cross-lagged relationship of narcissism predicting an adolescent’stendency to post appearance-related self-presentations onsocial media was significant. Multiple group tests for sex andreceiving positive feedback on one’s self-presentations showedno differences in these within-person associations.
Article
Full-text available
The authors articulate a model specifying links between (a) individuals and the physical environments they occupy and (b) the environments and observers' impressions of the occupants. Two studies examined the basic phenomena underlying this model: Interobserver consensus, observer accuracy, cue utilization, and cue validity. Observer ratings based purely on offices or bedrooms were compared with self-and peer ratings of occupants and with physical features of the environments. Findings, which varied slightly across contexts and traits, suggest that (a) personal environments elicit similar impressions from independent observers, (b) observer impressions show some accuracy, (c) observers rely on valid cues in the rooms to form impressions of occupants, and (d) sex and race stereotypes partially mediate observer consensus and accuracy. Consensus and accuracy correlations were generally stronger than those found in zero-acquaintance research.
Article
Full-text available
Reactions to trait self-enhancers were investigated in 2 longitudinal studies of person.perception in discussion groups. Groups of 4-6 participants met 7 times for 20 rain. After Meetings 1 and 7, group members rated their perceptions of one another. In Study 1, trait self-enhancement was indexed by measures of narcissism and self-deceptive enhancement. At the first meeting, self-enhancers made positive impressions: They were seen as agreeable, well adjusted, and competent. After 7 weeks, however, they were rated negatively and gave self-evaluations discrepant with peer evaluations they received. In Study 2, an independent sample of observers (close acquaintances) enabled a pretest index of discrepancy self-enhancement: It predicted the same deteriorating pattern of interpersonal perceptions as the other three trait measures. Nonetheless, all self-enhancement measures correlated positively with self-esteem.
Article
Full-text available
Four studies examined the construct validity of two global self-esteem measures. In Studies 1 through 3, the Single-Item Self-Esteem Scale (SISE) and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) showed strong convergent validity for men and women, for different ethnic groups, and for both college students and community members. The SISE and the RSE had nearly identical correlations with a wide range of criterion measures, including domain-specific self-evaluations, self-evaluative biases, social desirability, personality, psychological and physical health, peer ratings of group behavior, academic outcomes, and demographic variables. Study 4 showed that the SISE had only moderate convergent validity in a sample of children. Overall, the findings support the reliability and validity of the SISE and suggest it can provide a practical alternative to the RSE in adult samples. More generally, the findings contribute to the research literature by further elaborating the nomological network of global self-esteem.
Article
Full-text available
Which people are most swayed by self-image motives and hence most likely to make consumer choices in line with those motives? This article contends that the answer is narcissists-individuals who see themselves, and who want others to see them, as special, superior, 14 and entitled and who are prone to exhibitionism and vanity. This work hypothesizes that narcissists will, to validate their excessively positive self-views, strive to purchase the high-prestige products (i.e., expensive, exclusive, new, and flashy). In so doing, they will regulate their own esteem by increasing their apparent status and consequently earning others' admiration and envy. This article also hypothesizes that narcissists will show greater interest in the symbolic than utilitarian value of products and will exhibit, even controlling for self-esteem, more pronounced self-enhancement phenomena such as endowment and self-signaling effects.
Article
Which people are most swayed by self‐image motives and hence most likely to make consumer choices in line with those motives? This article contends that the answer is narcissists—individuals who see themselves, and who want others to see them, as special, superior, and entitled and who are prone to exhibitionism and vanity. This work hypothesizes that narcissists will, to validate their excessively positive self‐views, strive to purchase the high‐prestige products (i.e., expensive, exclusive, new, and flashy). In so doing, they will regulate their own esteem by increasing their apparent status and consequently earning others’ admiration and envy. This article also hypothesizes that narcissists will show greater interest in the symbolic than utilitarian value of products and will exhibit, even controlling for self‐esteem, more pronounced self‐enhancement phenomena such as endowment and self‐signaling effects.
Article
The role of verbal behavior in the encoding and decoding of eight interpersonal dispositions was investigated using the lens-mapping approach (Brunswik, 1956; Gifford, 1991). Sixty participants provided self-ratings on eight dispositional scales and, in groups of three, were videotaped in conversation. The conversations were transcribed and 10 verbal behaviors were scored from the transcripts. Observers read the transcripts and rated each of the original participants on the same eight dispositions. Encoding, analyzed conservatively, tended to be weaker than decoding but did account for up to 20 % of the variance in self-reported dispositions. Ten matched links were associated with self–other agreement correlations for extraversion and aloofness. The results suggest that the lens-mapping approach is a promising method for specifying the observable behaviors that underlie self–other agreement.
Article
The purpose of this paper is to establish the importance of distinguishing unmitigated agency and unmitigated communion from agency and communion. First, we examine the empirical overlap and distinctions among the four constructs. Then, we demonstrate the differential association of unmitigated agency, unmitigated communion, agency, and communion to relationship and health outcomes. We conclude that only unmitigated agency and unmitigated communion are associated with relationship difficulties and poor health. Finally, we distinguish between the difficulties of the unmitigated agency and the unmitigated communion individual by focusing on interpersonal problems.
Article
Seven basic research questions in interpersonal perception are posed concerning issues of consensus, assimilation, reciprocity, accuracy, congruence, assumed similarity and self—other agreement. All questions can be addressed at the individual level, and three at the dyadic level. It is shown how the Social Relations Model can be used to answer the questions.