Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study

Article (PDF Available)inBMJ (online) 344(jan04 4):d7622 · January 2012with36 Reads
DOI: 10.1136/bmj.d7622 · Source: PubMed
To estimate 10 year decline in cognitive function from longitudinal data in a middle aged cohort and to examine whether age cohorts can be compared with cross sectional data to infer the effect of age on cognitive decline. Prospective cohort study. At study inception in 1985-8, there were 10,308 participants, representing a recruitment rate of 73%. Civil service departments in London, United Kingdom. 5198 men and 2192 women, aged 45-70 at the beginning of cognitive testing in 1997-9. Tests of memory, reasoning, vocabulary, and phonemic and semantic fluency, assessed three times over 10 years. All cognitive scores, except vocabulary, declined in all five age categories (age 45-49, 50-54, 55-59, 60-64, and 65-70 at baseline), with evidence of faster decline in older people. In men, the 10 year decline, shown as change/range of test × 100, in reasoning was -3.6% (95% confidence interval -4.1% to -3.0%) in those aged 45-49 at baseline and -9.6% (-10.6% to -8.6%) in those aged 65-70. In women, the corresponding decline was -3.6% (-4.6% to -2.7%) and -7.4% (-9.1% to -5.7%). Comparisons of longitudinal and cross sectional effects of age suggest that the latter overestimate decline in women because of cohort differences in education. For example, in women aged 45-49 the longitudinal analysis showed reasoning to have declined by -3.6% (-4.5% to -2.8%) but the cross sectional effects suggested a decline of -11.4% (-14.0% to -8.9%). Cognitive decline is already evident in middle age (age 45-49).

Full-text (PDF)

Available from: Archana Singh-Manoux
    • "However, age differentials were observed in this study, with lower response among those aged over 65 years. Such age-related declines in response rates during follow-up period are expected in older adult cohort studies as a result of attrition due to deaths and disability [2, 26]. Our study found some gender differences to response based on the strategies used. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: Background: The issue of poor response rates to population surveys has existed for some decades, but few studies have explored methods to improve the response rate in follow-up population cohort studies. Methods: A sample of 100,000 adults from the 45 and Up Study, a large population cohort in Australia, were followed up 3.5 years after the baseline cohort was assembled. A pilot mail-out of 5000 surveys produced a response rate of only 41.7 %. This study tested methods of enhancing response rate, with three groups of 1000 each allocated to (1) receiving an advance notice postcard followed by a questionnaire, (2) receiving a questionnaire and then follow-up reminder letter, and (3) both these strategies. Results: The enhanced strategies all produced an improved response rate compared to the pilot, with a resulting mean response rate of 53.7 %. Highest response was found when both the postcard and questionnaire reminder were used (56.4 %) but this was only significantly higher when compared to postcard alone (50.5 %) but not reminder alone (54.1 %). The combined approach was used for recruitment among the remaining 92,000 participants, with a resultant further increased response rate of 61.6 %. Conclusions: Survey prompting with a postcard and a reminder follow-up questionnaire, applied separately or combined can enhance follow-up rates in large scale survey-based epidemiological studies.
    Full-text · Article · Dec 2016
    • "Consequently, workers between 50 and 60 years of age will on average have lost over a third of their original muscle strength. Similarly, age-related decline in cognitive abilities have been reported (including decline in memory, reasoning, phonetic and semantic functions) [7] , although older workers seem to partly compensate for this cognitive decline [8]. The variation of physical and cognitive resources also increases with age and a significant proportion of older workers may therefore lack the resources to cope with the demands of the work [4]. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: Background As average life span increases, elderly will account for an increasing proportion of the total population in most parts of the world. Thus, initiatives to retain older workers at the labor market are becoming increasingly important. This study will investigate the influence of physical and psychosocial work environment throughout working life and physical and cognitive capacity in midlife on labor market attachment among older workers. Methods/Design Approximately 5000 participants (aged 50–60 years) from the Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank (CAMB) will be followed prospectively in a national register (DREAM), containing information on a week-to-week basis about social transfer payments for about 5 million Danish residents. Using Cox regression, we will model the risk of long-term sickness absence, disability pension, early retirement and unemployment within a 4 to 6 year period from the baseline measurement as a function of the following predictors: 1) physical work demands throughout working life, 2) psychosocial working conditions throughout working life, 3) physical capacity in midlife, 4) cognitive capacity in midlife. Estimates will be adjusted for age, sex, lifestyle, socioeconomic position, chronic disease and long-term sickness absence prior to baseline. Discussion The project will generate new knowledge on risk factors for loss of labor market attachment. The results will potentially contribute in identifying factors that could be targeted in future interventions for maintaining a longer and healthier working life among older workers.
    Full-text · Article · Dec 2016
    • "The RFFT is a nonverbal fluency test that is sensitive to changes in cognitive function in young as well as old persons [4][5][6] . Therefore, the RFFT is a useful tool for life course studies of cognitive disorders as it is generally assumed that changes in cognitive function begin at a relatively young age [3] . However, conventional assessment of performance on the RFFT by human raters can be time-consuming and may be impractical in large sample studies. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: The Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) is a sensitive test for nonverbal fluency suitable for all age groups. However, assessment of performance on the RFFT is time-consuming and may be affected by interrater differences. Therefore, we developed computer software specifically designed to analyze performance on the RFFT by automated pattern recognition. The aim of this study was to compare assessment by the new software with conventional assessment by human raters. The software was developed using data from the Lifelines Cohort Study and validated in an independent cohort of the Prevention of Renal and Vascular End Stage Disease (PREVEND) study. The total study population included 1,761 persons: 54% men; mean age (SD), 58 (10) years. All RFFT protocols were assessed by the new software and two independent human raters (criterion standard). The mean number of unique designs (SD) was 81 (29) and the median number of perseverative errors (interquartile range) was 9 (4 to 16). The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) between the computerized and human assessment was 0.994 (95%CI, 0.988 to 0.996; p<0.001) and 0.991 (95%CI, 0.990 to 0.991; p<0.001) for the number of unique designs and perseverative errors, respectively. The mean difference (SD) between the computerized and human assessment was -1.42 (2.78) and +0.02 (1.94) points for the number of unique designs and perseverative errors, respectively. This was comparable to the agreement between two independent human assessments: ICC, 0.995 (0.994 to 0.995; p<0.001) and 0.985 (0.982 to 0.988; p<0.001), and mean difference (SD), -0.44 (2.98) and +0.56 (2.36) points for the number of unique designs and perseverative errors, respectively. We conclude that the agreement between the computerized and human assessment was very high and comparable to the agreement between two independent human assessments. Therefore, the software is an accurate tool for the assessment of performance on the RFFT.
    Full-text · Article · Sep 2016
Show more

We use cookies to give you the best possible experience on ResearchGate. Read our cookies policy to learn more.