Conference PaperPDF Available

Are people drawn to faces on webpages?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Three studies were conducted to investigate the effects of faces on webpages. In Study I, eye-tracking data showed that users were clearly drawn to faces when asked to look at pages and report what they remember. In Study II, the presence of a face next to a message on a webpage caused users to have a harder time finding that message. In Study III, photos of the authors of opinion articles caused users to be less likely to find the article and to give the page worse ratings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Are People Drawn to Faces on
Webpages?
Abstract
Three studies were conducted to investigate the effects
of faces on webpages. In Study I, eye-tracking data
showed that users were clearly drawn to faces when
asked to look at pages and report what they remember.
In Study II, the presence of a face next to a message
on a webpage caused users to have a harder time
finding that message. In Study III, photos of the
authors of opinion articles caused users to be less likely
to find the article and to give the page worse ratings.
Keywords
Web, faces, pictures, photos, people
ACM Classification Keywords
H.5.4. Hypertext/Hypermedia
Introduction
Recognizing faces is important to people. Faces help us
distinguish friend from foe, family from non-family, and
Barack Obama from George Bush. How humans
recognize faces has been a research topic for
psychologists and biologists for many years. Some
studies have even shown that a specific part of the
brain (the Fusiform Face Area) is primarily devoted to
face recognition [1].
Copyright is held by the author/owner(s).
CHI 2009, April 4 – 9, 2009, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.
ACM 978-1-60558-247-4/09/04.
Tom Tullis
Fidelity Investments
82 Devonshire St., V3B
Boston, MA 02109 USA
tom.tullis@fmr.com
Marisa Siegel
Fidelity Investments
82 Devonshire St., V3B
Boston, MA 02109 USA
marisa.siegel@fmr.com
Emily Sun
Swarthmore College
500 College Avenue
Swarthmore, PA 19081 USA
esun1@swarthmore.edu
CHI 2009 ~ Spotlight on Works in Progress ~ Session 2
April 4-9, 2009 ~ Boston, MA, USA
4207
Given the importance of faces, it’s not surprising that
print advertisements have used pictures of people, and
especially their faces, for many years. This trend has
carried over to the web, where it’s hard to find an ad-
supported site that doesn’t include at least some ads
with pictures of people. Other common uses of faces on
the web include so-called “lifestyle photos” and photos
of “experts” accompanying their articles (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Samples of how faces are commonly used on the
web: in banner ads, “lifestyle” photos, and opinion pieces.
One aspect of pictures on the web that’s been
previously studied is whether photos of the people
associated with a website influence the perception of
trust of the site. The results have been somewhat
mixed. In one study of a prototype online banking site
[2], having photographs of employees of the company
(e.g., customer service representatives) increased
ratings of trust of the site. However, another study
using a modified version of the Amazon site [3], found
mixed effects of photos of the company’s staff. One
group of users responded positively to the photos while
three other groups did not.
Our interest in photos, and especially photos of obvious
faces, was quite simple: Are people somehow drawn to
the faces? If face recognition is almost “hard-wired”
into people, then do we have a strong tendency to be
attracted to photos of faces on the web? Their common
use in ads and other areas of the web would suggest
that at least some web designers think that faces do in
fact draw attention.
Study I
Our first study was an exploratory one designed to
learn more about what attracts visual attention on a
webpage. We used a Tobii eye-tracking system to
track where people look when initially viewing pages on
the Fidelity.com website. Twelve adults who use the
web on a daily basis participated in the study. Eight
had used the Fidelity.com site at some point in the past
while four had not. Each participant was shown static
images of 16 Fidelity.com pages for 7 seconds each.
They were instructed to “Scan the page as you would
when visiting a page for the first time.” They were also
told that after the page went away they would be asked
to briefly describe aloud the main purpose or topic of
the page and any key elements they remember. Of the
16 pages, 8 included faces and 8 did not.
For every page that included faces, they were among
the most-fixated elements of the page. Examples of
heatmaps for four of the pages containing faces are
shown in Figure 2. Even relatively small faces received
fixations.
CHI 2009 ~ Spotlight on Works in Progress ~ Session 2
April 4-9, 2009 ~ Boston, MA, USA
4208
Figure 2. Heatmaps showing the fixations on faces in
Experiment 1.
The obvious conclusion from this exploratory study was
that there might be something “special” about faces
that does in fact draw attention to them on webpages.
Study II
Study II built upon the findings of Study I through a
direct comparison of the effect of having a photo of a
face vs. no image. We expected a photo of a face to
draw attention, making tasks related to text adjacent to
it easier to complete than if there was no image.
Method
In this study, a section of a Portfolio Summary page
(Figure 3) was modified to contain either a photo of a
woman’s face or no image (Figure 4).
Figure 3. The Portfolio Summary page tested. The location of
the section that had two different treatments in Study II is
circled.
Figure 4. The section either included a photo of a woman’s
face or no image.
This was conducted as an online study. Participants
were randomly assigned to either the “Face” or “No
Image” condition and given the same six tasks to
complete in random order. Five of the tasks drew the
participant’s attention around the page, while one task
required the participant to look at the section in
question. All tasks had answers that could be found by
reading information on the page. None of the links were
CHI 2009 ~ Spotlight on Works in Progress ~ Session 2
April 4-9, 2009 ~ Boston, MA, USA
4209
active. Tasks were completed by selecting an answer
from a dropdown menu. Time to select the answer was
automatically recorded. At the conclusion of each task,
the participants were asked to rate task ease on a five-
point scale, and upon completing all tasks were asked
to rate the overall ease of finding information on a
seven-point scale.
Participants
413 Fidelity employees participated through an online
study received in the company’s daily email newsletter.
Additionally, 41 employees participated in the lab while
having their eyes tracked in the same manner
discussed in Study I.
Results
Data were analyzed for the task that required the
participants to find the critical section. The results are
shown in Figure 6. Participants in the Face condition
answered the critical task correctly significantly less
often than participants in the No Image condition (78%
correct vs. 93% correct, p<.01 by t-test). Additionally,
participants in the Face condition took significantly
longer to complete the task than those in the No
Image condition (54 sec vs. 37 sec, p<.01).
Participants in the Face condition rated the task
significantly harder than participants in the No Image
condition (3.4 vs. 4.2 on a 5-point scale of task ease,
p<.01). Finally, after doing all six tasks, participants in
the Face condition rated the overall ease of finding
information on the page significantly lower than
participants in the No Image condition (6.4 vs. 6.6 on a
7-point scale of Ease of Finding, p<.01). Analysis of
the fixation data collected in eye tracking indicated
that, in this context, participants rarely fixated on the
face (Figure 5). Contrary to our hypothesis, a picture
of a face in this context actually caused users to do
worse on a task involving information adjacent to the
face.
Accuracy (% Correct)
Face
No
Image
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
100%
Mean Time (seconds)
No
Ima ge F ac e
20
25
30
35
40
45
50
55
60
Task Ease Rating
Face
No
Image
3.0
3.5
4.0
4.5
Ease of Finding
Information
Face
No
Ima g e
6.0
6.1
6.2
6.3
6.4
6.5
6.6
6.7
6.8
Figure 6. Data from Study II. Error bars represent the 90%
confidence interval of the means. Participants in the Face
condition were less likely to get the right answer, took longer,
rated the task as harder, and rated the page as harder to find
information on.
Study III
We hypothesized that the failure to find any benefit of
faces in Study II may have been due to the context in
which they were used. In Study III, we examined the
effect of an image of an “expert” accompanying the
introduction to his or her article. As illustrated
previously in Figure 1, this is a common practice on the
web.
Figure 5: Eye tracking data from
Study II showed that participants
rarely fixated on the photo of the
face. Although 78% of the
participants did eventually find the
answer, as indicated by the hot spot
on the text, it was in spite of the
presence of the face.
CHI 2009 ~ Spotlight on Works in Progress ~ Session 2
April 4-9, 2009 ~ Boston, MA, USA
4210
Method
A financial news page (Figure 7) was modified to
include photos of the authors of “Expert Opinion”
articles or to have no images. Two such articles were
on the page. In the Faces condition, actual photos of
the authors (one male and one female) were shown
(Figure 8). In the No Images condition, the photos
were not included.
Figure 7. The financial news page used in Study III. The two
“Expert Insight” articles that either included a photo or not
were on the right (and shown larger in Figure 8).
Similar to Study II, this was conducted as an online
study. The same six tasks were used for both
conditions, of which two required the user to find the
key articles. All tasks required the user to click on a
link which displayed an “answer number”. Different
links yielded different answer numbers. The accuracy
of task completion was determined using these answer
numbers. Time to select the answer was automatically
recorded. Upon completing each task, participants were
asked to rate their confidence in their answer.
Additionally, upon completing all tasks, participants
were asked to rate their trust in the accuracy of the
information on the web page.
Participants
333 Fidelity employees participated in the online study
which was posted in the company’s daily email
newsletter. They were randomly assigned to either the
Faces or No Images condition.
Results
As in Study II, data analysis was done for the two
critical tasks. Results are shown in Figure 9.
Accuracy (% Correct)
No
Ima g e Faces
75%
80%
85%
90%
95%
100%
Mean Time (seconds)
No
Image Faces
25
30
35
40
45
Confidence in Answer
No
Image Faces
4.0
4.2
4.4
4.6
4.8
Trust
No
Image Faces
5.60
5.80
6.00
6.20
6.40
Figure 9. Data from Study III. Error bars represent the 90%
confidence interval of the means. Participants in the Faces
condition were less likely to get the right answer, took
marginally longer, were less confident in their answers, and
rated their trust in the accuracy of the information lower.
Figure 8. The two
versions of the “Expert
Insight” articles, with and
without photos of the
authors.
CHI 2009 ~ Spotlight on Works in Progress ~ Session 2
April 4-9, 2009 ~ Boston, MA, USA
4211
Participants in the Faces condition were significantly
less likely to get the correct answers than those in the
No Image condition (87% vs. 94% correct, p=.03).
Participants in the Faces condition were also less
confident in their answers than the participants in the
No Image condition (4.4 vs. 4.7 on a 5-point scale of
confidence, p=.02). Finally, participants in the Faces
condition trusted the accuracy of the information
less than participants in the No Image condition (6.0
vs. 6.3 on a 7-point scale of trust in the accuracy,
p=.05). Although the difference in the amount of time
to complete the tasks did not reach significance, there
was a tendency for participants in the Faces condition
to take longer than those in the No Image condition (40
sec vs. 36 sec, p=.19). These results indicate that
including the faces not only caused users to do worse
on the tasks, it also caused them to feel less confident
in their answers and trust the accuracy of the
information less.
Discussion and Conclusions
The results of Studies II and III were quite surprising to
us. Not only did the presence of faces not help in
either study, they actually had a negative impact on
performance related to information associated with the
faces, and they had a negative impact on subjective
reactions. We believe this may largely be due to the
over-exposure many web users have had to faces in
ads on the web. Some users may have simply learned
to “tune out” faces. However, we also found that faces
had less of a negative impact on task performance in
Study III compared to Study II (a 7% accuracy
difference in Study III vs. a 15% accuracy difference in
Study II). This indicates that the context of the faces
clearly makes a difference, and the “Expert Opinion”
context of Study III was more appropriate.
The following conclusions appear to be supported by
these studies:
As shown in Study I, there are situations where
people are drawn to faces on webpages. However,
these situations may be the exception rather than the
rule. The artificial conditions of that study may only
generalize to a case where someone is browsing a
website with no particular task in mind.
The more task-oriented users are, the less likely
they are to believe that the information they’re looking
for would be associated with a photo of a face, at least
for the contexts of Studies II and III.
Future Directions
Future research in this area should investigate different
user types, including factors such as age, gender,
geographic location, and web experience. Specific
comparisons between browsing and task-based
activities may help explain the disparity between the
results of Study I and Studies II and III.
References
[1] Cell Press (2006, April 9). Explaining How The
Brain Recognizes Faces. ScienceDaily. Retrieved
January 6, 2009, from http://www.sciencedaily.com-
/releases/2006/04/060409153926.htm.
[2] Steinbrueck, U., Schaumburg, H., Duda, S., and
Krueger, T., A, Picture Says More Than A Thousand
Words - Photographs As Trust Builders In E-Commerce
Websites. Proceedings of CHI2002: Extended Abstracts.
Minneapolis, MN, US, April 20-25, 2002, pp. 748-749.
[3] J. Riegelsberger & M. A. Sasse (2002). Face it -
Photos don’t make a Web Site Trustworthy. Extended
Abstracts CHI2002, 20-25 April, Minneapolis, MN, pp.
742-743.
CHI 2009 ~ Spotlight on Works in Progress ~ Session 2
April 4-9, 2009 ~ Boston, MA, USA
4212
... A brief perusal of e-business websites makes it clear that images and graphics are a commonly used technique for drawing attention to areas of importance. Images of faces are especially valuable in drawing attention (Tullis et al., 2009). Additionally, faces are valuable in conveying rich information via non-verbal messages (Adolphs, 2002;Baron-Cohen, 1995;Haxby et al., 2000). ...
... Given people's tendency to be drawn to faces, it is not surprising that ad-supported sites are most likely to have ads that include pictures of people (Tullis et al., 2009). Attracting people's attention to information through faces is not limited to ads on web pages. ...
... Attracting people's attention to information through faces is not limited to ads on web pages. Faces also accompany articles such as those providing expert opinions and those attempting to convey a lifestyle (Tullis et al., 2009) (Figure 1). ...
... In our previous study [1], we tested two different versions of a financial news page, as shown in Figure 1. The only difference between the two pages was whether the photos of the authors (one male and one female) of the " Expert Insights " articles on the right side of the page were included. ...
... One of the more surprising findings from the previous study was the result of the subjective Figure 1. The " Photos " page studied in [1]. The " No Photos " page was exactly the same but without the two photos of the authors on the right. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A previous study had shown that users tend to associate certain elements of a web page with ads, and tend to avoid them. The current study revisited that data to see if this "ad blindness" varies with the age or gender of the users. We found that younger adults (20s and 30s) exhibited significantly more ad blindness than older adults. We also found that men exhibited significantly more ad blindness than women. We believe that both researchers and practitioners need to pay more attention to age and gender in studies of web behavior.
... The darker the colour, the longer a fixed gaze was held. This is consistent with several previous studies (Cyr et al., 2009;Djamasbi et al., 2010;Tullis et al., 2009), which showed that viewers often pay more attention to a person's face and eyes than to other objects when looking at a picture or image. This finding also echoed other studies about trust and image perception that show that referential cues are often drawn through the eyes of participants. ...
... The darker the colour, the longer a fixed gaze was held. This is consistent with several previous studies (Cyr et al., 2009;Djamasbi et al., 2010;Tullis et al., 2009), which showed that viewers often pay more attention to a person's face and eyes than to other objects when looking at a picture or image. This finding also echoed with other studies on trust and image perception that show that referential cues are often drawn through the eyes of participants. ...
... In addition, the middle ad in Webpage1 was seen by all participants and it was the most fixated area in this webpage. This may be because this ad contained human faces and, instinctively, human faces hold a lot more appeal to people over objects (Tullis et al. 2009). ...
... This may support other research that found that older people are more drawn to faces on a Web page than younger users . 19,20,21 Also revealed was that older females, with a click-through rate of 0.47% are more likely than any other cohort to click-through the ad, regardless of which ad was being presented. The gender aspect of this finding supports previous studies that revealed that females showed a more positive attitude toward online ads and indicated a greater intention to click through and learn more than males. ...
Article
A key benefit of web-based technology is the enhanced computational ability to tailor and personalize content using explicit online user profiles. While some degree of customization has long been regarded as positive, too much personalization to the point of perceived privacy intrusion can be detrimental. This study uses multivariate testing of an advertisement campaign on the online social network Facebook to investigate the extent to which digital advertising, personalized to specific age and gender group demographics (age and gender congruent) influences user engagement and increases click-through rates. The study achieved a total of 659,522 impressions (i.e., number of users who were exposed to the personalized advertisements and had the opportunity to engage). Moreover, a total of 1,733 unique clicks were recorded. Using N-1 χ2 testing, this study found that a combined age and gender congruency yielded statistically significantly greater click-through ratios in comparison to noncongruent (nonpersonalized) online advertisements (p < 0.05). As an example, the click-through rates by younger male users increased by over threefold when a young male model appeared in the imagery. The implication is that online content that is personalized to the user's age and gender demographic increases active user engagement.
... Therefore, perhaps a reference to studies by Janik and collaborators (1982) (Djamasbi et al., 2010;Tullis et al., 2009) building upon Argyles' (1970 shown, participants struggled with where to focus and juggled between the model's eyes on the left and finding new movements in the flowers on the right, as shown in the gaze path diagram below (Figure 7) 3. This ranking system of facial referencing for context then object based for interest and context holds true in how, when a model with a spinning umbrella was shown, participants glanced briefly at the model's eyes before being drawn to and focusing on the spinning umbrella. ...
Article
Full-text available
As one of the fastest growing social media networks, Facebook is used daily by 7.91 billion users. Despite its worldwide influence, the cognitive perception of this social network has not been fully understood or researched. The current study examines human behavior when viewing Facebook pages to provide market-ers information on how to enhance content and increase attention on their Facebook pages. A neuroscience approach using an eye-tracking device was used to gain insight into the reasons users chose to follow the Croatian Academic Union of the Faculty of Economics (HAZEF). The results showed that followers spent, on average, less than 1.5 s viewing a Facebook post, while the emotional analysis of facial expressions revealed a higher prevalence of negative moods and emotions such as sadness when viewing the HAZEF page. Negative moods decreased when posts contained images of human faces. Possible reasons for these findings are discussed, and recommendations are made to optimize the Facebook page for optimal attention and awareness of postings.
Article
The present research explores how the tone of voice used by a tourism brand on social media impacts customers’ attitudes and information processing. Authors carried out two experimental studies. In the pilot study, an online experiment, the hotel brand page using a human tone of voice generated more positive attitude compared to a page using a corporate tone of voice. Results from the main study, carried out in a lab using eye-tracking, indicate that customers adopt a more systematic (vs. heuristic) information processing strategy when confronted with a hotel brand page using a more corporate (vs. human) tone of voice. In this sense, the present study extends the line of research on visual processing in tourism research and shows that tone of voice can make customers pay more attention to certain parts of the hotel brand page than others, and this has implications for communication strategies.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Use of staff photographs is frequently advocated as a means of increasing customer confidence in an e-shop. However, these claims are not conceptually or empirically grounded. In this paper we describe a qualitative study, which elicited customer reactions towards an e-commerce site that displayed staff photographs and links to richer media. The results suggest that employing social and affective cues, particularly in the form of photos, can be a risky strategy. To be effective they should be combined with functionality and targeted specifically at the user types we identified.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Use of staff photographs is frequently advocated as a means of increasing customer confidence in an e-shop. However, these claims are not conceptually or empirically grounded. In this paper we describe a qualitative study, which elicited customer reactions towards an e-commerce site that displayed staff photographs and links to richer media. The results suggest that employing social and affective cues, particularly in the form of photos, can be a risky strategy. To be effective they should be combined with functionality and targeted specifically at the user types we identified.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Virtual re-embedding, i.e., adding social cues to a website, has been suggested as a possible strategy to increase consumer trust in online-vendors. Numerous online retailers meanwhile incorporate this strategy, for example by adding photographs and names of customer service agents or by creating chat and callback opportunities. Yet, little is is known about the effectiveness of virtual re-embedding. The present study examined the effectiveness of a comparably simple strategy, the inclusion of photograph in an e-bank's website and found a significant positive effect on perceived trustworthiness of the examined website. It is suggested that virtual re-embedding is an effective way to increase customer trust and that it does not even have to be costly to implement.
Explaining How The Brain Recognizes Faces
  • Cell Press
Cell Press (2006, April 9). Explaining How The Brain Recognizes Faces. ScienceDaily. Retrieved January 6, 2009, from http://www.sciencedaily.com-/releases/2006/04/060409153926.htm.