ArticlePDF Available

Rumble vocalizations mediate interpartner distance in African elephants, Loxodonta africana

Authors:
  • Proyecto Titi, Inc

Abstract and Figures

The ability to utilize contact calls to facilitate reunions with social partners has been documented in a number of species showing a fission/fusion social organization. Field observations and playback experiments suggest that African elephants use low-frequency rumble vocalizations to reunite with their herd members following periods of fission. Using a digital audio and GPS recording collar system, we documented the production of rumbles and subsequent movements of five adult female African elephants at Disney's Animal Kingdom, Bay Lake, Florida, U.S.A. This recording system allowed us to identify the producer of each rumble and to document the effect of rumbles on the movements of herd members relative to the caller. Our findings provide the first empirical evidence that spontaneously produced elephant rumble vocalizations function in part to mediate the spatial relationships of group members. Overall, the production of rumbles resulted in a net decrease in distance between the caller and her social partners. This approach behaviour was enhanced if the partner was highly affiliated with the caller, if the partner replied with a rumble of her own, and if the pair was initially far apart (≥61 m). Rumble production was likely to result in avoidance behaviour only when there was no rumble reply by the partner and the dyad was close together prior to the initial call. These results suggest that a general function of elephant rumbles is to promote spatial cohesion among separated group members, but they may also mediate a variety of other close-distance social interactions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A
vailable online at www.sciencedirect.com
Rumble vocalizations mediate interpartner distance in African
elephants, Loxodonta africana
KATHERINE A. LEIGHTY, JOSEPH SOLTIS, CHRISTINA M. WESOLEK & ANNE SAVAGE
Education & Science, Disney’s Animal Kingdom
(Received 30 January 2008; initial acceptance 11 March 2008;
final acceptance 7 May 2008; published online 29 August 2008; MS. number: A08-00062R)
The ability to utilize contact calls to facilitate reunions with social partners has been documented in a num-
ber of species showing a fission/fusion social organization. Field observations and playback experiments
suggest that African elephants use low-frequency rumble vocalizations to reunite with their herd members
following periods of fission. Using a digital audio and GPS recording collar system, we documented the
production of rumbles and subsequent movements of five adult female African elephants at Disney’s
Animal Kingdom, Bay Lake, Florida, U.S.A. This recording system allowed us to identify the producer of
each rumble and to document the effect of rumbles on the movements of herd members relative to the
caller. Our findings provide the first empirical evidence that spontaneously produced elephant rumble vo-
calizations function in part to mediate the spatial relationships of group members. Overall, the production
of rumbles resulted in a net decrease in distance between the caller and her social partners. This approach
behaviour was enhanced if the partner was highly affiliated with the caller, if the partner replied with
a rumble of her own, and if the pair was initially far apart (61 m). Rumble production was likely to result
in avoidance behaviour only when there was no rumble reply by the partner and the dyad was close
together prior to the initial call. These results suggest that a general function of elephant rumbles is to
promote spatial cohesion among separated group members, but they may also mediate a variety of other
close-distance social interactions.
Ó2008 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: African elephant; antiphonal exchange; contact call; fission/fusion; Loxodonta africana; rumble; vocal
communication
The vocalizations of animals can function to mediate
proximity both within and between social groupings.
Specifically, the production of sounds by socially living
animals can be used to pronounce territorial boundaries,
advertise sexual states, alert others to the presence of
predators, note the location of resources and promote
group cohesion (reviewed in: Boinski & Campbell 1996;
Boinski & Garber 2000). As social groups move within
their environment, individuals can become spatially sepa-
rated and move beyond the range of visual contact. Con-
tact call vocalizations provide a means by which animals
can maintain contact with group members over both
short and long distances (Oda 1996; Ramos-Fernandez
2005). Studies have documented the presence of contact
calling in a wide variety of species. These observations
note an attraction of group members to contact calls, an
increased likelihood of contact calling when animals
become separated from their group and/or as the group
becomes more dispersed, and an increased frequency of
contact calling during reunions as compared to disper-
sions (zebra finches, Poephilia guttata:Butterfield 1970;
Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis:Smith 1972;
greater spear-nosed bats, Phyllostomus hastatus:Wilkinson
& Boughman 1998; mongooses, Helogale parvula:Estes
1991; golden brown mouse lemurs, Microcebus ravelobensis:
Braune et al. 2005; ringtailed lemurs, Lemur catta:Oda
1996; squirrel monkeys, Saimiri oerstedi:Boinski 1991).
Perhaps the clearest examples of spatial separations of
social groups occur within ‘fission/fusion’ societies.
These groups show fluid association patterns while
group members maintain permanent social relationships
Correspondence: K. A. Leighty, Animal Programs Administration, P.O.
Box 10000, Lake Buena Vista, FL 32830, U.S.A. (email: katherine.
leighty@disney.com).
1601
0003e3472/08/$34.00/0 Ó2008 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
AN IM AL BE HA VI OU R , 2008, 76, 1601e1608
doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.022
(Ramos-Fernandez 2005). This social system is typified by
groups breaking into smaller subgroupings to seek out dis-
persed resources and then reuniting after a period of time.
Contact calls appear to be utilized by many species living
in fission/fusion societies to facilitate this reunion or ‘fu-
sion’ of their subgroupings. In primates, Guinea baboon,
Papio papio, subgroups have been observed to exchange
bark vocalizations when dispersed on the savanna until
the point in time when they reunite (Byrne 1981). Male
chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, are more likely to produce
long-distance pant-hoot vocalizations when preferred
social partners are nearby (but out of visual range) rather
than absent or in close proximity (Mitani & Nishida
1993). The ‘whinnies’ of spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi,
have been observed to be exchanged by affiliated individ-
uals when their subgroups are approaching (Teixidor &
Byrne 1999; Ramos-Fernandez 2005). In the fission/fusion
societies of social carnivores, contact calls are produced
less frequently, perhaps to avoid alerting prey species to
their presence (Holekamp et al. 2000). Lions, Panthera
leo, and coatis, Nasua nasua, will increase contact call rates
when travelling and foraging in conditions of low visibil-
ity (Kaufmann 1962; Schaller 1972). Spotted hyaenas,
Crocuta crocuta, on the other hand are highly vocal and
their whoop vocalization serves as a long-distance contact
call to which offspring and relatives quickly approach
(Estes 1991). Finally, among marine mammals, dolphins
are thought to use whistles to maintain contact with
pod members (Tursiops truncates:Caldwell & Caldwell
1965;Stenella longirostris:Lammers et al. 2006).
African elephants show a classic fission/fusion social
system. They live in matriarchical family groups of related
adult females and their dependent offspring. These groups
frequently and consistently associate with other family
groups to form larger ‘bond groups’ (Douglas-Hamilton
1972; Moss & Poole 1983; Wittemyer et al. 2005; Wit-
temyer & Getz 2007). The movements of family groups
of a shared bond group have been documented to be coor-
dinated, even when separated by several kilometres on the
savanna (Martin 1978; Poole et al. 1988). These groups
show parallel movement, synchronous direction changes
and mutual approach over large distances (Martin 1978).
It has been proposed that family groups coordinate their
movements by using low-frequency ‘rumble’ vocalizations
as contact calls (Poole et al. 1988; Langbauer et al. 1991).
Rumbles are the most frequent vocalization of elephants
and their structure affords long-distance propagation
(Langbauer et al. 1991; Langbauer 2000). Elephant rum-
bles are termed ‘infrasonic’ because their fundamental
frequency ranges between 13 and 35 Hz (Poole et al.
1988) and they experience little environmental attenua-
tion because of their low-frequency nature. In fact, field
experiments have documented that elephants detect and
respond to playbacks of rumble vocalizations at distances
of over 2 km (Langbauer et al. 1991; McComb et al. 2003).
Furthermore, McComb et al. (2003) found that elephants
are capable of recognizing a rumble as that of a bond or
family group member from distances of 2.5 km, although
discrimination was more accurate at 1e1.5 km.
Detailed study of the functions of elephant rumbles has
been challenging in the wild because the infrasonic nature
of these vocalizations makes it nearly impossible for
human observers to detect and localize the source of
individual rumbles (Langbauer 2000). The use of a collar-
mounted audio-recording system in the captive herds at
Disney’s Animal Kingdom, Bay Lake, Florida, U.S.A., has
allowed for both the detailed description of the acoustic
characteristics of these vocalizations and the determina-
tion of the identity of each rumble’s producer (see Leong
et al. 2003; Soltis et al. 2005a). Early analyses of data cap-
tured via this audio collar system revealed the presence of
clusters of rumble vocalizations (Leong et al. 2003). These
rumble clusters consisted of antiphonal call and reply
sequences between social partners (Leong et al. 2003;
Soltis et al. 2005a). These antiphonal sequences were
most likely to take place between individuals that were
highly affiliated (defined by time spent in close proxim-
ity), even when one individual had moved out of the vi-
sual range at the time of the vocal exchange (Soltis et al.
2005a; Leighty et al. 2008). Furthermore, both Clemins
et al. (2005) and Soltis et al. (2005b) found that adult
females produce acoustically distinctive rumble vocaliza-
tions. Thus, these findings appear to complement observa-
tions of vocal recognition made in the wild, in that
elephants have acoustically distinct rumbles and they
preferentially respond to the rumbles of their affiliated
partners, even when they are out of visual contact
(McComb et al. 2003).
In the present study we examined the effect of elephant
rumble vocalizations on the spatial relationships of group
members. Each member of our herd was outfitted with
a digital audio-recording collar that documented its
rumble production as well as its subsequent movements
using an on-board Global Positioning System (GPS). We
sought to determine whether the production of rumble
vocalizations resulted in attraction or avoidance, or had
a random effect on the subsequent distance from other
group members. We explored three factors that were likely
to influence proximity after a rumble was produced: the
partner’s degree of affiliation with the caller, the pro-
duction of an antiphonal reply rumble by the partner and
the initial interpartner distance of the dyad prior to the
call. Data presented here represent the first systematic
study of spontaneously produced elephant rumble vocal-
izations and their influence on the subsequent move-
ments of group members.
METHODS
Subjects and Housing
A herd of five unrelated adult female African elephants
(three with dependent calves), housed at Disney’s Animal
Kingdom, Bay Lake, FL, U.S.A., served as subjects of this
study (estimated ages 20e27 years) (Table 1; also see Soltis
et al. 2005a). Data presented here were collected between
January and November 2006. Animals were maintained in
two connected outdoor yards (1.639 ha and 2.321 ha) as
a single herd during the day and returned to the barn at
night. These naturalistic outdoor yards contain pools, in
which multiple adult elephants can fully submerge, mud
wallows, multiple scratching surfaces, and large rock rings
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 76,5
1602
to allow animals to move out of visual contact of other
herd members.
Audio and GPS Data Acquisition
Each of the five adult females was outfitted with a collar-
mounted data acquisition system. The system was de-
signed in 2005 by Brian Walter’s of Walt Disney World
Ride & Show Engineering (Bay Lake, FL, U.S.A.) and
constructed by Acumen Instruments Corporation (Ames,
IA, U.S.A.). The collars were constructed of fire hose (2.24e
2.77 m) and weighed between 4.0 and 4.5 kg. Collars were
fastened using a magnetic lock and key system that pro-
vided durability while allowing for easy removal. Mounted
to each of the collars was an 18 12 10 cm metal water-
tight containment box that housed both digital audio and
GPS recording units powered by two serial 6 V lead-acid
batteries. The recording of audio and GPS data was con-
trolled via a microprocessor and a compact flash card
that also served as the data storage device. One hour of
synchronized audio and GPS data was collected between
1200 and 1700 hours, two times per week, with a total
of 40 observation hours used in these analyses.
Audio data was recorded with a Knowles MR-8406
waterproof microphone that was exposed to the air in
an opening in the metal containment box. The acquired
audio signal was run through an amplifier and an 8-pole
Butterworth low-pass filter with a cut-off frequency of
5 kHz. The signal was then digitized at a sample rate of
10 kHz (Texas Instruments ADS320 audio-digital con-
verter) and saved as an uncompressed wav file to the
compact flash card. The audio device had a flat frequency
response (3 dB) between 8 Hz and 4.5 kHz. The GPS
device (Xemics no. RGPSM002, Camarillo, CA, U.S.A.)
acquired data via a satellite antenna embedded in the
fire hose with the internal receiver positioned on the
back of the elephant’s neck. The device was programmed
to collect location data every 10 s during the observation
period (accuracy 5 m).
All elephants were trained using operant conditioning
to engage in collaring and decollaring procedures. Collars
were put on the elephants before their release into the
outdoor yards as part of their morning husbandry routine
and removed at night to allow for battery recharging.
Upon removal of the collar, we downloaded the contents
of each compact flash card for analysis. This research was
approved by Disney’s Animal Care and Welfare Commit-
tee/IACUC in July of 1998 with the most recent adden-
dum approved in April of 2005.
Video Documentation of Behaviour
During each 1 h observation period, four camera per-
sons were positioned around the elephant yard to record
behaviour. At the onset of each observation, an airhorn
was sounded, and subsequent GPS time was recorded to
allow for the synchronization of the video, audio and
GPS signals during data analysis. Each camera (Panasonic
MiniDV camcorder no. PV-DV402) was then assigned a fo-
cal animal based on its location relative to each subject in
the enclosure. Cameras remained on the focal animals
until the animals moved out of view, at which point the
cameras were switched to alternate, visible animals. All an-
imals were recorded for approximately equal periods over
the course of the study. At the conclusion of each observa-
tion period, the digital video from each camera was trans-
ferred to computer and rendered as a quad-split digital
video file using Adobe Premiere Pro (Adobe Systems Incor-
porated, San Jose, CA, U.S.A.; version 2.0) to allow for
improved viewing of the herd during behavioural analysis.
An instantaneous scan sampling technique with a 5 min
interval was used on each of the 40 1-hour quad-split
videos to determine the overall affiliation of each dyad.
Affiliation was defined as the percentage of time that the
dyad spent in proximity. Individuals were defined to be
in ‘proximity’ when they were within 8 m (approximately
two elephant body lengths) of each other (see Soltis et al.
2005a). The percentage of time spent in proximity was de-
fined as the number of scans in which the dyad was in
proximity divided by the total number of scans in which
both members of the dyad were visible on the quad-split
video, multiplied by 100. The total number of samples in
which dyads were visible ranged from 299 to 333.
Rumble Localization and Identity
Determination
Rumble vocalizations were identified via visual inspec-
tion of the audio streams downloaded from the collars in
Adobe Audition (Adobe Systems Incorporated; version
1.5). The identity of the producer of the vocalization
was determined in two ways. First, rumbles that appeared
in the audio stream of a single collar were assigned as
being produced by the animal wearing that collar. Second,
since data collection on all collars was synchronized via
the satellite clock acquired by the GPS system, rumbles
that were recorded by multiple collars were assigned to the
individual on whose collar the vocalization had the
highest relative amplitude.
Antiphonal Exchanges
Antiphonal exchanges of rumble vocalizations were
defined using a 30 s window between the offset of the ini-
tial call and onset of the antiphonal reply (see Soltis et al.
2005a; Leighty et al. 2008). In these analyses, we only
used rumbles that were answered by at least one other
group member so that the effects of rumbles that were
answered could be compared to nonanswered rumbles.
Detailed examination of these antiphonal exchanges was
Table 1. Subject information
Subject
Estimated
DOB
Earliest known
origin
Date of arrival
at Disney’s
Animal Kingdom
Thandi 1981 Zimbabwe 25 Jun 1997
Moyo 1981 Zimbabwe 25 Jun 1997
Fiki 1979 Zimbabwe 4 Oct 1997
Vasha 1986 Zimbabwe 7 Nov 2000
Donna 1984 Africa 24 Nov 2003
LEIGHTY ET AL.: VOCALIZATIONS MEDIATE INTERPARTNER DISTANCE 1603
carried out by randomly selecting 30 initial calls from each
female (with the exception of Vasha who provided 20
because her rumbles were less frequently answered) and
documenting the females that produced an antiphonal
reply to that call within the defined 30 s window. The
140 initial calls selected for analysis resulted in a total of
187 unique antiphonal replies across group members.
Movements Surrounding Antiphonal
Exchanges
We used GPS technology supplemented with digital
video data to assess the movements of all females in the
herd relative to the producer of the initial call. To do this,
we documented the GPS reading of all herd members 60 s
before the production of the initial call, at the onset of the
call, as well as 60 and 120 s following this call. We then
calculated the Euclidean distance in metres between the
producer of the initial call and all other females at each
of these time markers. In those instances in which a GPS
unit did not have a valid reading at the necessary time
marker (i.e. poor satellite connection due to dense cloud
cover or positioning underwater or beneath deep cover),
we determined the interanimal distance using the quad-
split digital video files. In these cases, animals were located
on the playback screen and their positions were mapped
to a geo-referenced aerial image of the enclosures from
which distance could be extracted.
To quantify approach and avoidance movements within
each caller/partner dyad, we calculated a movement ratio
for each dyad: Movement Ratio ¼Interpartner distance
60 s after initial call/Interpartner distance at initial call on-
set. The same calculation was made using the interpartner
distance observed 120 s after the initial call. These ratios
give the proportional distance between partners at 60
and 120 s following the initial call relative to the distance
between them at the time the call was produced. For
example, doubling the initial distance yields a score of
2.0 and halving the distance yields a score of 0.5 (no
change ¼1.0). We performed a negative log-tranforma-
tion (log(x)(1)) of these ratios that resulted in the follow-
ing desired properties: distributions became normal,
increases in interpartner distance yielded negative values
(‘avoidance’), decreases in interpartner distance yielded
positive values (‘approach’), and avoidances and ap-
proaches of equal magnitude received identical scores
with opposite signs (e.g. doubling the distance results in
a movement ratio of 0.301 and halving the distance
yields an movement ratio of þ0.301).
Statistical Analyses
A three-way univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA)
was used to determine the effect of social affiliation of the
dyad, antiphonal reply production by the partner and the
dyad’s interpartner distance before the initial call on their
log-transformed movement ratios at both 60 and 120 s fol-
lowing the call. Social affiliation was defined according to
each dyad’s percentage of time spent in proximity (as de-
fined above). Dyads were categorized as ‘High affiliation’
if they were observed to spend 10% or more of their time
in proximity, ‘Moderate affiliation’ if they spent between
3 and 10% of their time in proximity, and ‘Low affiliation’
if they spent less than 3% if their time in proximity (see
Table 2). For each initial call, the antiphonal reply produc-
tion of each caller/partner dyad was categorized as ‘Reply
produced’ if the partner produced a rumble within 30 s
of the offset of the initial call, and ‘No reply’ if they did
not rumble during the 30 s window. Finally, the interpart-
ner distance of caller/partner dyads 60 s before the initial
call was classified as ‘Close’ or ‘Far’ based on a median-split
of these scores (median interpartner distance ¼61 m;
range 1e337 m). Inclusion of this factor allowed us to dis-
tinguish between the interactions of relatively close and
distant dyads. ANOVAs were calculated using SPSS (version
15.0, Chicago, Illinois, U.S.A.) and two-tailed alpha was set
to 0.05. The assumptions of ANOVA (normality and
homogeneity of variance) were tested and met according
to the guidelines set forth by Quinn & Keough (2002).
RESULTS
Affiliation
The social affiliation of each adult female dyad was
calculated according to the percentage of time they spent
in proximity (see Methods). Dyads ranged in this measure
of affiliation from 0.0 to 26.4% with a mean SD score of
7.7 9.04% of time spent in proximity. Of the 10 poten-
tial dyads in this herd, three were categorized as High
affiliation, three as Moderate affiliation and four as Low
affiliation (see Table 2).
Movement Analyses
The social affiliation of caller/partner dyads had a signif-
icant main effect on movement ratios at both 60 and 120 s
following production of the initial call (F
2,548
¼3.660,
P¼0.026 and F
2,548
¼4.381, P¼0.013, respectively).
Caller/partner dyads of High affiliation showed approach
behaviour 60 and 120 s after the initial call, while those
of Moderate and Low affiliation showed lesser degrees of
approach (Fig. 1).
The production of an antiphonal reply by the social
partner also had a significant main effect on the dyad’s
movement ratio at 60 s following the initial call (F
1,548
¼
Table 2. Social affiliation categorization of each dyad (percentage of
time observed in proximity)
High affiliation Moderate affiliation Low affiliation
Fiki/Thandi
(26.4%)
Donna/Vasha
(5.7%)
Donna/Thandi
(2.3%)
Fiki/Moyo
(17.1%)
Fiki/Vasha
(4.2%)
Thandi/Vasha
(1.0%)
Moyo/Thandi
(16.3%)
Donna/Fiki
(3.3%)
Donna/Moyo
(0.3%)
Moyo/Vasha
(0.0%)
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 76,5
1604
12.557, P<0.001), but not at 120 s (F
1,548
¼3.298,
P¼0.070). Sixty seconds after the initial call, dyads in
which partners produced an antiphonal reply showed ap-
proach behaviour while those that did not produce a reply
showed little average change in their interpartner dis-
tance. This statistical effect was not present at 120 s as
dyads in both response categories moved closer together
by this time (Fig. 2).
Interpartner distance of the caller/partner dyad 1 min
before the initial call had a significant main effect on
the dyad’s movement ratio at both 60 and 120 s following
initial call production (F
1,548
¼6.358, P¼0.012 and
F
1,548
¼12.691, P<0.001, respectively). Caller/partner
dyads classified as being ‘far’ apart (61 m) prior to the
production of the initial call showed overall approach be-
haviour at 60 and 120 s after the initial call. Caller/partner
dyads that were classified as ‘close’ together (<61 m) be-
fore the initial call moved relatively little on average dur-
ing the first 60 s and showed some signs of avoidance at
120 s following initial call production (Fig. 3).
There were virtually no interactions between the three
factors (affiliation, initial distance, antiphonal reply). Of
the eight possible interactions arising from the analysis of
three factors in two ANOVAs, only one significant in-
teraction was documented (affiliation by interpartner
distance before the initial call in the post-60 s analysis:
F
2,548
¼3.876, P¼0.021). Thus, we focus here on the
main effects, which were largely additive (Fig. 4).
DISCUSSION
Results of this study reveal that rumble vocalizations of
African elephants can function to mediate spatial relation-
ships within the herd. While this function has been
suggested based on evidence from field observations and
playback studies, data presented here, collected from our
captive herd using a unique digital audio and GPS re-
cording collar system, provide the first evidence that
spontaneously produced rumbles influence the move-
ments of group members over a wide variety of contexts.
Both anecdotal field observations and results from play-
back experiments conducted with wild populations have
shown a tendency of female elephants to approach the
calls of their herd members (Martin 1978; Poole et al.
1988; Langbauer et al. 1989, 1991; McComb et al. 2000,
2003). Our findings expand on these observations and
show that vocalization-induced approach behaviour is
mediated by a dyad’s social affiliation. That is, while all
dyads of the herd tended towards approach behaviour
following the production of the initial call, those that
were highly affiliated did so to the greatest degree (Fig. 1).
Our results also indicate a significant increase in ap-
proach behaviour among dyads in which partners pro-
duced an antiphonal reply compared to those that did not
0.76
Approach
0.81
0.87
0.93
1
Avoid
Movement ratio
High Moderate Low
Affiliation cate
g
or
y
60 s
120 s
Figure 1. Mean movement ratio (1 SE) of caller/partner dyads at
60 and 120 s following rumble vocalizations across affiliation cate-
gories (high, moderate, low). Values show the proportional change
in interpartner distance relative to the distance at the time of the ini-
tial call.
0.79
Approach
0.83
0.87
0.91
0.95
1.05
1
Avoid
Movement ratio
Reply produced No reply
Anti
p
honal re
p
l
y
p
roduction
60 s
120 s
Figure 2. Mean movement ratio (1 SE) of caller/partner dyads at
60 and 120 s following rumble vocalizations across reply categories
(reply produced, no reply). Values show the proportional change
in interpartner distance relative to the distance at the time of the ini-
tial call.
0.79
Approach
0.84
0.89
0.94
1
1.06
Avoid
Movement ratio
Far Close
Inter
p
artner distance
p
rior to initial call
60 s
120 s
Figure 3. Mean movement ratio (1 SE) of call/partner dyads at 60
and 120 s following rumble vocalizations across distance categories
(close, far). Values show the proportional change in interpartner dis-
tance relative to the distance at the time of the initial call.
LEIGHTY ET AL.: VOCALIZATIONS MEDIATE INTERPARTNER DISTANCE 1605
answer the initial call (Fig. 2). The production of reply
rumbles in conjunction with approach behaviour has
also been documented in field-based playback experiments
(Langbauer et al. 1989, 1991; McComb et al. 2000, 2003).
Reply rumbles may serve to inform the caller of the loca-
tion or impending approach of the social partner, further
supporting the claim that rumbles may function to facili-
tate social reunions.
Movement within dyads following the production of
the initial call was also significantly influenced by inter-
partner distance before to call production. Members of
dyads that were far apart (61 m) before production of the
initial call showed approach behaviour in the first 2 min
following call production (Fig. 3). That approach behav-
iour increased in magnitude with increasing distance
from the caller further supports the proposal that rumbles
maintain group cohesion within fission/fusion societies of
elephant herds. In contrast, when dyad members were
close together before the initial call they showed relatively
little movement over the first minute and showed some
degree of avoidance behaviour in the second minute fol-
lowing call production (see below).
Figure 4 shows the additive effects of all three factors
(affiliation, reply production, initial interpartner distance)
on the movements of dyad members in the first minute
following the initial call. Overall, dyads within the herd
tended to decrease their interpartner distance after the
production of a rumble, suggesting that a general func-
tion of rumbles in African elephants is to maintain group
cohesion. Examination of the particular results in Fig. 4
may help generate more specific hypotheses for future
testing. For example, the top panels of Fig. 4 show inter-
partner movement when dyads were initially far apart
(61 m). There was a general decrease in interpartner dis-
tance, particularly when an antiphonal reply was pro-
duced and the members of the dyad were highly
affiliated (Fig. 4, top left panel). In this latter context,
dyads reduced their interpartner distance by 56.2% rela-
tive to their initial distance (i.e. on average these dyads
reduced their distance by nearly one-half). This finding
shows the potential for rumbles to function specifically
as long-distance ‘contact calls’ that allow for animals to
decrease interpartner distance or reunite when out of
the visual range, as argued above. In addition, some of
these cases could also be instances of ‘distress calls’ that
elicited approaches and aid from closely bonded social
partners (e.g. Gouzoules et al. 1984; Gouzoules & Gou-
zoules 1995; Slocombe & Zuberbu
¨hler 2007).
When pairs were relatively close together (<61 m), the
cohesion effect was weaker than when pairs were initially
far apart (see Fig. 4, bottom panels). Nevertheless, when
there was a reply to the initial rumble among dyads
with strong social bonds, rumble production on average
still resulted in approach behaviour within the dyad.
Rumbles produced under these circumstances may func-
tion to mediate close-distance social interactions of affili-
ated females and therefore serve to reinforce existing
social bonds (Berg 1983; Poole et al. 1988). Some of these
cases may also be instances of ‘greeting rumbles’, which
are given during reunions of bond group members follow-
ing periods of separation (Moss 1981; Berg 1983; Poole
et al. 1988).
0.40
0.50
0.63
0.79
1
1.26
Avoid
Approach
Approach
0.40
0.50
0.63
0.79
1
1.26
Avoid
Movement ratio Movement ratio
Reply produced No reply
High
affiliation
Moderate
affiliation
Low
affiliation
High
affiliation
Moderate
affiliation
Low
affiliation
Far Close
Interpartner distance before antiphonal call
Figure 4. Mean movement ratio (1 SE) at 60 s following rumble production across the three factors (affiliation, reply production, initial in-
terpartner distance). Values show the proportional change in interpartner distance relative to the distance at the time of the initial call. Values
above the dotted line indicate approach and values below the line indicate avoidance.
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 76,5
1606
Only certain combinations of factors resulted in an
average increase in interpartner distance (Fig. 4, bottom
right panels). Such avoidance behaviour occurred when
dyads were close together before the initial call and an
antiphonal reply was not produced by the partner, and
the strongest case occurred when dyads were also weakly
affiliated (bottom right panel). These cases may involve
competition over resources and/or close-range dominance
interactions. In these situations, subordinate animals may
simply disperse upon detecting the vocalization of a nearby
dominant animal, or some of these rumbles could be spe-
cific warning calls by dominants that function to displace
subordinates from valued resources.
While our findings do face the limitations of being
collected from a herd formed of unrelated females in
a captive environment with plentiful food resources, we
found that a general function of rumbles across a wide
variety of contexts is to promote spatial cohesion among
group members. We would expect to see an amplification
of this cohesion effect in situ where herds may have
higher degrees of affiliation and undergo greater degrees of
fission due to larger herd size and wider distribution of
resources. Furthermore, our finding that rumbles may also
mediate avoidance behaviours at closer distances also has
the potential to be amplified in situ, particularly during
periods when the bond group is fused in the dry season,
because of the potential for greater within-group resource
competition (Wittemyer et al. 2005). To continue to disen-
tangle the complex functioning of elephant rumbles, our
future investigations will look for variation in acoustic
structure and differential movements and behaviours of
social partners following their production across a variety
of specific social contexts.
Acknowledgments
The project would not have been possible without the
support and assistance of the keepers and managers of the
Elephant team. We thank the Disney’s Animal Kingdom
Education and Science and Animal Operations teams for
support of the project and assistance in data collection.
Acumen Instruments Corporation designed and main-
tained the GPS recording collars. The work was supported
in part by the National Science Foundation (NSF-IIS-
0326395).
References
Berg, J. K. 1983. Vocalizations and associated behaviors of the
African elephant (Loxodonta africana) in captivity. Zeitschrift fu
¨r
Tierpsychologie,63,63e79.
Boinski, S. 1991. The coordination of spatial position: a field study of
the vocal behaviour of adult female squirrel monkeys. Animal
Behaviour,41,89e102.
Boinski, S. & Campbell, A. F. 1996. The huh vocalization of white-
faced capuchins: a spacing call disguised as a food call. Ethology,
102, 826e840.
Boinski, S. & Garber, P. (Eds) 2000. On the Move: Group Travel in
Primates and Other Animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Braune, P., Schmidt, S. & Zimmermann, E. 2005. Spacing and
group coordination in a nocturnal primate, the golden brown
mouse lemur (Microcebus ravelobensis): the role of olfactory and
acoustic signals. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,58, 587e596.
Butterfield, P. A. 1970. The pair bond in the zebra finch. In: Social
Behavior in Birds and Mammals (Ed. by H. Crook), pp. 249e278.
New York: Academic Press.
Byrne, R. W. 1981. Distance vocalizations of Guinea baboons (Pa-
pio papio) in Senegal: an analysis of function. Behaviour,78,
283e313.
Caldwell, M. C. & Caldwell, D. K. 1965. Individualized whistle
contours in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Nature,
207, 434e435.
Clemins, P. J., Johnson, M. T., Leong, K. M. & Savage, A. 2005.
Automatic classification and speaker identification of African ele-
phant (Loxodonta africana) vocalizations. Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America,117, 956e963.
Douglas-Hamilton, I. 1972. On the ecology and behaviour of the
African elephant. Ph.D. thesis, University of Oxford.
Estes, R. D. 1991. The Behavior Guide to African Mammals: Including
Hoofed Mammals, Carnivores, Primates. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Gouzoules, S. & Gouzoules, H. 1995. Recruitment screams of
pigtail monkeys (Macaca nemestrina): ontogenetic perspectives.
Behaviour,132, 431e450.
Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H. & Marler, P. 1984. Rhesus monkey
(Macaca mulatta) screams: representational signaling in the
recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour,32, 182e193.
Holekamp, K. E., Boydston, E. E. & Smale, L. 2000. Group travel in
social carnivores. In: On the Move: Group Travel in Primates and
Other Animals (Ed. by S. Boinski & P. Garber), pp. 587e627.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kaufmann, J. H. 1962. Ecology and social behavior of the coati,
Nasua narica, on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. University of
California Publications in Zoology,60,95e222.
Lammers, M. O., Schotten, M. & Au, W. W. L. 2006. The spatial
context of free-ranging Hawaiian spinner dolphins (Stenella longir-
ostris) producing acoustic signals. Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America,119, 1244e1250.
Langbauer, W. R. 2000. Elephant communication. Zoo Biology,19,
425e445.
Langbauer, W. R., Payne, K. B., Charif, R. A. & Thomas, E. M.
1989. Responses of captive African elephants to playback of low-
frequency calls. Canadian Journal of Zoology,67, 2604e2607.
Langbauer, W. R., Payne, K. B., Charif, R. A., Rapaport, L. &
Osborn, F. 1991. African elephants respond to distant playbacks
of low-frequency conspecific calls. Journal of Experimental Biology,
157,35e46.
Leighty, K. A., Soltis, J., Leong, K. & Savage, A. 2008. Antiphonal
exchanges in African elephants (Loxodonta africana): collective
response to a shared stimulus, social facilitation, or true communi-
cative event? Behaviour,145, 297e312.
Leong, K. M., Ortolani, A., Burks, K. D., Mellen, J. D. & Savage, A.
2003. Quantifying acoustic and temporal characteristics of vocali-
zations for a group of captive African elephants Loxodonta africana.
Bioacoustics,13,213e231.
McComb, K., Moss, C., Sayialele, S. & Baker, L. 2000. Unusually
extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants.
Animal Behaviour,59, 1103e1109.
McComb, K., Reby, D., Baker, L., Moss, C. & Sayialel, S. 2003.
Long-distance communication of acoustic cues to social identity
in African elephants. Animal Behaviour,65, 317e329.
Martin, R. B. 1978. Aspects of elephant social organisation. Rhodesia
Science News,12, 184e187.
Mitani, J. & Nishida, T. 1993. Contexts and social correlates of
long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour,
45, 735e746.
LEIGHTY ET AL.: VOCALIZATIONS MEDIATE INTERPARTNER DISTANCE 1607
Moss, C. J. 1981. Social circles. Wildlife News,16,1e7.
Moss, C. J. & Poole, J. H. 1983. Relationships and social structure
in African elephants. In: Primate Social Relationships: an Integrated
Approach (Ed. by R. A. Hinde), pp. 315e325. Oxford: Blackwell.
Oda, R. 1996. Effects of contextual and social variables on contact
call production in free-ranging ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta).
International Journal of Primatology,17, 191e205.
Poole, J. H., Payne, K., Langbauer, W. R. & Moss, C. J. 1988. The
social contexts of some very low frequency calls in African ele-
phants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,22, 385e392.
Quinn, G. P. & Keough, M. J. 2002. Experimental Design and Data
Analysis for Biologists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ramos-Fernandez, G. 2005. Vocal communication in a fissione
fusion society: do spider monkeys stay in touch with close associ-
ates? International Journal of Primatology,26, 1077e1091.
Schaller, G. 1972. The Serengeti Lion: a Study of PredatorePrey
Relations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Slocombe, K. E. & Zuberbu
¨hler, K. 2007. Chimpanzees modify
recruitment screams as a function of audience composition.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.,104,
17228e17233.
Smith, S. T. 1972. Communication and other social behavior in
Parus carolinensis.Publications of the Nuttall Ornithological Club,
11,1e125.
Soltis, J., Leong, K. & Savage, A. 2005a. African elephant vocal
communication I: antiphonal calling behaviour among affiliated
females. Animal Behaviour,70, 579e587.
Soltis, J., Leong, K. & Savage, A. 2005b. African elephant vocal
communication II: rumble variation reflects the individual identity
and emotional state of callers. Animal Behaviour,70, 589e599.
Teixidor, P. & Byrne, R. W. 1999. The ‘‘whinny’’ of spider monkeys:
individual recognition before situational meaning. Behaviour,136,
279e308.
Wilkinson, G. S. & Boughman, J. W. 1998. Social calls coordinate
foraging in greater spear-nosed bats. Animal Behaviour,55,
337e350.
Wittemyer, G. & Getz, W. M. 2007. Hierarchical dominance
structure and social organization in African elephants, Loxodonta
africana.Animal Behaviour,73, 671e681.
Wittemyer, G., Douglas-Hamilton, I. & Getz, W. M. 2005. The
socioecology of elephants: analysis of the processes creating mul-
titiered social structures. Animal Behaviour,69, 1357e1371.
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 76,5
1608
... Over the past 20 years, researchers have been interested in the mechanisms that help mitigate separation risk and maintain group cohesion, with a primary focus on human (Homo sapiens) societies [53,54], and increasing interest in numerous animal species [55][56][57]. When social animals move for foraging or travelling, they risk being spatially separated and losing contact with one another [58]. To maintain cohesion, they can produce vocalizations to keep in contact, locate lost members and subsequently reunite [59]. ...
... 'Contact calls' are the main type of affiliative vocalizations used across species to maintain contact with conspecifics, thus functioning to mitigate separation risk and playing a role in fission-fusion dynamics ( §2c) [57]. African bush elephants (Loxodonta africana) exchange 'contact calls' between family members to locate each other and reunite [61], and the production of these 'rumbles' results in a net decrease in distance between individuals in the wild [58]. In goats (Capra aegagrus hircus), peripheral individuals belonging to a semi-free-ranging group showed higher probabilities of producing contact calls than animals in the centre, and the production of these calls was found to be associated with the end of the group expansion, followed by group contraction [62]. ...
... The term 'fission-fusion dynamics' describes processes where subgroups split and merge over time, leading to temporal changes in group composition and size [76]. This provides an ideal setting to study how individuals regulate group dynamics [58,77]. Many species use vocalizations to facilitate fission-fusion dynamics, and more specifically, during splitting and reunion of sub-groups. ...
Article
Full-text available
Because of the diverging needs of individuals, group life can lead to disputes and competition, but it also has many advantages, such as reduced predation risk, information sharing and increased hunting success. Social animals have to maintain group cohesion and need to synchronize activities, such as foraging, resting, social interactions and movements, in order to thrive in groups. Acoustic signals are highly relevant for social dynamics, some because they are long-ranging and others because they are short-ranging, which may serve important within-group functions. However, although there has been an increase in studies concentrating on acoustic communication within groups in the past decade, many aspects of how vocalizations relate to group dynamics are still poorly understood. The aim of this review is to present an overview of our current knowledge on the role of vocalizations in regulating social group dynamics, identify knowledge gaps and recommend potential future research directions. We review the role that vocalizations play in (i) collective movement, (ii) separation risk and cohesion maintenance, (iii) fission–fusion dynamics, and (iv) social networks. We recommend that future studies aim to increase the diversity of studied species and strengthen the integration of state-of-the-art tools to study social dynamics and acoustic signals. This article is part of the theme issue ‘The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics’.
... Existing studies have predominantly focused on temporal relationships, specifically the latency between the offset of the first turn by interactant A to the onset of the second turn by interactant B, investigating overlap avoidance and gap minimization (Sacks et al., 1974). This element has been shown in a range of species, including great apes (e.g., Fröhlich et al., 2016), small apes (e.g., Hylobates lar, Terleph et al., 2018), monkeys (e.g., Callithrix jacchus, Takahashi et al., 2013), cetaceans (e.g., Schulz et al., 2008), elephants (e.g., Loxodonta africana, Leighty et al., 2008), seals (e.g., Phoca vitulina, Anichini et al., 2023), bats (e.g., Diaemus youngi, Carter et al., 2009), mongooses (e.g., Suricata suricatta, Demartsev et al., 2018, and rodents (e.g., Scotinomys teguina, Okobi et al., 2019). ...
... They also flip their ears to prevent being stung in the sensitive area. These disturbances may be expressions of moderate emotional intensity (Rendall 2003) or may serve as contact calls that coordinate group movements (Poole et al. 1988;Leighty et al. 2008) or as alarm calls to further elephants (Langbauer 2000;Poole et al. 1988). McComb et al. (2001) stated that it is possible that such calls are used in social facilitation to educate inexperienced and vulnerable youth on the same dangerous threats. ...
Article
Full-text available
Human-elephant conflict is a common issue in the agricultural sector, often resulting in crop damage. This study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of threatening vocalisation playbacks as a mitigation method to deter elephant encroachment into agricultural areas. The study was conducted at the National Elephant Conservation Centre (NECC) in Kuala Gandah, Pahang, Malaysia, involving two male and five female elephants. Five soundtracks were played to observe the elephants’ responses: the sound of a buzzing bee, a tiger roar, an elephant rumble, rain (control) and nocturnal jungle sounds (control). The elephants’ behaviours were recorded during and after exposure to each soundtrack. The results showed that the elephants responded most strongly to the tiger roar (33%), followed by the buzzing bee sound (23%), while the elephant rumble elicited the fewest responses (8%). The tiger roar and buzzing bee sounds also resulted in the longest halt times, with the elephants stopping and standing still, particularly the older group (p < 0.05). Male and female elephants exhibited similar responses to the sound playbacks (p > 0.05). This study suggests that playback of threatening vocalisations could serve as an additional mitigation strategy to deter elephants from encroaching on agricultural sites, such as oil palm plantations.
... For animals who are nomadic or who migrate between territories, such as some birds, whales or elephants, communication is essential to aid navigation and maintain social cohesion during journeys and across distances. For example, elephants emit low-frequency vocalisations that humans cannot hear (infrasound) to coordinate the herd's movements or to find group members [58], [25], [42]; geese produce honking sounds to ensure flock members don't fall behind and to maintain V-formations in flight [72]; and parrots use contact calls to identify the location of other flock members [4]. ...
... In this study, we were not able to test the specific effects of neighbour identity, affiliation and approach behaviour to the caller due to sample size of calls per neighbour identity and limited visibility of neighbour behaviour due to high seasonal grass. Social relationships between individuals have been shown to influence production and response to calls (Bouchard & Zuberbühler, 2022;Jenikejew et al., 2020;Leighty et al., 2008). For example, Vampire bats, Desmodus rotundus, are more attracted to the contact calls from frequent blood donors (Carter & Wilkinson, 2016). ...
... Regular information through contact calls have been reported in a broad range of group-living species (e.g. pied babblers, Turdoides bicolor, Radford andRidley 2006, barnacle geese, Hausberger et al. 1990, zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, Vignal et al. 2008; Greater spear-nosed bats Phyllostomus hastatus, Bohn et al. 2007; African elephants, Loxodonta Africana, Leighty et al. 2008; white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, Digweed et al. 2007; Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata, Koda et al. 2008;vervet monkeys, Chlorocebus pygerythrus, Cheney et al. 1996;Ordóñez-Gómez et al. 2019). In non-human primates, contact calling has been suggested to play a function of "grooming-at-distance" (Dunbar 1996; Arlet et al. 2015;Pougnault et al. 2022), which is also a hypothesis proposed by Chereskin et al. (2022) to explain that bottlenose dolphins with the weakest social bonds and the least physical contacts presented the highest rate of signature whistles' exchanges. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dolphins are known for their complex vocal communication, not least because of their capacity for acoustic plasticity. Paradoxically , we know little about their capacity for flexible vocal use. The difficulty in describing the behaviours performed underwater while vocalizing makes it difficult to analyse the contexts of emissions. Dolphins' main vocal categories are typically considered to be used for scanning the environment (clicks), agonistic encounters (burst pulses) and socio-affiliative interactions (whistles). Dolphins can also combine these categories in mixed vocal emissions, whose use remains unclear. To better understand how vocalizations are used, we simultaneously recorded vocal production and the associated behaviours by conducting underwater observations (N = 479 events) on a group of 7 bottlenose dolphins under human care. Our results showed a non-random association between vocal categories and behavioural contexts. Precisely, clicks were preferentially emitted during affiliative interactions and not during other social/solitary contexts, supporting a possible complementary communicative function. Burst pulses were associated to high arousal contexts (agonistic and social play), pinpointing on their use as an "emotively charged" signal. Whistles were related to solitary swimming and not preferentially produced in any social context. This questions whistles' functions and supports their potential role as a distant contact call. Finally, mixed vocalizations were especially found associated with sexual (bust pulse-whistle-click), solitary play (burst pulse-whistle) and affiliative (click-whistle) behaviours. Depending on the case, their emission seems to confirm, modify or refine the functions of their simple counterparts. These results open up new avenues of research into the contextual use of dolphin acoustic signals.
... The repertoire of vocalizations produced by Asian elephants is remarkably diverse, encompassing a wide range of sounds that facilitate their communication. These vocalizations include low-frequency rumbles and growls, which can propagate over long distances, allowing elephants to communicate effectively over large areas [9,15]. Additionally, they are capable of producing high-frequency trumpets, chirps, roars, and barks, showcasing their remarkable vocal versatility in acoustic communication [14,22,25]. ...
Chapter
This paper investigates the automatic classification of four types of Asian elephant vocalizations (rumble, roar, trumpet, and chirp) recorded in Kaziranga National Park. Apart from the call type classification, the study explores individual identification and contextual analysis. Various classifiers using openSMILE features are developed to facilitate the classification process. The results demonstrate accurate classification of elephant call types and successful classification of context-specific behavior and individual identity based on trumpet and chirp calls, respectively. This study highlights the potential of acoustic analysis for understanding elephant communication and well-being, offering insights into their context-specific behavior and individual identities.
... Future research may focus on understanding the role of partner cooperation and coordination in relation to individual and pair fitness outcomes, to quantify the effects of partner choice, parental investment, fidelity, and behavioural coordination in chick provisioning 70 . Importantly, we show an association between maleassociated behaviours and their partner's behaviour, which is one of very few examples in the wider literature where individual movement-based behaviour has been shown to affect that of the respective partner 71 . In the future, we expect that with a growing collection of high-resolution biologging data, in avian and non-avian species, as well as an increasing theoretical and statistical framework on intra-and interspecific interactions 4,5,21,72 these findings will be more common. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent biologging technology reveals hidden life and breeding strategies of nocturnal animals. Combining animal movement patterns with individual characteristics and landscape features can uncover meaningful behaviours that directly influence fitness. Consequently, defining the proximate mechanisms and adaptive value of the identified behaviours is of paramount importance. Breeding female barn owls (Tyto alba), a colour-polymorphic species, recurrently visit other nest boxes at night. We described and quantified this behaviour for the first time, linking it with possible drivers, and individual fitness. We GPS-equipped 178 female barn owls and 122 male partners from 2016 to 2020 in western Switzerland during the chick rearing phase. We observed that 111 (65%) of the tracked breeding females were (re)visiting nest boxes while still carrying out their first brood. We modelled their prospecting parameters as a function of brood-, individual- and partner-related variables and found that female feather eumelanism predicted the emergence of prospecting behaviour (less melanic females are usually prospecting). More importantly we found that increasing male parental investment (e.g., feeding rate) increased female prospecting efforts. Ultimately, females would (re)visit a nest more often if they had used it in the past and were more likely to lay a second clutch afterwards, consequently having higher annual fecundity than non-prospecting females. Despite these apparent immediate benefits, they did not fledge more chicks. Through biologging and long-term field monitoring, we highlight how phenotypic traits (melanism and parental investment) can be related to movement patterns and the annual potential reproductive output (fecundity) of female barn owls.
Preprint
Full-text available
Chimpanzee pant-hoots are frequently produced long-distance vocalisations that serve a number of social functions, such as to indicate coordinate travelling, food discovery, or social status. Calling often triggers replies by others, offering potential for some form of group-level decision-making through vocal exchanges. However, despite important physiological and social differences between male and female chimpanzees, pant-hoot research has traditionally focused on males, with very little known about female use. To address this gap, we collected all-occurrence behavioural data from wild adult female and male Eastern chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ) in two communities in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. We show that females pant-hooted regularly, but less frequently than males and, when they did so, mostly in response to others. Response pant- hoots were more common than initiating pant-hoots in both sexes (females: 90.1%; males: 69.5%), and male and female chimpanzees responded to pant-hoots heard at a similar rate (females: 14.1%, males excluding alpha: 15.8%). Both sexes were more likely to respond to pant-hoots from within their own party, but this effect was stronger for females. The likelihood of male chimpanzees pant-hooting in response was inhibited in larger party sizes, while females’ response pant-hoots were not. Finally, female response pant-hoots were less common during periods of maximal oestrous as compared to other stages of their reproductive cycle. Overall, our findings demonstrate that response pant-hoots are subject to social factors, such as within-sex social competition and between-sex reproductive strategies, which affect male and female chimpanzees in different ways, a further demonstration of the high degree of audience awareness in this primate species. Highlights Chimpanzees use pant-hoots in both sex-specific and sex-non-specific ways Individual and social factors impact likelihood of pant-hoot production Our findings suggest pant-hoots are important flexible elements of vocal behaviour for male and female chimpanzees.
Article
Full-text available
The social complexity hypothesis for the evolution of communication posits that complex social environments require greater communication complexity for individuals to effectively manage their relationships. We examined how different socially uncertain contexts, reflecting an increased level of social complexity, relate to variation in signalling within and between two species, which display varying levels of fission–fusion dynamics (sympatric-living chimpanzees and sooty mangabeys, Taï National Park, Ivory Coast). Combined signalling may improve message efficacy, notably when involving different perception channels, thus may increase in moments of high social uncertainty. We examined the probability of individuals to emit no signal, single or multisensory or combined (complex) signals, during social approaches which resulted in non-agonistic outcomes. In both species, individuals were more likely to use more combined and multisensory signals in post-conflict approaches with an opponent than in other contexts. The clearest impact of social uncertainty on signalling complexity was observed during chimpanzee fusions, where the likelihood of using complex signals tripled relative to other contexts. Overall, chimpanzees used more multisensory signals than mangabeys. Social uncertainty may shape detected species differences in variation in signalling complexity, thereby supporting the hypothesis that social complexity, particularly associated with high fission–fusion dynamics, promotes signalling complexity.
Book
1. Introduction 2. Estimation 3. Hypothesis testing 4. Graphical exploration of data 5. Correlation and regression 6. Multiple regression and correlation 7. Design and power analysis 8. Comparing groups or treatments - analysis of variance 9. Multifactor analysis of variance 10. Randomized blocks and simple repeated measures: unreplicated two-factor designs 11. Split plot and repeated measures designs: partly nested anovas 12. Analysis of covariance 13. Generalized linear models and logistic regression 14. Analyzing frequencies 15. Introduction to multivariate analyses 16. Multivariate analysis of variance and discriminant analysis 17. Principal components and correspondence analysis 18. Multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis 19. Presentation of results.
Article
In several species of nonhuman primates screams given by victims of attack elicit interventions from allies in the social group. In the present study, the screams of 16 pigtail macaques (M. nemesirina) under three years of age were assessed with respect to production and contextual usage. Data obtained from these animals three years later were compared with those from the previous period for evidence of changes in usage and production. It was hypothesized that these monkeys (8 males and 8 females), when three years older, would show patterns of contextual usage and production for agonistic screams that were more similar to those of adult animals. For control and comparison, 10 monkeys (9 females and 1 male) who had been older than three years of age during the earlier study were also sampled during the subsequent period. Calls of all subjects were assigned to one of four agonistic contexts (defined by the severity of the aggression and the dominance rank of the opponent) using discriminant functions that had been generated from the earlier data. While the successful classification rate improved for the 16 subjects that were originally three years of age or younger, no significant change was found for screams of the 10 older control monkeys. The probabilities with which screams of the 16 younger subjects were assigned correctly to context (a measure of call production) also were significantly higher during the second period. No such change was evident in the calls of the 10 control subjects. Data from the earlier study period had suggested that, among juveniles, females appeared to be more proficient than males in both the production and usage of these calls. During the second period of study, the proportion of calls that was classified correctly increased significantly for both males and females. The results of tests comparing the proportion of correctly classified calls for individuals revealed no differences between males and females during the second period, confirming similar results for pooled data analyses. Furthermore, there were no significant differences between males and females in the assignment probabilities with which calls were correctly classified. These results suggest that, with age, the younger monkeys became more proficient in the contextual usage and production of screams.
Article
Guinea baboons (Papio papio) were studied in the field at Mont Assirik, Senegal; visibility is poor, baboon predators are frequent and baboon population density is high relative to other prey species at this site. Data were collected on the rates of occurrence of vocalisations in relation to baboon ranging and group fission-fusion, to habitat, and to activities and circumstances of use. Factor analysis, with varimax rotation of orthogonal axes, was used to identify reliable clusters of calls whose rates covaried. For each, a weighted index was constructed to examine variation with social and environmental context. Nocturnal volleys of adult male loud calls were found to be provoked by distant sounds of major predators and by similar volleys from other sleeping sites, and playback confirmed this