ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Poor sleep as a potential causal factor in aggression and violence

Authors:

Abstract

Clinical observations suggest that sleep problems may be a causal factor in the development of reactive aggression and violence. In this review we give an overview of existing literature on the relation between poor sleep and aggression, irritability, and hostility. Correlational studies are supporting such a relationship. Although limited in number, some studies suggest that treatment of sleep disturbances reduces aggressiveness and problematic behavior. In line with this is the finding that sleep deprivation actually increases aggressive behavior in animals and angriness, short-temperedness, and the outward expression of aggressive impulses in humans. In most people poor sleep will not evoke actual physical aggression, but certain individuals, such as forensic psychiatric patients, may be particularly vulnerable to the emotional dysregulating effects of sleep disturbances. The relation between sleep problems and aggression may be mediated by the negative effect of sleep loss on prefrontal cortical functioning. This most likely contributes to loss of control over emotions, including loss of the regulation of aggressive impulses to context-appropriate behavior. Other potential contributing mechanisms connecting sleep problems to aggression and violence are most likely found within the central serotonergic and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal-axis. Individual variation within these neurobiological systems may be responsible for amplified aggressive responses induced by sleep loss in certain individuals. It is of great importance to identify the individuals at risk, since recognition and adequate treatment of their sleep problems may reduce aggressive and violent incidents.
Review Article
Poor sleep as a potential causal factor in aggression and violence
Jeanine Kamphuis
a,
, Peter Meerlo
b
, Jaap M. Koolhaas
b
, Marike Lancel
a,c
a
Department of Forensic Psychiatry, Mental Health Services Drenthe, Assen, Netherlands
b
Department of Behavioral Physiology, University of Groningen, Haren, Netherlands
c
University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
article info
Article history:
Received 18 August 2011
Received in revised form 21 October 2011
Accepted 15 December 2011
Available online 1 February 2012
Keywords:
Sleep
Sleep disturbances
Sleep deprivation
Aggression
Violence
Prefrontal cortex
Serotonin
Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis
abstract
Clinical observations suggest that sleep problems may be a causal factor in the development of reactive
aggression and violence. In this review we give an overview of existing literature on the relation between
poor sleep and aggression, irritability, and hostility. Correlational studies are supporting such a relation-
ship. Although limited in number, some studies suggest that treatment of sleep disturbances reduces
aggressiveness and problematic behavior. In line with this is the finding that sleep deprivation actually
increases aggressive behavior in animals and angriness, short-temperedness, and the outward expression
of aggressive impulses in humans. In most people poor sleep will not evoke actual physical aggression,
but certain individuals, such as forensic psychiatric patients, may be particularly vulnerable to the emo-
tional dysregulating effects of sleep disturbances. The relation between sleep problems and aggression
may be mediated by the negative effect of sleep loss on prefrontal cortical functioning. This most likely
contributes to loss of control over emotions, including loss of the regulation of aggressive impulses to
context-appropriate behavior. Other potential contributing mechanisms connecting sleep problems to
aggression and violence are most likely found within the central serotonergic and the hypothalamic–
pituitary–adrenal-axis. Individual variation within these neurobiological systems may be responsible
for amplified aggressive responses induced by sleep loss in certain individuals. It is of great importance
to identify the individuals at risk, since recognition and adequate treatment of their sleep problems may
reduce aggressive and violent incidents.
Ó2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
It is well known that sleep loss can have serious detrimental ef-
fects on cognitive performance. For example, sleep deprivation re-
duces sustained attention, executive functioning, and memory
[1,2]. Moreover, sleep loss may also affect emotional function [3],
and chronically disrupted sleep may even sensitize individuals to
mood disorders [4,5]. However, relatively little attention has been
paid to the effects of sleep loss on other areas of affective function-
ing. Yet, many people have the experience that disturbed sleep is
accompanied by emotional instability expressed, for instance, by
a greater irritability and short-temperedness. In most people this
will not result in physical outbursts of aggression. However, this
may be different in vulnerable individuals, such as psychiatric pa-
tients, who often experience serious sleep problems. For example,
about 80% of patients suffering from a depressive disorder experi-
ence sleep problems [6], and sleep disturbances are found in 30–
80% of schizophrenic patients [7]. Similar numbers may apply to
forensic psychiatric patients, but published data are lacking. Foren-
sic psychiatric hospitals treat patients who have committed (vio-
lent) offences but have diminished responsibility due to a mental
disorder. The most important treatment goal for this specific group
of patients is to reduce the risk of (violent) recidivism. Based on
clinical observations in forensic psychiatry, we have the impres-
sion that poor sleep may contribute to the loss of emotional con-
trol, including the regulation of aggression. If sleep problems are
a potential risk factor for hostile and (reactive) aggressive behavior,
treating sleep disturbances and promoting good sleep in mentally
disordered offenders may be beneficial in crime-preventing treat-
ment programs. Therefore, in this paper we present an overview
of existing literature concerning the relationship between sleep
and aggression/hostility.
2. Methods
A literature search was performed in PubMed and Ovid with the
following search terms: sleep, sleep disturbances, sleep problems,
sleep deprivation, sleep architecture, aggression and aggressive
behavior. The search terms were used in different combinations.
Cross-references were checked for relevant papers. We included
1389-9457/$ - see front matter Ó2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2011.12.006
Corresponding author. Address: P.O. Box 30007, 9400 RA Assen, Netherlands.
Tel.: +31 592 334800; fax: +31 592 334402.
E-mail address: Jeanine.Kamphuis@ggzdrenthe.nl (J. Kamphuis).
Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Sleep Medicine
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep
studies that covered sleep and its effects on aggression, anger, and
irritability.
With regards to the interpretation of relevant studies, it is impor-
tant to bear in mind that aggression is often defined and measured in
different ways, including self-report questionnaires, observed
behavior by family or experimenters, or endpoints measured in an
experimental task. These measures are not necessarily analogous.
In addition, in several studies it is not aggression that is measured
but anger, hostility, or irritability by means of mood questionnaires.
However, hostility, irritability, anger, and rage appear to play an
important role in violent behavior and individuals with a poor regu-
lation of these emotions are at greater risk for actual aggression. Fur-
thermore, aggressive acts do not occur as frequently as aggressive
feelings. Therefore, only studying aggressive behavioral actions re-
duces the ability to detect relationships.
We will first discuss studies investigating correlations between
sleep quality and aggression or angriness, separating for children/
adolescents and adults. Secondly, we will discuss studies per-
formed in aggressive individuals, including studies employing pol-
ysomnography. We will then review studies examining the effects
of treatment of sleep disorders on daytime problematic (aggres-
sive) behavior. Thereafter, we will focus on studies concerning
the influence of experimental sleep deprivation on emotional func-
tioning in animals as well as in human subjects. Finally, we will
theorize on potential physiological mechanisms for the putative
relation between sleep and aggression, such as poor prefrontal
functioning, serotonergic mechanisms, and Hypothalamic–Pitui-
tary–Adrenal (HPA)-axis mediation. We will end with speculations
on the possibility of individual vulnerability to the emotional ef-
fects of poor sleep.
3. Correlations between sleep and aggression
3.1. Childhood populations
In addition to the more obvious consequences such as sleep-
iness, inattention, and poor cognitive performance and anxiety
poor sleep in children and adolescents appears to be associated
with aggression and conduct problems [8,9]. In a large group of
2- to 3-year-old children the number of night time awakenings
was positively correlated with parent-rated aggressive behavior
[10]. Parent-reported sleep problems in 3- to 4-year-old twins cor-
related positively with conduct problems, anxiety, and hyperactiv-
ity [11]. In a sample of older children (mean age 7) suffering from
insomnia, parents were asked to rate daytime problematic behav-
ior with the Childhood Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Shorter total
sleep-time was associated with higher scores on delinquent behav-
ior [12]. A similar observation was made in a large group of 2- to 5-
year-old children in the US [13]. In contrast, in a large Canadian
population, those children who slept more than other children
were rated as more aggressive on a parent-rated scale [14]. How-
ever, the item used to assess sleep time asked the parents to in-
clude both night sleep and day sleep. More sleep during the day
could of course be a sign of inadequate night sleep quality, inde-
pendent of sleep duration. In fact, children who were assessed as
being overtired also had higher scores on aggressive behavior.
Comparable observations were made in adolescents: both total
sleep time and self-rated tiredness correlated positively with
self-rated aggression, also, after accounting for other risk factors
[15]. In a large 2- to 14-year-old population, those children at high
risk for sleep-disordered breathing-problems or periodic leg move-
ments during sleep (as measured by the Pediatric Sleep Question-
naire) had significantly more parent-reported aggression and
conduct problems [16]. In a large group of US schoolchildren symp-
toms of sleep-disordered breathing were significantly correlated
with parent- or teacher-rated conduct problems, bullying behavior,
and discipline referrals [17].
Interestingly, sleep problems at young age predicted, though
modestly, conduct behavior, anxiety, and hyperactivity several
years later [11]. In addition, parent-rated sleep problems in child-
hood are correlated with higher scores on an aggression behavior
scale later in life [18]. The authors state that sleep problems during
childhood may constitute risk indicators of behavioral difficulties
later in life.
Taken together, multiple studies in healthy and insomniac chil-
dren show correlations between inadequate sleep (in sleep dura-
tion as well as in sleep quality) and daytime problematic
behavior such as aggression and conduct problems.
3.2. Adult populations
In two groups of healthy young men, with one group habitually
sleeping 7–8 h per night and the other 9.5–10.5 h per night, the
group with the shorter sleep duration scored significantly higher
on anger and hostility on a mood scale [19]. Other studies failed
to show a significant positive correlation between sleep duration
and self-rated aggression, hostility, or angriness [20–22]. Yet,
Granö et al. found an association between shorter sleep duration
and higher hostility, which was partially related to psychiatric
problems [23]. In Israeli medical residents, work-induced sleep
deprivation and fragmentation, measured with actigraphy, ampli-
fied the negative emotional consequences of disruptive daytime
events while reducing the positive effect of goal-enhancing events
[24]. In a Polish study, the difference between desired sleep dura-
tion and actual self-reported sleep time correlated positively with
mood complaints (including irritability) in adolescents and stu-
dents, but not in young working adults [25]. To summarize, the
studies in adults correlating short sleep duration to daytime
aggression are not conclusive.
One complication in many of the studies that rely on indices of
sleep duration is that sleep quality is not taken into account. In fact,
sleep quality may be even more important than actual sleep time
with regard to the relationship between sleep problems and nega-
tive emotions, such as angriness and hostility. In a large group of
Finnish hospital employees a hostility scale was used to assess
proneness for angriness, irritability, and argumentativeness [23].
Sleep problems during the past four weeks were assessed with a
four-item scale. The authors found that sleep disturbances, sleep-
onset and sleep-maintenance insomnia, and unrefreshing sleep cor-
related independently with self-reported hostility. This relationship
remained after adjusting for several potential confounders, includ-
ing psychiatric disorders. A large adult Korean population was asked
whether they were suffering from sleep problems. In addition, trait-
anger was measured with the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale, which
measures the general disposition toward angry feelings (referred
to by the authors as temperament) and the tendency to express an-
ger (referred to as reaction). Difficulties maintaining sleep, excessive
daytime sleepiness, a bad condition in the morning after awakening,
and awakening late in the morning were all significantly associated
with trait-anger [22]. Unfortunately, the authors did not differenti-
ate between the angry temperament and angry reaction.
A small group of US students with insomnia complaints were
asked, using the Novaco Provocative Inventory [26], with how much
anger they would react to a number of potential provocative situa-
tions. As the authors expected, the insomniacs scored higher than
the controls. The subjects also underwent a stress elicitation test
with the simultaneous recording of electrophysiological measures:
sleep problems, such as difficulty falling asleep and overall dimin-
ished sleep quality, were correlated with higher skin conductance
and heart rate, both signs indicative of higher arousal. In two studies,
US college students were asked how often they experienced mood
328 J. Kamphuis et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334
changes during the last weeks using the Profile of Mood States
(POMS) [21,27]. The POMS is a questionnaire which assesses the
severity of active fluctuating mood states such as depression, ten-
sion, fatigue, confusion, vigor, and anger. Those students with poor
subjective sleep quality, as measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index (PSQI; used cut of >8), experienced significantly more anger
during the last weeks.
All together, poor sleep quality in both healthy and insomniac
adults indeed correlates with increased frequency and intensity
of self-reported anger, hostility, and aggression.
4. Aggressive individuals
4.1. Correlations
The correlation between disturbed or short sleep and disturbed
control of aggression could be even stronger in subjects with a high
anger-trait as part of their character. A condition in which aggres-
sion and angriness are core symptoms and can be considered part
of the subject’s character is antisocial personality disorder (ASPD).
Individuals with ASPD probably suffer from sleep problems compa-
rable to other psychiatric patients. In fact, in a group of Turkish
male military subjects diagnosed with ASPD, approximately 80%
scored >5 on the PSQI, indicating poor sleep quality [28]. As ex-
pected, these subjects had significantly higher levels of aggression
compared to the control group, but also more subjective sleep
complaints. Aggression was assessed with the Aggression Ques-
tionnaire, in which subjects have to judge how applicable state-
ments on aggression and angriness are to themselves, measuring
trait aggression. Ireland and Culpin studied an adolescent prison
population [29]. Since 50–80% of incarcerated offenders have diag-
nosable ASPD [30], it is reasonable to assume that a majority of
these prisoners met the criteria for ASPD. In the adolescent offend-
ers sleep disturbances and short sleep duration were associated
with increased hostility. Female victims of domestic violence re-
ported significantly more sleep problems in their abusive partners
as compared to controls [31]. Intriguingly, significantly more of the
abused women reported increased aggression from their partners
after a poor night’s sleep (58% vs 7% in controls) and half of them
remembered being battered on those days. The authors suggested
that sleep deprivation could be an important causal factor for
aggression in domestic violence. To our knowledge, there are no
studies on aggressive populations, such as ASPD patients or a pris-
on population, using objective measures of aggression or employ-
ing more provocative inventories to measure reactive aggression.
Also, no correlational studies were found in which objective mea-
sures of aggression were employed.
Taken together, aggressive populations seem to have serious
sleep problems which correlate positively with increased self-re-
ported aggression/hostility and partner-reported aggressive
outbursts.
4.2. Measuring sleep in aggressive individuals
Though limited in number, there are some studies suggesting
that polysomnographically recorded night sleep is different in
aggressive individuals. In healthy adults certain sleep variables
appeared to correlate with specific personality factors: aggressive
tension and impulsiveness, including a lack of affect control and
frustration tolerance and a need to dominate and criticize others,
correlate positively with the amount of superficial sleep during
the night [20]. No correlation was found with deep, slow wave
sleep (SWS). In another study, 19 male psychiatric patients with
ASPD who committed violent crimes exhibited significantly more
night time awakenings and, consequently, lower sleep efficiency
than control subjects [32]. These results remained after correc-
tion for alcoholism, sleep deprivation, and head injuries. On
the other hand, the ASPD patients also had more SWS, which
is supposed to be the most refreshing part of sleep; despite this,
they had a lower self-reported sleep quality. No differences in
rapid eye movement (REM) sleep parameters were found. A
higher amount of SWS was also observed in three female psychi-
atric patients with ASPD charged for violent crimes [33] and in
preadolescent boys with conduct disorder [34], a childhood psy-
chiatric disorder which may develop into ASPD after the age of
18 years.
Measurements of autonomic physiological functions suggest
that hyperarousal might mediate the relationship between sleep
problems and aggression. In a small study, male subjects with
so-called type A behavior characterized by impatience, hostility,
aggression, and ambition and high scores on a trait-anger-scale
were compared with males with the exact opposite, type B
behavior and low scores on a trait-anger-scale [35]. Compared to
the latter group, the angry type A scorers showed cardiovascular
hyper-reactivity even when asleep.
All together, sleep and physiological recordings suggest that
sleep patterns are different in aggressive individuals compared to
controls. The causes and implications of these observations remain
to be elucidated.
5. Effects of treatment of sleep problems
An interesting case report describes two boys (six and eight
years old), both admitted to a psychiatric unit for increasingly
aggressive and violent behavior towards peers and family mem-
bers [36]. During admission, they were diagnosed with obstructive
sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS), which in children is most often due
to enlarged tonsils. After adenotonsillectomy, both children had, as
expected, a significant reduction in their apneic episodes. More
importantly, prominent reductions both in the number and sever-
ity of violent outbursts were observed. After several months this
effect was still present and improvements had taken place in other
areas as well, including school performance and social interactions.
In unreferred schoolchildren nighttime breathing problems corre-
lated positively with aggression and oppositional behavior [37].
In addition, children with OSAS receiving adenotonsillectomy
showed long-term improvement in parent-reported aggression
[38,39]. Furthermore, a small sample of sex offenders suffering
from OSAS showed significantly lower scores on an aggression
questionnaire after OSAS treatment with continuous positive air-
way pressure (CPAP) [40].
Treatment of sleep disturbances other than OSAS may also lead
to a reduction of aggression. This is suggested by a study in adoles-
cents recently treated for substance abuse who also experienced
sleep problems [41]. These adolescents received weekly sessions
of behavioral sleep therapy over the course of six weeks. Adoles-
cents with greater improvements in total sleep time reported sig-
nificantly less aggressive ideations and fewer aggressive actions, as
compared to those with less change in total sleep time. A case re-
port on an aggressive and severely mentally retarded adult male
described that he showed increased aggression during demand
conditions, especially after having slept for less than 5 h [42]. After
implementation of a support plan, explicitly including rest periods,
his aggressive behavior diminished.
J. Kamphuis et al. /Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334 329
Taken together, although limited in number and not always
properly controlled, these studies suggest that treatment of sleep
problems can reduce daytime aggression and problematic
behavior.
6. Laboratory findings
6.1. Animal studies
Various experimental studies in rats suggest that sleep depriva-
tion may increase aggression: rats subjected to enforced wakeful-
ness by placing them together on top of a rotating drum
surrounded by water, died after 3–14 days, not directly from sleep
loss per se, but from fighting [43]. These rats became so hyper-
reactive that even slight physical contacts precipitated vicious,
aggressive behavior. This behavior was not always directed against
the actual offender, and occasionally several innocent rats would
become involved in a fight, while the original aggressor stood by
and watched. Webb kept six rats continuously awake by individu-
ally forced locomotion for 27 days [44]. After 16 days the animals
began to exhibit aggressive behavior, when they were paired in
an observation cage during 5 min. Treadmill-induced total and par-
tial sleep deprivation in rats increases exploratory behavior in an
open field test of anxiety [45]. According to the authors this can
be interpreted as a reduction in anxiety, loss of fear, and increase
in locomotor activity, but can possibly also be interpreted as irrita-
bility. Whether changes in aggression in these studies were a direct
result of sleep loss or, instead, physical activity or other confound-
ing factors involved in the procedures remains a question.
Several studies report increased aggressive behavior following
selective REM-sleep deprivation in rats [46–50] and mice [51].
Although these findings suggest that manipulative changes in sleep
patterns can produce aggression, the results are hard to interpret,
since only REM-sleep is deprived. In addition to this, the flower
pot method, by which REM-sleep is selectively deprived, is often
criticized because it induces high levels of stress.
The animal studies discussed so far differ in the type of aggressive
outcome measured (proactive vs reactive, fighting with conspecifics
vs rats that display mouse killing, pain-induced aggression, irritabil-
ity towards experimenters, etc.). It is important to realise that this in
itself is another source of variability, thereby further complicating
comparability and extrapolation to human aggressiveness.
Another view on the direction of the relationship between sleep
and aggression is offered by studies using a model of social stress
in which animals are placed in the territory of an older, bigger,
and more aggressive male animal and are attacked and defeated.
Immediately following defeat, rats [52,53] and mice [54,55] show
increased amounts of NREM sleep or increased NREM sleep inten-
sity as reflected in elevated slow wave activity in the electroen-
cephalography. This was not seen after a sexual interaction, a
non-aversive social interaction [55].
So, results of animal studies support a potential causal relation-
ship between sleep disturbances and aggressive discharges, but fu-
ture studies are needed to elucidate the exact effect and type of
behavior observed after total sleep deprivation.
6.2. Human studies
In 1964 a 17-year-old male in the US stayed awake for 264 h,
thereby setting the record for prolonged wakefulness. Psychiatric
and neurological consequences were investigated during the entire
sleep deprivation period. In addition to becoming intermittently
delusional and paranoid after several days, he became irritable
and uncooperative [56]. This course of symptom development
has been observed in more cases of long-lasting total sleep
deprivation in humans [57]. Understandably, most of these exper-
iments used shorter sleep deprivations. In fact, after one night of
sleep deprivation healthy young men already scored higher on
the aggression scale of a mood check list [58]. But, the subjects also
showed higher scores on the friendly subscale, which makes the
results difficult to interpret.
In a small British sample subjects who were sleep deprived for
30 h scored higher on the Profile of Mood subscales of depression
and fatigue, but not on the subscales anger and tension [59]. Another
study investigated the effect of chronic insufficient sleep on mood
[60]. Both the experimental and control group slept for 16 days in
the laboratory. The experimental group was allowed 4 h of sleep
per night and the control group was allowed 8 h. Four factors of
mood and physical symptoms, namely optimism-sociability, tired-
ness-fatigue, anger-aggression, and bodily discomfort, showed sig-
nificant effects. Anger-aggression was moderately but significantly
elevated in the 4-h sleep group compared to the 8-h sleep group.
Unfortunately, the authors do not discuss this finding.
In a US healthy adult sample, 56 h of total sleep deprivation in-
duced significant changes in psychopathological symptoms mea-
sured with the Personality Assessment Inventory [61]. Scores on
the antisocial subscale changed significantly in 8.3% of the sample.
Changes on other subscales were seen in more subjects, especially
paranoia, which represented a subtle elevation of interpersonal
mistrust and hostility toward others.
The studies discussed so far use self-report questionnaires to as-
sess the mood state of aggression and anger. To our knowledge
only two studies tried to measure aggression objectively. Kahn-
Greene et al. asked their 55-h sleep-deprived subjects to write a re-
sponse for a cartoon character that is confronted with a frustrating
situation [62], e.g., he gets splashed from a water puddle by a pass-
ing car. The responses after sleep deprivation showed a signifi-
cantly greater tendency to assign blame to others. Sleep
deprivation was also associated with increased outward expres-
sion of aggressive responses and a reduced willingness to take
the blame or offer amends to the other party. The authors suggest
that sleep deprivation weakens the inhibition of aggression. Con-
trary to this Vohs et al. did not measure higher aggression after
sleep deprivation [63]. Participants played an aggression-game
where level of noise blasted at the opponent (the computer) was
the measure of aggression. A part of the study group was also de-
prived of emotional regulation abilities, the so-called ego deple-
tion, by watching a disgusting video while they were not allowed
to show any emotion on their face. Ego-depleted subjects behaved
more aggressively in the game, but there was no additional effect
of sleep deprivation. Unfortunately, the authors only deprived
sleep for 24 h. Possibly, longer total sleep deprivation or a longer
period of partial sleep deprivation is needed in humans to elicit
the effect of a failing inhibition of aggressive urges.
6.3. Failing behavioral inhibition: sleep deprivation and impulsivity
The failing inhibition of aggressive urges possibly reflects en-
hanced impulsivity. There are different forms of impulsive behav-
ior, such as delay discounting, risk taking, and sensation seeking,
but there is also a lack of behavioral response inhibition. The latter
is, in our view, most important in discussing the relationship be-
tween sleep problems and aggressive behavior. Although the detri-
mental effects of sleep deprivation on decision-making [64] and
risk-taking [65,66] are quite clear, the effect of sleep deprivation
on behavioral response inhibition has not been studied exten-
sively. In healthy 24-h sleep-deprived subjects no effect was found
on a behavioral response inhibition task, namely the Stop Task
[67]. This task uses neutral signals, such as a tone or a letter on a
computer screen, to measure a response. Anderson and Platten
sleep deprived healthy subjects for 36 h and had them perform a
330 J. Kamphuis et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334
behavioral inhibition task using positive and negative emotional
stimuli in addition to neutral stimuli [68]. They also did not find
a significant difference in ability to suppress the response to neu-
tral stimuli after sleep deprivation. But, interestingly and more
importantly, sleep-deprived subjects were less able to inhibit their
response to negative emotional stimuli.
Speculatively, loss of behavioral inhibition to negative emo-
tional circumstances mediates the relationship between sleep
deprivation and unwanted or context-inappropriate aggressive
responses.
7. Potential mechanisms
7.1. Prefrontal cortical functioning
One hypothesis on the relationship between poor sleep and
aggression is that sleep deprivation results in poor prefrontal cor-
tical (PFC) functioning. When PFC functioning is reduced, the abil-
ity to anticipate, delay, and initiate behavioral responses based on
cognitive and social context declines [69]. That sleep deprivation
affects the PFC is supported by the finding that sleep deprivation
as short as 24 h leads to significant declines in PFC metabolic activ-
ity [70]. In addition, after sleep deprivation, behavior comparable
to PFC neuropsychological anomalies is observed, which is re-
versed after recovery sleep [69]. Among these anomalies are the
weakening or diminution of goal directed behaviors and instability
in emotional responses [71]. The instability in emotional responses
is reflected by a study showing that healthy volunteers had de-
creased emotional intelligence after 50 h of sleep deprivation
[72]. Emotional intelligence includes the ability to understand
one’s own and others’ emotions, effectiveness in dealing with
interpersonal relationships, and capacity to cope with environ-
mental needs [62], and can be considered a reflection of the level
of PFC functioning. In a group of US healthy subjects, 30 h of sleep
deprivation impaired accurate recognition of human facial emo-
tions [73]. Extreme emotional faces were still correctly recognized,
but the ability to recognize moderately angry and happy faces was
reduced after sleep deprivation. The blunted recognition could re-
flect the susceptibility of the prefrontal lobe to sleep deprivation. A
specific region of the prefrontal lobe, the anterior cingulate cortex,
is implicated in the emotions anger and happiness [73,74].
PFC dysfunctioning is also associated with the loss of inhibition
of context-inappropriate responses, which is also seen after sleep
deprivation [71]. A potential mechanism for this loss of inhibition
and these changed emotional reactions after sleep deprivation is
provided by an fMRI study of Yoo et al. [75]. During scanning, sub-
jects performed an emotional viewing task: 35-h sleep deprivation
significantly weakened the connectivity between the amygdala
and the medial PFC and elevated amygdala activation in response
to negative pictures. The authors suggest that, as the medial PFC
is proposed to exert top-down control of amygdala activity, result-
ing in context-appropriate emotional responses, sleep deprivation
leads to a failure of top-down prefrontal control [75].
Although aggression may in some situations be considered
appropriate and meaningful behavior, most often it is not compat-
ible with the rules in modern society. Raine and Yang hypothesize
that rule-breaking behavior is in part due to impairments in brain
structures belonging to the PFC [76]. In this regard the PFC can be
considered as the cortical region where context-inappropriate
aggression is kept under control. Speculating further, when PFC
control is lost or diminished as a consequence of sleep deprivation,
this could potentially lead to uncontrolled, impulsive aggressive
responses (primitive reactions).
7.2. Serotonin
Evidence from both animal [77] and human [78,79] studies
strongly support an important role for serotonin in aggressive
behavior. Emphasizing the importance of serotonin is the finding
that selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors decrease irritability,
angriness, impulsivity, and assault in violent offenders [80]. An eti-
ological hypothesis states that lower brain serotonin (5-HT) neuro-
transmission in the orbitofrontal cortex contributes to reduced
cortical top-down control, which is implicated in the development
of aggression and violence [81]. The serotonin deficiency hypothe-
sis of aggression is currently debated [77]. Booij et al. showed that
male adult individuals with a history of childhood physical aggres-
sion had currently low 5-HT brain synthesis, measured with posi-
tron emission tomography, but, despite this, no differences in
measures of aggression, emotional intelligence, or impulsivity
compared to control subjects [82]. The authors suggest that low
5-HT does not mediate current behavior, but should be considered
a vulnerability factor for impulsive-aggressive behavior that may
or may not be expressed depending on other variables. For exam-
ple, trait aggression has been shown to be significantly higher in
men with decreased 5-HT availability in combination with high
testosterone levels, but also in men with increased 5-HT availabil-
ity and low testosterone, reflecting the interaction of 5-HT func-
tioning with other variables, in this case testosterone [83]. Thus,
although serotonin seems to have an important role in aggression,
it is almost certainly not the only causal factor for the development
of aggression.
Sleep and wakefulness are associated with clear changes in
serotonergic activity [84]. Particularly, wakefulness and sleep
deprivation are associated with higher serotonergic activity than
during sleep, as established by numerous microdialysis studies
[85]. Total sleep deprivation in rats seems to increase 5-HT turn-
over in the frontal cortex, hippocampus, hypothalamus, and brain
stem [86]. Whether this is also true in humans remains to be elu-
cidated. Moreover, experimental studies in rats have shown that
chronically restricted and disrupted sleep leads to gradual changes
in serotonin-receptor sensitivity [87,88]. To our knowledge, there
are no studies directly investigating the relationship between sleep
problems, serotonergic dysfunctioning, and aggression. However,
indirect evidence supporting such a relationship comes from stud-
ies on suicidal behavior. Both sleep loss and decreased serotonergic
activity are associated with suicide and suicidal behavior [89–91].
The association between low cerebrospinal fluid 5-HIAA (the prin-
cipal metabolite of 5-HT) and suicide is most prominent in or may
even be confined to patients who use particularly violent methods
in their suicide attempts [89]. In addition, lifetime externally direc-
ted aggression is more frequent in suicide attempters, and criminal
offenders also have increased suicidal behavior. Singareddy and
Balon suggest that decreased serotonergic function in suicidal pa-
tients and criminal offenders may predispose them to reduced
inhibitive capabilities or to increased impulsive behavior [89]. This
can be self-directed (suicidal behavior) or externally directed
(impulsive and aggressive behavior toward property or other
persons).
In conclusion, both preclinical and clinical evidence supports
the view that serotonin may be an important causal or modulatory
link between sleep problems and aggression.
7.3. The Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal (HPA) axis system
HPA axis dysfunctioning contributes to aggressive behavior in
antisocial and conduct-disordered individuals [92]. However, the
generally accepted hypothesis is that this is explained by the
J. Kamphuis et al. /Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334 331
hypo-arousal driven aggression model. This is not compatible with
the hyperarousal seen in insomniacs: Waters et al. observed that
insomniac subjects exhibited signs indicative of hyperarousal,
namely higher skin conductance and heart rate, and also scored
higher on an anger scale compared to non-insomniac controls
[26]. Animal studies focusing on the involvement of the HPA sys-
tem in aggressive behavior produce contradicting results [93].
Sleep, particularly deep sleep, has an inhibitory influence on the
HPA axis, whereas HPA axis activation produces arousal and sleep-
lessness [5,94,95]. Insomnia is associated with higher levels of
ACTH and cortisol, especially during the evening and first part of
the night, suggesting central nervous system hyperarousal [95].A
rat study which showed increased exploration in an open field test
of anxiety after sleep deprivation possibly reflecting irritability
did not reveal differences in plasma corticosterone levels [45]. This
suggests that sleep deprivation-induced explorative behavior is not
mediated by HPA activation.
In conclusion, sleep disturbance may affect the HPA axis, but
whether this contributes to the relation between sleep problems and
aggression remains to be elucidated. To our knowledge, no study exists
specifically addressing the role of the HPA-system in this relation.
8. Individual vulnerability
It is possible that certain individuals are more susceptible to the
emotional consequences of poor sleep. For example, individual dif-
ferences in emotional intelligence predict the influence of sleep
deprivation on written responses to cartoons displaying frustrating
situations [62]. This could indicate that individuals with low emo-
tional intelligence are more vulnerable to the negative effects of
sleep deprivation on mental functioning. Speculatively, individuals
with poor prefrontal functioning, such as certain psychiatric pa-
tients, are potentially more vulnerable to the effects of sleep depri-
vation. This may imply that individuals with an aggressive trait,
such as many forensic psychiatric patients, may represent a group
with poor prefrontal inhibition of aggressive impulses. In fact,
researchers have found lower volumes of blood flow in the frontal
lobes of people with various kinds of impulse-control disorders
[96]. A meta-analysis of brain-imaging studies confirmed prefron-
tal structural and functional impairments in antisocial populations
[97]. Possibly, poor sleep exacerbates the loss of their already low
impulse control, leading to verbal or even physical acting out of
aggressive impulses. This makes the relationship between sleep
and aggression of special importance in these subgroups.
In addition to individual differences in prefrontal cortical func-
tioning, interindividual differences in serotonergic function may
also contribute to the variability in effects of sleep loss on aggres-
sion. Variability in central serotonin function is associated with indi-
vidual differences in effect, temperament, and personality: for
example, high-aggressive male rats respond in a more sensitive
way to a 5-HT
1A
-receptor agonist compared to low-aggressive ani-
mals [93]. As discussed earlier, low central 5-HT may be a vulnera-
bility factor for impulsive-aggressive behavior [82]. Sleep
deprivation most certainly has an effect on serotonergic activity in
the brain, but to what degree this effect differs between individuals
is not clear. Thus, although we know that individual variation in the
serotonergic system is associated with differences in aggressive-
ness, the potential role of sleep in this relation remains to be
elucidated.
Similar speculations can be made for individual variation in the
HPA axis. Differences in behavioral coping style between high and
low aggressive mice have been associated to differences in the HPA
stress system [98]. Overall, the general picture of the relationship
between aspects of variation in coping style and HPA-axis activity
is rather complicated and results are not conclusive [93]; yet, the
possibility that certain variations in the HPA-axis make individuals
vulnerable to the effect of sleep problems on behavioral problems
cannot be excluded.
9. Conclusions
Clinical and anecdotal observations in forensic psychiatric pa-
tients suggest that sleep loss is a potential risk factor for impulsive,
reactive aggression. The larger part of the reviewed literature con-
cerning the relationship between poor sleep and emotional regula-
tion supports this hypothesis. Yet, most studies measured
correlations and, thus, did not provide information on causality:
poor sleep may affect aggressiveness or aggressiveness may produce
sleep problems. Another option is that both sleep problems and
aggressive tensions/behavior are due to a shared risk factor, such
as a psychiatric disorder. However, the limited number of studies
showing that adequate treatment of sleep problems reduces day-
time aggression support the hypothesis that sleep problems are, in
fact, a risk factor for aggression. Unfortunately, relevant animal re-
search on sleep deprivation and aggression is largely confined to
the influence of REM-sleep deprivation. Moreover, many of these
studies have not taken into account various confounding factors
associated with the sleep deprivation procedures. Sleep deprivation
in humans appears to exert profound effects on mood, including in-
creased irritability and altered reactions to frustrations. Whether or
not these effects actually result in physically acting out aggressive
urges is still obscure.
Sleep problems may impair prefrontal cortical functioning,
thereby weakening the top-down inhibition of aggressive impulses.
This offers a neurobiological mechanism explaining how sleep prob-
lems may be a risk factor for aggression and violent behavior. Possi-
bly, some individuals may be more vulnerable to the emotional
consequences of poor sleep. If so, it is highly important to identify
these individuals, because promoting good sleep in these people
may reduce the frequency or severity of aggressive outbursts. People
known to be aggressive, such as violent delinquents and some ASPD
patients, are most likely to belong to this risk group.
In view of the fact that most existing literature supports an
association between sleep loss and aggression, it seems worth-
while to investigate in more detail the impact of sleep problems
and experimental sleep deprivation on aggression and hostility.
Studies exploring individual vulnerability for the negative conse-
quences of poor sleep on the regulation of emotions are also of par-
ticular interest. Moreover, there is a need for studies elucidating
the neurobiological mechanisms that may mediate the influence
of poor sleep on aggressive feelings and behavior. Speculatively,
when evidence in support of the relationship between sleep and
aggression is substantiated, it is of utmost importance to pay atten-
tion to sleep complaints and correctly diagnose and adequately
treat sleep disturbances in individuals at risk. The treatment of
sleep problems may be a valuable, innovative element in aggres-
sion-reducing and crime-preventing treatment programmes.
Conflict of Interest
The ICMJE Uniform Disclosure Form for Potential Conflicts of
Interest associated with this article can be viewed by clicking on
the following link: doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2011.12.006.
References
[1] Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science
2001;294(5544):1048–52.
[2] Walker MP. Cognitive consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Med
2008;9(1):29–34.
332 J. Kamphuis et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334
[3] Walker MP, van der Helm E. Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional
brain processing. Psychol Bull 2009;135(5):731–48.
[4] Baglioni C, Battagliese G, Feige B, Spiegelhalder K, Nissen C, Voderholzer U,
et al. Insomnia as a predictor of depression: A meta-analytic evaluation of
longitudinal epidemiological studies. J Affect Disord 2011;135(1–3):10–9.
[5] Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. Restricted and disrupted sleep: effects on
autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity.
Sleep Med Rev 2008;12:197–210.
[6] Abad VC, Guilleminault C. Sleep and psychiatry. Dialogues Clin Neurosci
2005;7(4):291–303.
[7] Corhs S. Sleep disturbances in patients with schizophrenia: impact and effect
of antipsychotics. CNS Drugs 2008;22(11):939–62.
[8] O’Brien LM. The neurocognitive effects of sleep disruption in children and
adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2009;18(4):813–23.
[9] Dahl RE. Sleeplessness and aggression in youth. J Adolesc Health
2006;38(6):641–2.
[10] Reid GJ, Hong RY, Wade TJ. The relation between common sleep problems and
emotional and behavioral problems among 2- and 3-year-olds in the context
of known risk factors for psychopathology. J Sleep Res 2009;18(1):49–59.
[11] Gregory AM, Eley TC, O’Connor T, Plomin R. Etiologies of associations between
childhood sleep and behavioral problems in a large twin sample. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004;43(6):744–51.
[12] Velten-Schurian K, Hautzinger M, Poets CF, Schlarb AA. Association between
sleep patterns and daytime functioning in children with insomnia: the
contribution of parent-reported frequency of night waking and wake time
after sleep onset. Sleep Med 2010;11(3):281–8.
[13] Lavigne JV, Arend R, Rosenbaum D, et al. Sleep and behavior problems among
preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1999;20(3):164–9.
[14] Coulombe JA, Reid GJ, Boyle MH, Racine Y. Concurrent associations among
sleep problems, indicators of inadequate sleep, psychopathology, and shared
risk factors in a population-based sample of healthy Ontario children. J Pediatr
Psychol 2009;35(7):790–9.
[15] Coulombe JA, Reid GJ, Boyle MH, Racine Y. Sleep problems, tiredness, and
psychological symptoms among healthy adolescents. J Pediatr Psychol
2011;36(1):25–35.
[16] Chervin RD, Dillon JE, Archbold KH, Ruzicka DL. Conduct problems and
symptoms of sleep disorders in children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
2003;42(2):201–8.
[17] O’Brien LM, Lucas NH, Felt BT, et al. Aggressive behavior, bullying, snoring, and
sleepiness in schoolchildren. Sleep Med 2011;12(7):652–8.
[18] Gregory AM, Ende van der J, Willis TA, Verhulst FC. Parent-reported sleep
problems during development and self-reported anxiety/depression, attention
problems and aggressive behavior in later life. Arch Pediatr Adolesc
2008;162(4):330–5.
[19] Taub JM. Behavioral and psychological correlates of a difference in chronic
sleep duration. Biol Psychol 1977;5(1):29–45.
[20] Schubert FC. Personality traits and polygraphic sleep parameters. Waking
Sleeping 1977;1:165–70.
[21] Pilcher JJ, Ginter DR, Sadowsky B. Sleep quality versus sleep quantity:
relationships between sleep and measures of health, well-being and
sleepiness in college students. J Psychosom Res 1997;42(6):583–96.
[22] Shin C, Kim J, Yi H, Lee H, Lee J, Shin K. Relationship between trait-anger and
sleep disturbances in middle-aged men and women. J Psychosom Res
2005;58(2):183–9.
[23] Granö N, Vahtera J, Virtanen M, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L, Kivimaki M.
Association of hostility with sleep duration and sleep disturbances in an
employee population. Int J Behav Med 2008;15(2):73–80.
[24] Zohar D, Tzischinsky O, Epstein R, Lavie P. The effects of sleep loss on medical
residents’ emotional reactions to work events: a cognitive-energy model. Sleep
2005;28(1):47–54.
[25] Oginska H, Pokorski J. Fatigue and mood correlates of sleep length in three age-
social groups: school children, students, and employees. Chronobiol Int
2006;23(6):1317–28.
[26] Waters WF, Adams Jr SG, Binks P, Varnado P. Attention, stress and negative
emotion in persistent sleep-onset and sleep-maintenance insomnia. Sleep
1993;16(2):128–36.
[27] Lund HG, Reider BD, Whiting AB, Prichard JR. Sleep patterns and predictors of
disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. J Adolesc Health
2010;46(2):124–32.
[28] Semiz UB, Algul A, Basoglu C, et al. The relationship between subjective sleep
quality and aggression in male subjects with antisocial personality disorder.
Turk Psikiyatri Derg 2008;19(4):373–81.
[29] Ireland JL, Culpin V. The relationship between sleeping problems and
aggression, anger, and impulsivity in a population of juvenile and young
offenders. J Adolesc Health 2006;38(6):649–55.
[30] Hare RD. Diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder in two prison
populations. Am J Psychiatry 1983;140:887–90.
[31] Hoshino K, Pasqualini JC, D’Oliveira EP, da Silva CP, Modesto AE,
Silveira RSM. Is sleep deprivation involved in domestic violence? Sleep Sci
2009;2(1):14–20.
[32] Lindberg N, Tani P, Appelberg B, et al. Sleep among habitually violent offenders
with antisocial personality disorder. Neuropsychobiology 2003;47:198–205.
[33] Lindberg N, Tani P, Sailas E, et al. Sleep architecture in homicidal women with
antisocial personality disorder a preliminary study. Psychiatry Res
2006;145(1):67–73.
[34] Coble PA, Taska LS, Kupfer DJ, Kazdin AE, Unis A, French N. EEG sleep
‘‘abnormalities’’ in preadolescent boys with a diagnosis of conduct disorder. J
Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1984;23(4):438–47.
[35] Madigan Jr MF, Dale JA, Cross JD. No respite during sleep: heart-rate
hyperreactivity to rapid eye movement sleep in angry men classified as type
A. Percept Mot Skills 1997;85:1451–4.
[36] Pakyurek M, Gutkovich Z, Weintraub S. Reduced aggression in two inpatient
children with the treatment of their sleep disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 2002;41(9):1025.
[37] Mulvaney SA, Goodwin JL, Morgan WJ, Rosen GR, Quan SF, Kaemingk KL.
Behavior problems associated with sleep disordered breathing in school-aged
children the Tucson children’s assessment of sleep apnea study. J Pediatr
Psychol 2006;31(3):322–30.
[38] Mitchell RB, Kelly J. Child behavior after adenotonsillectomy for obstructive
sleep apnea syndrome. Laryngoscope 2005;115:2051–5.
[39] Mitchell RB, Kelly J. Long-term changes in behavior after adenotonsillectomy
for obstructive sleep apnea syndrome in children. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg
2006;134(3):374–8.
[40] Booth BD, Fedoroff JP, Curry SD, Douglass AB. Sleep apnea as a possible
factor contributing to aggression in sex offenders. J Forensic Sci
2006;51(5):1178–81.
[41] Haynes PL, Bootzin RR, Smith L, Cousins J, Cameron M, Stevens S. Sleep and
aggression in substance-abusing adolescents: results from an integrative
behavioral sleep-treatment pilot program. Sleep 2006;29(4):512–20.
[42] O’Reilly MF. Functional analysis and treatment of escape-maintained
aggression correlated with sleep deprivation. J Appl Behav Anal
1995;28(2):225–6.
[43] Licklider JCR, Bunch ME. Effects of enforced wakefulness upon the growth and
the maze learning performance of white rats. J Comp Psychol 1946;39:339–50.
[44] Webb WB. Some effects of prolonged sleep deprivation on the hooded rat. J
Comp Physiol Psychol 1962;55:791–3.
[45] Tartar JL, Ward CP, Cordeira JW, et al. Experimental sleep fragmentation and
sleep deprivation in rats increases exploration in an open field test of anxiety
while increasing plasma corticosterone levels. Behav Brain Res
2009;197(2):450–3.
[46] Sloan MA. The effects of deprivation of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep on
maze learning and aggression in the albino rat. J Psychiatr Res
1972;9(2):101–11.
[47] Hicks RA, Moore JD, Hayes C, Phillips N, Hawkins J. REM sleep deprivation
increases aggressiveness in male rats. Physiol Behav 1979;22(6):1097–100.
[48] Peder M, Elomaa E, Johansson G. Increased aggression after rapid eye
movement sleep deprivation in Wistar rats is not influenced by reduction of
dimensions of enclosure. Behav Neural Biol 1986;45(3):287–91.
[49] Paula de HM, Hoshino K. Correlation between the fighting rates of REM sleep-
deprived rats and susceptibility to the ‘wild running’ of audiogenic seizures.
Brain Res 2002;926(1–2):80–5.
[50] Marks CA, Wayner MJ. Effects of sleep disruption on rat dentate granule cell
LTP in vivo. Brain Res Bull 2005;66(2):114–9.
[51] Benedetti F, Fresi F, Maccioni P, Smeraldi E. Behavioural sensitization to
repeated sleep deprivation in a mice model of mania. Behav Brain Res
2008;187(2):221–7.
[52] Meerlo P, Pragt B, Daan S. Social stress induces high intensity sleep in rats.
Neurosci Lett 1997;225:41–4.
[53] Meerlo P, De Bruin EA, Strijkstra AM, Daan S. A social conflict increases EEG
slow-wave activity during subsequent sleep. Physiol Behav 2001;73:331–5.
[54] Lancel M, Droste SK, Sommer S, Reul JMHM. Influence of regular voluntary
exercise on spontaneous and social stress-affected sleep in mice. Eur J
Neurosci 2003;17:2171–9.
[55] Meerlo P, Turek FW. Effects of social stimuli on sleep in mice: non-rapid-eye-
movement (NREM) sleep is promoted by aggressive interaction but not by
sexual interaction. Brain Res 2001;907(1–2):84–92.
[56] Ross JJ. Neurological findings after prolonged sleep deprivation. Arch Neurol
1965;12:399–403.
[57] Orzel-Gryglewska J. Consequences of sleep deprivation. Int J Occup Med
Environ Health 2010;23(1):95–114.
[58] Roth AT, Kramer M, Lefton WL, Thomas. The effects of sleep deprivation on
mood. Psychiat J Univ Ottawa 1976;1:136–9.
[59] Scott JP, McNaughton LR, Polman RC. Effects of sleep deprivation and exercise on
cognitive, motor performance and mood. Physiol Behav 2006;87(2):396–408.
[60] Haack M, Mullington JM. Sustained sleep restriction reduces emotional and
physical well-being. Pain 2005;119(1–3):56–64.
[61] Kahn-Greene ET, Killgore DB, Kamimori GH, Balkin TJ, Killgore WD. The effects
of sleep deprivation on symptoms of psychopathology in healthy adults. Sleep
Med 2007;8(3):215–21.
[62] Kahn-Greene ET, Lipizzi EL, Conrad AK, Kamimori GH, Killgore WDS. Sleep
deprivation adversely affects interpersonal responses to frustration. Pers Indiv
Differ 2006;41:1433–43.
[63] Vohs KD, Glass BD, Todd Maddox WT, Markman AB. Ego depletion is not just
fatigue: evidence from a total sleep deprivation experiment. Soc Psychol Pers
Sci 2011;2(2):166–73.
[64] Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a
review. J Exp Psychol 2003;6(3):236–49.
[65] McKenna BS, Dickinson DL, Orff HJ, Drummond SP. The effects of one night of
sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. J Sleep Res
2007;16(3):245–52.
J. Kamphuis et al. /Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334 333
[66] Venkatraman V, Chuah YM, Huettel SA, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation elevates
expectation of gains and attenuates response to losses following risky
decisions. Sleep 2007;30(5):603–9.
[67] Acheson A, Richards JB, de Wit H. Effects of sleep deprivation on impulsive
behaviors in men and women. Physiol Behav 2007;91:579–87.
[68] Anderson C, Platten CR. Sleep deprivation lowers inhibition and enhances
impulsivity to negative stimuli. Behav Brain Res 2011;217:463–6.
[69] Dahl RE. The regulation of sleep and arousal: development and
psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 1996;8:3–27.
[70] Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G, et al. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive
performance impairments during sleepiness: I. Effects of 24 h of sleep
deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J Sleep Res
2000;9(4):335–52.
[71] Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Implications for the
prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1993;162:413–9.
[72] Killgore WD, Kahn-Greene ET, Lipizzi EL, Newman RA, Kamimori GH, Balkin TJ.
Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive
thinking skills. Sleep Med 2008;9(5):517–26.
[73] van der Helm E, Gujar N, Walker MP. Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate
recognition of human emotions. Sleep 2010;33(3):335–42.
[74] Blair RJR. The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex: functional
contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy. In: Hodgkins S, Vidding E,
Plodowksi A, editors. The neurobiological basis of violence. Oxford: Oxford
University Press; 2009. p. 123–36.
[75] Yoo SS, Gujar N, Hu P, Jolesz FA, Walker MP. The human emotional brain
without sleep a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr Biol
2007;17(20):877–8.
[76] Raine A, Yang Y. Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial
behavior. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2006;1(3):203–13.
[77] Boer de SF, Caramaschi D, Natarajan D, Koolhaas JM. The vicious cycle towards
violence: focus on the negative feedback mechanisms of brain serotonin
neurotransmission. Front Behav Neurosci 2009;3(52):1–6.
[78] Dolan M, Anderson IM, Deakin JF. Relationship between 5-HT function and
impulsivity and aggression in male offenders with personality disorders. Br J
Psychiatry 2001;178:352–9.
[79] Stolpmann G, Fromberger P, Jordan K, Müller JL. Biologische Faktoren bei
forensisch-psychiatrischen Prognosen. Mschr Krim 2010;93:300–12.
[80] Butler T, Schofield PW, Greenberg D, et al. Reducing impulsivity in repeat
violent offenders: an open label trial of a selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitor. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2010;44(12):1137–43.
[81] Siever LJ. Neurobiology of aggression and violence. Am J Psychiatry
2008;165:429–42.
[82] Booij L, Tremblay RE, Leyton M, et al. Brain serotonin synthesis in adult males
characterized by physical aggression during childhood: a 21-year longitudinal
study. PLoS One 2010;5(6):e11255.
[83] Kuepper Y, Alexander N, Osinsky R, et al. Aggression-interactions of serotonin
and testosterone in healthy men and women. Behav Brain Res
2010;206(1):93–100.
[84] Monti JM. Serotonin control of sleep–wake behavior. Sleep Med Rev
2011;15(4):269–81.
[85] Portas CM, Bjorvatn B, Ursin R. Serotonin and the sleep/wake cycle: special
emphasis on microdialysis studies. Prog Neurobiol 2000;60:13–35.
[86] Asikainen M, Toppila J, Alanko L, Ward DJ, Stenberg D, Porkka-Heiskanen.
Sleep deprivation increases brain serotonin turnover in the rat. Neuroreport
1997;8(7):1577–82.
[87] Roman V, Walstra I, Luiten PGM, Meerlo P. Too little sleep gradually
desensitizes the 5-HT 1A receptor system in rats. Sleep 2005;28:1505–10.
[88] Novati A, Roman V, Cetin T, et al. Chronically restricted sleep leads to
depression-like changes in neurotransmitter receptor sensitivity and
neuroendocrine stress reactivity in rats. Sleep 2008;31:1579–85.
[89] Singareddy RK, Balon R. Sleep and suicide in psychiatric patients. Ann Clin
Psychiatry 2001;13(2):93–101.
[90] Kohyama J. Sleep, serotonin, and suicide in Japan. J Physiol Anthropol
2001;30(1):1–8.
[91] Goodwin RD, Marusic AM. Associations between short sleep and suicidal
ideation and suicide attempt among adults in the general population. Sleep
2008;31(8):1097–101.
[92] Goozen van SH, Fairchild G. The neuroendocrinology of antisocial behaviour.
In: Hodgkins S, Vidding E, Plodowksi A, editors. The neurobiological basis of
violence. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. p. 201–21.
[93] Koolhaas JM, Boer de SF, Coppens CM, Buwalda B. Neuroendocrinology of
coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front
Neuroendocrinol 2010;31:307–21.
[94] Steiger A. Sleep and the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical system. Sleep
Med Rev 2002;6:125–38.
[95] Vgontzas AN, Chrousos GP. Sleep, the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis,
and cytokines: multiple interactions and disturbances in sleep disorders.
Endocrinol Metab Clin North Am 2002;31(1):15–36.
[96] Hollander E, Cohen LJ. Psychobiology and psychopharmacology of compulsive
spectrum disorders. In: Odham JM et al., editors. Impulsivity and
compulsivity. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1996. p. 148–50.
[97] Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in
antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry
Res: Neuroimaging 2009;174:81.
[98] Veenema AH, Koolhaas JM, Kloet de ER. Basal and stress-induced differences in
HPA axis, 5-HT responsiveness, and hippocampal cell proliferation in two
mouse lines. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004;1018:255–65.
334 J. Kamphuis et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 327–334
... Circadian misalignment and sleep disturbances have been linked to heightened impulsivity, aggression, and violence (16). It is well established that psychiatric patients who have committed offenses often experience significant challenges in emotion regulation and behavioral inhibition (17), making them more susceptible to aggressive and violent responses to both environmental and intrinsic stimuli. ...
... Regarding non-planning impulsivity, poor sleep quality has been shown to contribute to difficulties in planning and forethought in patients with bipolar disorder (40), which are key components of this impulsivity domain, aligning with our findings. Poor sleep impairs prefrontal cortical (PFC) functioning, reducing top-down inhibition of aggressive impulses, which may lead to heightened impulsive and contextinappropriate aggression (16). Sleep disturbances impair emotion regulation and cognitive control, likely contributing to heightened aggressive behaviors across general and clinical populations (16). ...
... Poor sleep impairs prefrontal cortical (PFC) functioning, reducing top-down inhibition of aggressive impulses, which may lead to heightened impulsive and contextinappropriate aggression (16). Sleep disturbances impair emotion regulation and cognitive control, likely contributing to heightened aggressive behaviors across general and clinical populations (16). Poor sleep quality has been shown to correlate with increased hostility, anger, and irritability, which are subtypes of aggression (14). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This study aimed to investigate the relationships between chronotype, sleep quality, and aggressive and impulsive traits in forensic psychiatric inpatients with schizophrenia. Method: A cross-sectional, observational study was conducted with 71 male forensic psychiatric inpatients diagnosed with schizophrenia. Participants completed self-report scales for chronotype using the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, sleep quality using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), aggression using the Buss-Warren Aggression Questionnaire, and impulsivity using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Results: The results indicated that 37% of participants had an evening chronotype, and 55% were classified as poor sleepers (PSQI>5). Eveningness was associated with poorer sleep quality, increased indirect aggression, and higher levels of attentional impulsivity. Poor sleep quality was linked to most aggressive and impulsive traits. Mediation analysis demonstrated that indirect aggression mediated the relationship between chronotype and attentional impulsivity, with greater eveningness associated with higher levels of indirect aggression, which, in turn, increased attentional impulsivity. Conclusion: The findings underscore the significance of chronotype and sleep quality in shaping aggressive and impulsive traits in forensic psychiatric inpatients with schizophrenia. Indirect aggression partially mediated the relationship between chronotype and attentional impulsivity, highlighting the intricate interplay between circadian rhythms, emotion regulation, and cognition. Strategies aimed at correcting circadian misalignment and enhancing sleep quality may aid in managing aggressive and impulsive behaviors in this vulnerable population
... The literature on the link between sleep and aggression is limited and inconsistent. Previous research primarily focused on clinical or forensic populations, rather than sub-clinical or population based cohorts (Kamphuis et al., 2012). Results have been mixed, as some studies observed a relationship between sleep disturbances and aggression, while others did not (Chervin et al., 2003;Lindberg et al., 2008;Madrid-Valero et al., 2019;Mayes et al., 2009;Shanahan et al., 2014). ...
... Therefore, the causal role of aggression in sleep disturbances remains unclear. One possible mechanism is impaired pre-frontal cortex functioning, which worsens with sleep deprivation (Kamphuis et al., 2012). However, since our sample is not clinical or forensic, it may lack individuals with severe pre-frontal dysfunction, which could explain the absence of an association between aggression and sleep. ...
... It is known to have a major impact on general mental health, influencing both the onset and severity of mental health disorders as well as relapses [4,5]. Moreover, disturbed sleep has specifically been associated with poor emotion regulation and self-control [6,7], potentially leading to problems in impulsivity, hostility, and even aggression [8,9]. The association of poor sleep with aggression is confirmed by large meta-analyses and seems to be even more pronounced in those with existing mental health problems [10,11]. ...
... Despite the high prevalence of insomnia, sleep-improving interventions have hardly been studied in forensic patients [8]. In the multifactorial and complex treatments that are often required in forensic psychiatry, insomnia may have been regarded as merely a symptom of psychopathology that would subside upon treatment of the mental health disorder, thus neglecting the necessity of directly targeting sleep [2,5]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Insomnia is common in forensic psychiatric patients. Not only does insomnia severely impair general mental health, but it has specifically been associated with poor emotion regulation and self-control, potentially leading to problems in impulsivity, hostility, and even aggression. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) could therefore be beneficial in this patient group. Methods: We conducted a 14-week randomized controlled trial of the effects of cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) on sleep, general psychopathology, hostility, impulsivity, and aggression in 31 male forensic psychiatric patients. Results: The CBT-I group (n = 11) showed a stronger reduction in self-reported insomnia symptoms and hostility than the waitlist group (n = 11). No differences were found in post-treatment self-reported general psychopathology, impulsivity, or aggression, nor on actigraphy-measured sleep efficiency. Conclusions: This study demonstrates the effectiveness of CBT-I in forensic psychiatric patients and indicates the importance of insomnia treatment in this population, especially considering the effect on hostility.
... For example, nearly 50 to 70 million Americans suffer from sleep disorders (Altevogt & Colten, 2006), which adversely affect their well-being (Eslami et al., 2014;Steptoe et al., 2008). Findings of previous studies reveal that individuals with poor qualitysleep and insufficient sleep would be more likely to experience or engage in depression (Ivanenko et al., 2005;Nutt et al., 2022), anxiety (McMakin & Alfano, 2015;Staner, 2022), media addiction (Masoed et al., 2021;Sümen & Evgin, 2021), procrastination (Sirois et al., 2015;Zhang & Wu, 2020), negative affect (Bouwmans et al., 2017;Prather et al., 2013), weapon carrying (Hildenbrand et al., 2013;Royle et al., 2023) and fighting (Meldrum & Restivo, 2014;Vincent et al., 2016), aggression and violence (Kamphuis et al., 2012;Mahowald et al., 2005), academic problems (Curcio et al., 2006;Hartmann & Prichard, 2018), alcohol use, drug use, and deviance (Vazsonyi at al., 2014), as well as delinquency or crime (Clinkinbeard et al., 2011;Javakhishvili & Widom, 2022). Moreover, previous research has also provided evidence that poor sleep quality or insufficient sleep are strongly related to low self-control (Barnes, 2012;Pilcher et al., 2015b). ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study tested the direct and indirect links between sleep, internalizing (depression, anxiety, media addiction, obsessive-compulsive thoughts, negative affect, and procrastination) and externalizing problems (media misuse, violence, general crime, and academic problem), as mediated by self-control; it also tested for moderation effects in these relationships. Based on 37 studies for internalizing problems and 16 studies for externalizing problems, with N = 36,487 and N = 32,793 participants, the studies used Meta-Analytic Structural Equation Modeling to address methodological limitations of previous studies based on bivariate meta-analytic approach. One Stage Meta-Analytic Structural Equation Modeling (OSMASEM), provided evidence that sleep was negatively associated with internalizing problems, both directly and indirectly via self-control (β = -.17 and β = -.05, respectively). Sleep was also negatively associated with externalizing problems, again both directly and indirectly via self-control (β = -.12, and β = -.04, respectively). Thus, self-control was a partial mediator of these effects. OSMASEM provided evidence that sleep has both direct and indirect effects on internalizing as well as externalizing problems, partially mediated by self-control. This work identifies both sleep as well as self-control is highly salient when considering preventative and intervention efforts for both internalizing as well as externalizing problems.
... However, hostile behavior is a common problem among children, especially among Iraqi children, because the nurturing of parents depends on the punishments and nerviness, especially toward boys than girls, and it often extends to violence; this result of the study revealed that more than half of the children had hostile behavior extending from mild (35.9%) to severe (8.1%) and lived with their biological parents about (83.9%), which is consistent with the results of Kamphuis et al. as well as with studies by Rahayu and Putera, which ex- amined the relationship between hostile behaviors with some habits among school-age children [23,24]. ...
Article
Objectives: The health of school-aged children is measured using normal physical and psychological indicators. The most common behavioral problem among children is hostility, which increases and has a negative impact. This study aims to assess psychophysical indicators and hostile behavior among school-aged children and to detect the relationship between this behavior and psychophysical indicators. Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted in Al-Najaf governmental schools. The cluster sample included 384 (girls and boys) school-aged children. Data were collected using an adopted Arabic version of the questionnaire to test psychophysical indicators and hostile behaviors. Experts in this field confirmed the tool’s content validity, and its reliability was obtained using Cronbach’s α (0.88 and 0.92). Results: This study showed that most children are boys (62.2%), aged 8-10 years (46.1%), and in the first and second stages (42.5%). About (51.8%) had hostile behavior extending from mild (35.9%) to severe (8.1%). While psychophysical indicators (58.1%) were abnormally assessed, all physical indicators (70%) were normally assessed in children. Finally, the relationship between hostility and psychophysical indicators are significant at (P=0.012) and a strong positive correlation is observed between them (0.94). Discussion: This study concluded that more than half of children have hostile behavior and problems with psychophysical indicators, but some indicators are normal, such as body mass index (BMI) percentile, heart rate, breathing, cognitive, and movement indicators. Also, this study concluded that the psychophysical indicators are abnormal among children, which increased the severity and symptoms of hostile behavior among them. This study recommends increasing parents’ awareness of mental health by teaching them how to nurture their children and integrate programs in the education system, especially in the primary school curriculum, and how to control their children’s anger and accept a situation that triggers and prevents hostile behavior.
... On a behavioral level, experimental studies in non-clinical populations, e.g. using (partial) sleep deprivation schemes, show that sleep loss results in less adaptive emotion regulating behavior and more negative and less positive moods [119], and higher emotional (threat) sensitivity [118], although findings are not always consistent [120]. In different populations varying from non-clinical to non-BPD psychiatric patients, sleep disturbances, like insomnia, or poor (subjective) sleep quality are similarly associated with less adaptive emotion regulation, more negative and less positive moods [121,122], continued hyperarousal [123], as well as with increased impulsive and aggressive behavior [124,106], more dissociative symptoms [111], and more NSSI and suicidal behavior [125,126]. ...
... Daytime sleepiness, in particular, increases risk factors for conduct problems, including learning difficulties, low school achievement, absenteeism, and decreased involvement in extracurricular activities [21e23]. Daytime sleepiness affects emotional regulation and cognitive control, which may exacerbate impulsivity and aggression [24]. Pathways linking caffeine use to conduct problems via effects on daytime sleepiness, however, remain to be investigated. ...
Article
Purpose This paper synthesises literature on non-visual effects of light in prison environments, drawing attention to disconnect between research evidence and current practices. This paper aims to guide prison designers and decision-makers towards lighting strategies that better support inmates’ mental health and rehabilitation. Design/methodology/approach A focused literature search was conducted from June to October 2024 using Google Scholar and PubMed, targeting peer-reviewed studies published between 1985 and 2024. Included studies addressed the non-visual effects of light related to circadian rhythm, mental health, artificial and natural lighting, stress, aggression and hormonal regulation. Only English-language studies providing empirical or theoretical insights relevant to confinement settings were considered. Studies focused solely on visual performance, non-human subjects or lacking health implications were excluded. Keyword combinations were refined iteratively, although some relevant interdisciplinary work may have been missed due to indexing or terminological variations. Findings Neglecting the role of lighting in prison design contributes to poor psychological outcomes. Limited access to daylight and the overuse of artificial lighting with high blue light content disrupt circadian regulation, worsening sleep, mood and mental health. Conversely, designs that maximise daylight exposure and use adjustable artificial lighting with appropriate spectral qualities can promote emotional stability, reduce aggression and support rehabilitation. Research limitations/implications Relevant interdisciplinary studies may still have been missed due to database indexing limitations or terminological variations across fields. Originality/value This paper bridges the gap between lighting design, environmental psychology and prison reform. By focusing on how light affects inmates’ psychological health and rehabilitation, it offers insights into how prison design can be improved to foster well-being.
This study explored a new method for nursing staff's work with collaborative violence risk management, to be used in forensic psychiatric inpatient settings. The components of the method, the background to, and the rationale for collaborative risk management in forensic mental health settings were outlined. An inductive content analysis of 50 of these collaborative violence risk management plans was conducted. The focus of the analysis was the types and frequencies of early warning signs for aggression, risk factors/scenarios for aggression, risk management strategies, and responsibilities devised to address that risk as well as goals agreed upon between patients and staff concerning that risk. The results showed that patients and staff could generate collaborative violence risk management plans which displayed a content with a high degree of face validity and content validity when comparing the content of the plans to previous literature on violence risk assessment and management in forensic mental health settings. Although collaborative work on violence risk and violence risk management poses several challenges within forensic psychiatric contexts, the results showed that these approaches hold some promise in such settings - focusing on risk reduction through goal-oriented collaboration between patient and staff - and are worthy of further development and investigation.
Article
Daylight saving time (DST) affects millions of people in various everyday situations and is a common topic of debate in legislative parliaments around the world. This paper presents a literature review on the effects of the clock change and DST on electricity usage, health, crime rates, road safety, and economic aspects. This addresses a gap in current literature reviews, as there is a lack of linked analyses considering these research fields. We show that there are partial positive effects on crime rates and road safety generally that result from the delay in ambient light availability. This contrasts with the clearly negative effects on health and the economic aspects, which are mainly driven by the disturbed circadian rhythm and the resulting sleep problems. Furthermore, we find that the initial idea of DST to save electricity will probably no longer apply and may even lead to increased usage. This literature review provides a basis for future research and promotes interdisciplinary research by summarizing current findings in a cross-disciplinary manner and identifying research gaps and opportunities. Furthermore, the findings may guide policy-making discussions and decisions.
Article
Full-text available
Aggression and violence represent a significant public health concern and a clinical challenge for the mental healthcare provider. A great deal has been revealed regarding the neurobiology of violence and aggression, and an integration of this body of knowledge will ultimately serve to advance clinical diagnostics and therapeutic interventions. We will review here the latest findings regarding the neurobiology of aggression and violence. First, we will introduce the construct of aggression, with a focus on issues related to its heterogeneity, as well as the importance of refining the aggression phenotype in order to reduce pathophysiologic variability. Next we will examine the neuroanatomy of aggression and violence, focusing on regional volumes, functional studies, and interregional connectivity. Significant emphasis will be on the amygdala, as well as amygdala–frontal circuitry. Then we will turn our attention to the neurochemistry and molecular genetics of aggression and violence, examining the extensive findings on the serotonergic system, as well as the growing literature on the dopaminergic and vasopressinergic systems. We will also address the contribution of steroid hormones, namely, cortisol and testosterone. Finally, we will summarize these findings with a focus on reconciling inconsistencies and potential clinical implications; and, then we will suggest areas of focus for future directions in the field.
Article
Objective: Conduct problems and hyperactivity are frequent among children referred for sleep-disordered breathing (SDB), restless legs syndrome, or periodic leg movements during sleep (PLMS), but children not referred to sleep centers have received little study. Method: Parents of children aged 2 to 14 years were surveyed at two general clinics between 1998 and 2000. A Pediatric Sleep Questionnaire generated validated scores for SDB and PLMS. The Conners Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-48) produced an age- and sex-adjusted Conduct Problem Index (CPI) and Hyperactivity Index. Results: Parents of about 1,400 children were approached; those of 872 (62%) completed the surveys. Bullying and other specific aggressive behaviors were generally two to three times more frequent among 114 children at high risk for SDB than among the remaining children. An association between high CPI and SDB scores (p<.0001) retained significance after adjustment for sleepiness, high Hyperactivity Index, stimulant use, or PLMS scores. Analogous results were obtained for the association between high CPI and PLMS scores. Conclusions: Conduct problems were associated with symptoms of SDB, restless legs syndrome, and PLMS. Although these results cannot prove a cause-and-effect relationship, assessment for sleep disorders may provide a new treatment opportunity for some aggressive children.
Article
Prognoses in forensic psychiatry are demanded and delivered on a daily basis. Given the multitude of different causes, it is not surprising that prognostic questions also widely differ. To predict the likelihood of criminal re-offences, risk checklists are used; biological parameters are rarely considered. This text outlines exemplary (neuro-)biological investigations and methods and their relevance to the diagnosis and treatment of psychiatric disturbances. Their possible relevance with regard to forensic psychiatric prognoses is also discussed. It is suggested that biological facts must be investigated as additional predictors.
Article
Difficulties initiating or maintaining sleep are frequently encountered in patients with schizophrenia. Disturbed sleep can be found in 30–80% of schizophrenic patients, depending on the degree of psychotic symptomatology. Measured by polysomnography, reduced sleep efficiency and total sleep time, as well as increased sleep latency, are found in most patients with schizophrenia and appear to be an important part of the pathophysiology of this disorder. Some studies also reported alterations of stage 2 sleep, slow-wave sleep (SWS) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep variables, i.e. reduced REM latency and REM density. A number of sleep parameters, such as the amount of SWS and the REM latency, are significantly correlated to clinical variables, including severity of illness, positive symptoms, negative symptoms, outcome, neurocognitive impairment and brain structure. Concerning specific sleep disorders, there is some evidence that schizophrenic patients carry a higher risk of experiencing a sleep-related breathing disorder, especially those demonstrating the known risk factors, including being overweight but also long-term use of antipsychotics. However, it is still unclear whether periodic leg movements in sleep or restless legs syndrome (RLS) are found with a higher or lower prevalence in schizophrenic patients than in healthy controls. There are no consistent effects of first-generation antipsychotics on measuresof sleep continuity and sleep structure, including the percentage of sleep stages or sleep and REM latency in healthy controls. In contrast to first-generation antipsychotics, the studied atypical antipsychotics (clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, risperidone, ziprasidone and paliperidone) demonstrate a relatively consistent effect on measures of sleep continuity, with an increase in either total sleep time (TST) or sleep efficiency, and individually varying effects on other sleep parameters, such as an increase in REM latency observed for olanzapine, quetiapine and ziprasidone, and an increase in SWS documented for olanzapine and ziprasidone in healthy subjects. The treatment of schizophrenic patients with first-generation antipsychotics is consistently associated with an increase in TST and sleep efficiency, and mostly an increase in REM latency, whereas the influence on specific sleep stages is more variable. On the other hand, withdrawal of such treatment is followed by a change in sleep structure mainly in the opposite direction, indicating a deterioration of sleep quality. On the background of the rather inconsistent effects of first-generation antipsychotics observed in healthy subjects, it appears possible that the high-potency drugs exert their effects on sleep in schizophrenic patients, for the most part, in an indirect way by suppressing stressful psychotic symptomatology. In contrast, the available data concerning second-generation antipsychotics (clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone and paliperidone) demonstrate a relatively consistent effect on measures of sleep continuity in patients and healthy subjects, with an increase in TST and sleep efficiency or a decrease in wakefulness. Additionally, clozapine and olanzapine demonstrate comparable influences on other sleep variables, such as SWS or REM density, in controls and schizophrenic patients. Possibly, the effects of second-generation antipsychotics observed on sleep in healthy subjects and schizophrenic patients might involve the action of these drugs on symptomatology, such as depression, cognitive impairment, and negative and positive symptoms. Specific sleep disorders, such as RLS, sleep-related breathing disorders, night-eating syndrome, somnambulism and rhythm disorders have been described as possible adverse effects of antipsychotics and should be considered in the differential diagnosis of disturbed or unrestful sleep in this population.
Article
background and objective: Sleep deprivation promotes an increase in aggressiveness; however, this effect has not been investigated in humans. Since high alcohol intake promotes desynchronized sleep deprivation and alcoholism is directly associated with domestic violence (DV), this study aimed to collect information on sleep characteristics, anxiety, and alcohol consumption by DV perpetrators. Methods: Having this purpose in mind, 53 female victims of physical violence perpetrated by their intimate partners (DV group) were interviewed after providing free informed consent. The interviews took place on the occasion that the abused women registered a formal accusation at the Specialized Police Station for Women. The interviews were structured in a previously tested questionnaire. Results: Thirty women from the general population without any complaint of DV composed the control group. The DV group showed prevalent insomnia, high sleep fragmentation, intense daily snoring, a tendency to disregard sleep hygiene rules and thus feel sleepy during the day, sleep complaints, high alcohol intake, and high anxiety levels. Increased aggressiveness from their partners after a poor night's sleep was reported by 58% of DV victims, and half of them reported having been battered on those days. Conclusions: Data obtained support the possible involvement of sleep-deprivation-induced aggressiveness in DV etiology; this fact requires confirmation by further studies using other reliable methods.
Article
Throughout early development, a child spends more time asleep than in any waking activity. Yet, the specific role of sleep in brain maturation is a complete mystery. In this article, the developmental psychobiology of sleep regulation is conceptualized within the context of close links to the control of arousal, affect, and attention. The interactions among these systems are considered from an ontogenetic and evolutionary biological perspective. A model is proposed for the development of sleep and arousal regulation with the following major tenets: 1. Sleep and vigilance represent opponent processes in a larger system of arousal regulation. 2. The regulation of sleep, arousal, affect, and attention overlap in physiological, neuroanatomical, clinical, and developmental domains. 3. Complex interactions among these regulatory systems are modulated and integrated in regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). 4. Changes at the level of PFC underlie maturational shifts in the relative balance across these regulatory systems (such as decreases in the depth/length of sleep and increased capacity for vigilance and attention), which occur with normal development. 5. The effects of sleep deprivation (including alterations in attention, emotions, and goal-directed behaviors) also involve changes at the level of PFC integration across regulatory systems. This model is then discussed in the context of developmental pathology in the control of affect and attention, with an emphasis on sleep changes in depression.