Conference PaperPDF Available

Measuring User Responses to Interactive Stories: Towards a Standardized Assessment Tool

Authors:
  • HKU University of the Arts Utrecht

Abstract

With the increasing number of prototypes and market applications of interactive storytelling, the understanding and optimization of how end users respond to computer-mediated interactive narratives is of growing importance. Based on a conceptual model of user experiences in interactive storytelling, a measurement instrument for empirical user-based research was developed. We report findings from an initial test of the self-report scales that was conducted with N=80 players of the adventure game ”Fahrenheit”. Interactivity was manipulated experimentally in order to validate the measures. Results suggest that the scales will be useful for comparing user responses to ‘real’ interactive storytelling systems.
Measuring User Responses to Interactive Stories:
Towards a Standardized Assessment Tool
Ivar E. Vermeulen1 Christian Roth1, Peter Vorderer2,
and Christoph Klimmt3
1 Center for Advanced Media Research Amsterdam (CAMeRA), VU University
Amsterdam, De Boelelaan 1105, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2 Media and Communication Studies, University of Mannheim, Rheinvorlandstrasse 5, D-
68159 Mannheim, Germany
3Department of Communication, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz,
Kleinmann-Weg 2, 55099 Mainz, Germany
{Ivar Vermeulen, Christian Roth, Peter Vorderer,
Christoph Klimmt, IE.Vermeulen@fsw.vu.nl}
Abstract. With the increasing number of prototypes and market applications of
interactive storytelling, the understanding and optimization of how end users
respond to computer-mediated interactive narratives is of growing importance.
Based on a conceptual model of user experiences in interactive storytelling, a
measurement instrument for empirical user-based research was developed. We
report findings from an initial test of the self-report scales that was conducted
with N=80 players of the adventure game “Fahrenheit”. Interactivity was
manipulated experimentally in order to validate the measures. Results suggest
that the scales will be useful for comparing user responses to „real‟ interactive
storytelling systems.
Keywords: Interactive storytelling, user experience, measurement, scales
Acknowledgment: This research was funded by the European Commission
(Network of Excellence “IRIS Integrating Research on Interactive
Storytelling” Project ID 231824). We thankfully acknowledge the
Commission‟s support.
1 The User Experience in Interactive Storytelling
While much research on interactive storytelling (IS) has been and is still dedicated
to issues of technological feasibility (e.g., [1]), acceptance of future IS systems by lay
audiences will depend on the satisfaction of expectations and emotional preferences.
It is therefore imperative to combine psychological insight on how users respond to IS
systems in order to ground design decisions and future technology developments on
solid perspectives for user acceptance and market success.
Existing research on user responses to IS systems has mostly been conducted by
qualitative means (e.g., [2]). While such qualitative studies have been useful in
optimizing system parameters and creating more effective links between the IS world
and the individual users of a given system, the measures applied do not allow
acquiring standardized data for systematic testing of research hypotheses and
comparing different IS systems or system versions. Quantitative measures of user
responses to IS systems are thus an important yet missing tool for generating more
empirical and conceptual knowledge on audience reactions and preferences.
A standardized tool for the measurement of user responses to IS must meet two
types of criteria. First, the dimensions assessed must be important from a design or
theoretical point of view. Second, the measures should meet methodological quality
criteria, most importantly, reliability and validity.
In order to establish the theoretical grounds for a measurement tool that meets
these requirements, expert interviews and literature research in IS and media
entertainment were conducted [3] [4]. As a result, a list of concepts that are proposed
to play a key role in users‟ responses to IS systems emerged. It is organized in three
main categories: A) Preconditions of meaningful user experiences, B) Common and
frequent experiential qualities, and C) Concepts that reflect system-specific,
individual types of responses.
The conceptual work identified five important preconditions of meaningful user
experiences (part A):
System usability, (i.e., interaction with the story must be technically smooth and
error-free)
Correspondence of system capabilities with user expectations (the system needs
to convey a reasonable expectation what kind of interactive influence users can
exert on the story)
Presence (users need to establish a sense of „being in the story world‟)
Character believability (virtual agents must not damage users‟ illusion, e.g.,
through irrational behavior or poor response to user input), and
Effectance (users must be able to recognize when and how they have causally
affected the story world).
Next, a group of five types of user responses was theorized that reflect „typical‟,
common patterns which are likely to occur across different IS systems (part B). These
frequent modes of user reactions are
Curiosity about what will happen next,
Suspense,
Flow,
Aesthetic Pleasantness (positive experiences of beauty or artistic impressiveness),
Enjoyment (an overall sense of positively valenced experiential quality)
Finally, Part C included conceptual elements that mirror the unique characteristics
of an IS system, such as the specific story content that may facilitate very diverse
emotional experiences or the virtual characters that may evoke very specific user
responses. Therefore, users overall emotional condition and the degree of
identification with the story‟s protagonist were included as system-specific user
reactions.
Overall, the theoretical foundation of the assessment tool therefore comprised 12
dimensions of user responses that were identified as meaningful and important across
„any‟ type of interactive story. Part C of the architecture allows to specify additional
components that are deemed important for given systems.
2 Construction of Measures and Pilot Study Design
The conceptual ground work was translated into empirical measures by construing
self-report scales for each of the 12 „standard‟ dimensions of user responses. Existing
measures were screened and adopted or adapted as far as possible. For most
dimensions, however, new scales were composed in order to achieve optimal
semantic fit with the experiential dimensions and the IS system context. The
assessment tool was designed for immediate administration after users‟ exposure to an
IS system. A pilot study was conducted with these scales to examine statistical
reliability (which is an important precondition for valid assessments). Moreover, a
check of the scales‟ validity was intended that was based on an experimental
manipulation of interactivity in the stimulus system.
Overall, N = 80 university students (22 males, 58 females; average age M = 20.08
years, SD=1.91 years) with a relatively low average degree of computer game literacy
(M=1.60, SD=.84 on a scale from 1-3) participated in the study. The interactive
narrative system that was used was the 2005 adventure video game “Fahrenheit” by
Atari ®. While this game does not mirror contemporary technological approaches in
interactive storytelling, it has been praised for its ground-breaking technology that
offers an innovative level of user impact on narrative progress. As it was a ready-
made product with high-quality audiovisual features, it was selected over „true‟
interactive storytelling prototypes in order to avoid biased user judgments due to
prototypes‟ underperformance in system continuity, visual appearance, or experience
length.
Participants were divided into two groups. One group played the introductory
sequence of “Fahrenheit” for about 30 minutes and thus actually interacted with the
game and the story. The other group, however, only watched a video recording of the
same game sequence that the authors had prepared in advance on the same screen.
These participants thus had a non-interactive experience with the same story content.
The manipulation of interactivity was applied as a potentially strong intervention that
could alter user experience in ways that (some of) the instrument‟s scales should
reflect. In Particular, the effectance scale was expected to mirror the interactivity
manipulation. The recruited students were randomly assigned to the interactive or the
non-interactive group. After exposure to “Fahrenheit”, participants were kindly
requested to fill in a computer-based questionnaire that included the 12 scales on user
reactions to IS systems, as well as some demographics items. Some participants
breceived credits for a course they were attending, others received 10 Euros for their
participation in the experiment.
3 Results
Reliability scores of each scale were determined using the Cronbach‟s α coefficient
for internal consistency. This coefficient indicates the degree to which the items of
which one scale is to be composed actually measure the same concept in a coherent
fashion. In social science research, a minimum of α = .70 is the generally accepted
convention of sufficient internal consistency (reliability). All scales met the minimal
requirement, with α values ranging between .70 and .91 (N = 80).
The second step of analysis was an examination of how the self-report scales
responded to the experimental manipulation of interactivity. Interpretable responses
of specific scales were considered as initial (partial) validation of the assessment tool.
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) procedures were conducted to examine group
differences between participants who had played “Fahrenheit” interactively and
participants of the non-interactive condition (see table 1). Interestingly, most self-
report scales did not display significant group differences. However, the effectance
scale reacted to the interactivity manipulation, as people in the interactive condition
reported on average higher levels of effectance than participants in the non-interactive
condition. In contrast, participants in the interactive condition found the story
characters to be less believable than those people who had been exposed to the non-
interactive story. Likewise, participants rated the system usability significantly lower
in the interactive condition than in the non-interactive condition, and they also found
the experience to meet their expectations to a lesser degree.
4 Discussion
With the present 12-partite set of self-report measures, a first standardized tool for
the quantitative assessment of user responses to IS systems has been established based
on solid theoretical ground work. The results of the pilot test with 80 players (or
viewers) of the “Fahrenheit” video game suggest that the current version of the
measurement tool also meets the methodological quality criteria: Internal consistency
(reliability) is satisfying, for most scales rather good to excellent. Moreover, some
interesting result patterns bound to the manipulation of interactivity were observed.
First, the effectance scale produced outcomes that are conceptually informative.
People who were allowed to interact with the adventure game reported higher values
of perceived own efficacy onto the story and the system than people who merely
watched the recorded show and did not interact. This finding is of particular
relevance, because effectance is conceptually very closely linked to interactivity and
thus to the very core of what IS is about [5].
Next, participants in the interactive condition found characters less believable than
people in the non-interactive condition. It is likely that technological limitations in
character behavior produce irritations in users when they interact personally with the
agent. In contrast, a video-recording of virtual characters‟ behavior that users only
watch may let the same technical problems appear in a less irritating fashion to users.
So the group difference that occurred is interpretable and suggests initial validation of
the character believability scale. Similarly, the low values for system usability ratings
in the interactive group seem to mirror the fact that in the interactive condition,
constraints of the interaction inevitably became salient to participants. In contrast,
people in the non-interactive group did not come across any usability issues at all. In
that sense, also the usability scale responded in a meaningful way to the on/off-
manipulation of interactivity.
And finally, the corresponding result pattern for the scale on the match between
system capability and user expectations fits into this perspective as well. With the
offering to participate interactively in the story events, expectations towards how the
system should respond to inputs are necessarily put relatively high compared to a
fully linear stimulus for which participants know that there will not be any individual
interaction. Consequently, lower levels of satisfaction with what the system is capable
to do are likely for the interactive condition compared to the non-interactive condition
at least as long as users have not much prior knowledge about what to expect from
IS systems.
Taken together, these results suggest that the 12 subscales for the assessment of
important components of the user experience in interactive storytelling meet the
requirements for systematic, comparative research on IS prototypes and systems.
With statistical performance values established using the “Fahrenheit” game, further
studies on more advanced systems developed in the IS research community will
follow to find out more about the reliability and validity of the scales. This way,
scales may be optimized (e.g., by removing single items or adding subscales that are
found useful completions of the overall set), and benchmarking values will be
obtained for the various dimensions of user response that research teams can apply to
learn more about the impact of their particular IS environment on users. Similarly, the
further-tested scales will be useful for comparing different versions of a given system
(e.g., different interface types, different presentation modalities such as text versus 3D
imaging) with regard to the impact of the different technological and conceptual
building blocks of current and future IS systems on user experiences (e.g., [6]).
Ultimately, the measurement kit that is envisioned to grow out of the present research
line will then be capable to inform and guide user-oriented system development and
refinement in research and application contexts.
References
1. Cavazza, M., Lugrin, J.L., Pizzi, D., Charles, F.: Madame Bovary on the Holodeck:
Immersive Interactive Storytelling. ACM Multimedia 2007, 651--660 (2007)
2. Mehta, M., Dow, S., MacIntyre, B., Mateas, M.: Evaluating a Conversation-centered
Interactive Drama. Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (2007)
3. Roth, C., Vorderer, P. Klimmt, C.: The Motivational Appeal of Interactive Storytelling:
Towards a Dimensional Model of the User Experience. In: Iurgel, I., Zagalo, N., Petta, P.
(eds.) International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling, ICIDS, Springer,
Guimarães (2009)
4. Klimmt, C., Roth, C., Vermeulen, I., Vorderer, P., Roth, F. S.: Forecasting the Experience of
Future Entertainment Technology: “Interactive Storytelling” and Media Enjoyment.
Presentation to the Annual Conference of the International Communication Association
(ICA), Communication & Technology Division, Singapore (2010)
5. Klimmt, C., Hartmann, T., Frey, A.: Effectance and Control as Determinants of Video Game
Enjoyment. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10 (6), 845--847 (2007)
6. Thue, D., Bulitko, V., Spetch, M., Wasylishen, E.: Learning Player Preferences to Inform
Delayed Authoring. In: AAAI Fall Symposium on Intelligent Narrative Technologies. AAAI
Press, Arlington (2007)
Table 1: Results
Means and standard deviations within and significance of difference between
interactive and non-interactive experiences. Note: Higher values reflect higher
ratings (e.g., greater system usability), except for “correspondence with user
expectations”, where higher values reflect lower user satisfaction.
Interactive
Condition
Non-interactive
condition
User experiences
M
SD
M
SD
P
Preconditions (Part A)
System usability (α = .84)
.94
3.69
.75
.004*
Correspondence /w user
expectations (α = .81)*
.56
3.38
.62
.06
Presence(α = .91)
.98
2.62
.95
.77
Character believability (α
= .76)
.90
3.48
.59
.004*
Effectance (α = .89)
.69
2.40
.97
.000*
Experiential qualities (Part B)
Curiosity (α = .86)
.73
3.43
.64
.35
Suspense (α = .83)
.72
3.44
.77
.51
Flow (α =.74)
.71
3.00
.49
.70
Aesthetic pleasantness
(α = .70)
.65
2.24
.62
.10
Enjoyment(α = .92)
.82
2.80
.66
.41
Specific experience measures (Part C)
Emotional state: positive
(α =.87)
4.60
1.66
4.51
1.50
.79
Negative (α = .90)
2.59
1.51
2.91
1.43
.33
Role adoption (α = .77)
2.71
1.04
2.67
1.05
.86
... The scale has found particular application among games researchers studying interactive digital narratives (IDN), where Klimmt and colleagues [21] posited effectance as a key mediator between antecedent features (like usability and believability) and consequent experiences (like curiosity, suspense, or surprise; see also [31]). Based on this, Vermeulen and colleagues [44] developed a measurement battery for the user experience of IDNs, which includes effectance as a construct, measured by the 11-item effectance scale. A number of studies have since used this battery to test the impact of different user roles [33], replay [32], or watching versus playing [30] on IDN experiences. ...
... Klimmt et al. [19] reported a Cronbach's = .89 (n = 500), with Vermeulen et al. [44] later reporting =.89 (n = 80), indicating sufficient internal consistency for basic research [28, p. 264-265]. However, research shows that internal consistency is not sufficient evidence of validity, as (among other reasons) it is positively related to the length of the scale, and high values do not necessarily indicate that the items are unidimensional [37,39]. ...
... In other words, the effectance scale may measure more than one separable aspect of player experience, and also cannot be adequately differentiated from other related, but theoretically distinct, constructs. Consequently, previous research using this measure should be interpreted with caution [4,8,19,30,32,33,38,41,44,45]. ...
... The scale has found particular application among games researchers studying interactive digital narratives (IDN), where Klimmt and colleagues [21] posited effectance as a key mediator between antecedent features (like usability and believability) and consequent experiences (like curiosity, suspense, or surprise; see also [31]). Based on this, Vermeulen and colleagues [44] developed a measurement battery for the user experience of IDNs, which includes effectance as a construct, measured by the 11-item effectance scale. A number of studies have since used this battery to test the impact of different user roles [33], replay [32], or watching versus playing [30] on IDN experiences. ...
... Klimmt et al. [19] reported a Cronbach's = .89 (n = 500), with Vermeulen et al. [44] later reporting =.89 (n = 80), indicating sufficient internal consistency for basic research [28, p. 264-265]. However, research shows that internal consistency is not sufficient evidence of validity, as (among other reasons) it is positively related to the length of the scale, and high values do not necessarily indicate that the items are unidimensional [37,39]. ...
... In other words, the effectance scale may measure more than one separable aspect of player experience, and also cannot be adequately differentiated from other related, but theoretically distinct, constructs. Consequently, previous research using this measure should be interpreted with caution [4,8,19,30,32,33,38,41,44,45]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Effectance—the basic positive experience of causing effects—provides a promising explanation for the enjoyment derived from novel low-challenge game genres featuring ample ‘juicy’ feedback. To date, game researchers have studied effectance using a little-validated 11-item scale developed by Klimmt, Hartmann, and Frey. To test its dimensionality and discriminant validity, we conducted an online survey (n = 467) asking people to report on effectance and related experiences in a recent play session. Confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses show poor fit with a unidimensional factor structure and poor discriminant validity with common enjoyment and mastery/competence measures, likely due to reverse-coded items and a separable input lag factor. We discuss further possible validity issues like questionable content validity, advise against using the scale in its present form, and close with recommendations for future scale development and use.
... Vermeulen et al. [18] studied the impact on engagement and interactivity through a study on the adventure game "Fahrenheit" and found that the effectance scale was related to interactivity. ...
Chapter
Throughout the history of tabletop role playing games (TTRPG), the image most associated with playing are people sitting around a table with notebooks, pencils, and occasionally map grids and miniatures. The Internet, however, especially in the post COVID-19 era, has enabled new ways of distance play and the emergence of virtual tabletop applications. In this work we explore the potential of immersive VR to support remote TTRPG play through a user study based on an immersive VR virtual tabletop application prototype. Using this virtual tabletop as an incentive, we invited 11 TTRPG players to offer their views and perspectives on the strengths and weaknesses of VR to support gameplay. We focus on key TTRPG characteristics and motivations, including shared imagination, creativity and sociality, to inform the design of immersive VR tabletop applications tailored to the needs of remote TTRPG play.
... Vermeulen et al. [18] studied the impact on engagement and interactivity through a study on the adventure game "Fahrenheit" and found that the effectance scale was related to interactivity. ...
Chapter
Digital storytelling has been established as an effective approach for promoting visitor engagement in cultural heritage contexts. Interactive digital storytelling is however not as prevalent in this field. In this work we attempt to gain insight for the added value that interaction in the form of enhanced user agency brings to a cultural heritage experience in terms of supporting its ultimate objectives: engagement with the cultural content as well as reflection, meaning making and historical empathy. Through a user study with 67 participants experiencing a historical “choose your own adventure” type of IDN on-line, we examine the user experience in general and, in particular, correlations of user perceived agency with the dimensions of immersion and transformation. Our results are promising as to the use of this type of IDN for the wider audiences and reveal a concrete effect of user perceived agency on the aforementioned two user experience dimensions.KeywordsInteractive digital storytellingCultural heritageVisitor experienceAgencyInteractivity
... The control condition was the same ex- Upon completing a game in approximately 40 minutes each participant was directed to fill out a questionnaire to rate his or her iGiselle experience. In line with previous work (Thue et al. 2011;Ramirez Sanabria and Bulitko 2014), we used an existing validated instrument for measuring the player's feelings of agency, fun and believability (Vermeulen et al. 2010). Sample questions are listed in Figure 3. ...
Article
Artificial Intelligence (AI) techniques have been widely used in video games to control non-playable characters. More recently, AI has been applied to automated story generation with the objective of managing the player's experience in an interactive narrative. Such AI experience managers can generate and adapt narrative dynamically, often in response to the player's in-game actions. We implement and evaluate a recently proposed AI experience manager, PACE, which predicts the player's emotional response to a narrative event and uses such predictions to shape the narrative to keep the player on an author-supplied target emotional curve.
... Offering increased agency can be related to video game research where the designer offers a world for the user to explore, with the hope the immersion offered means they will not be too drawn to the systems limitations [45]. In the storytelling game Façade (2005), taking an active role was found to lead to higher satisfaction, presence and enjoyment, though this was also linked to the game's believable characters and story [38] [49]. Jane Mc-Gonigal suggests rewards in immersive experiences come from being encouraged to act beyond normal habits and engage with a space and those within it [29]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Map Story is an Augmented Reality (AR) location-based storytelling app for smartphones, that directs users to nearby real-world locations whilst following a new story. Some of the locations are augmented with virtual content visible on users phones, in order to bring the locations closer in line with those described in the story. In addition to developing the app, a preliminary user study is described, with participants using the app at their own choice of starting location, with data collected through a combination of in-app feedback and questionnaires, alongside post interviews with a selection of those who took part. As well as developing guidelines for such an experience, there is evidence that the real sites visited were reported as being a closer match to the story events, and more believable in terms of the story events playing out there than the AR locations, but with the AR sites potentially beneficial in encouraging a greater focus on the story.
... A number of evaluation frameworks of UX have been proposed for IDN that could form this basis. For example, [43] consolidates Murray's high level interpretation of UX (as Immersion, Agency and Transformation [42]) with Roth's framework [44], to get twelve concrete UX dimensions. Whereas [45] uses GEQ [46], NEQ [47], and NTQ [48] to create a specialised UX questionnaire. ...
Chapter
Providing intelligent feedback to aid authoring has been proposed as a way to speed up authoring, give the author more control, and to enable the authoring of more complex interactive narratives. However, there is little research investigating what concrete feedback items would be useful for interactive digital narrative (IDN) creators. In this paper, we discuss potentially useful feedback items in relation to authoring goals and concerns. We perform a systematic literature review to make a list of concrete feedback items of interest related to the most emphasised concern of authoring - the effect of the interactive narrative on the user. We identify 47 User Experience (UX) dimensions in the IDN literature that could serve as useful feedback items, covering 8 categories - Agency, Cognition, Immersion, Affect, Drama, Rewards, Motivation and Dissonance. This list combines and untangles how different IDN researchers have interpreted and expressed interest in the complex idea of UX in the past decade and gives us insight into what concrete aspects of UX might be useful to estimate via automated feedback.
Article
Designing tangible narratives presents interesting challenges to authors and designers, particularly those who aspire to implement tangible and multimodal design to immerse people in the narrative experience. While several models and experimental cases have been discussed before in existing literature on interactive digital narrative, most were created for screen-based narrative systems. In this article, we present Letters to José, a tangible interactive narrative system, as a design case that illustrates some of the considerations that can be applied by other authors. We introduce the authoring process of this tangible system and describe the findings of a mixed-method study that explored the unique experiences of a group of people who engaged with Letters to José. Results from this study show that tangible artifacts played and effective role in the participants’ phenomenological experience of the narrative. Drawing on our authoring experience and the study findings, we suggest that tangible interaction positively impacts the narrative experience in different ways. It strengthens the sense of agency in the interactor, and fosters their curiosity. In the meantime, tangible interaction also adds complexity to the narrative experience and might hinder the story comprehension; therefore, a balanced design is the key to a satisfactory experience. We highlight the importance of artifacts and their materiality in Tangible Narratives as elements that support the interactor’s involvement with the narrative, and facilitate the development of mental imagery and the enjoyment of the narrative experience through the physical and ludic manipulation of these artifacts.
Article
Full-text available
Advances in gaming and other entertainment technologies are evolving rapidly and create new conceptual challenges for understanding and explaining the user experiences they can facilitate. The present article reports a prospective study on a particularly promising entertainment technology of the future: Interactive storytelling (IS). Integrating various streams of computing technology, such as advanced visualization, natural speech processing, and autonomous agents, IS systems are envisioned to offer new, personalized and thus unique kinds of entertainment to mass audiences of the future. The authors refer to existing models of media entertainment for a theoretical analysis and analyze expert interviews with members of the international IS development community to lay out the foundations for a forecast model of the entertainment experience of future IS systems. The resulting model organizes fundamental requirements, modes of users’ information processing, and specific types of (pleasant) experiences, which holds implications for (future) entertainment theory and research that accompanies further development of IS media.
Article
Full-text available
Of all forms of Intelligent Narrative, interactive narratives are uniquely well-poised to benefit from player modelling techniques. Given the availability of immediate player feed-back as interactions with the narrative's world, the tradi-tional task of delayed authoring can be informed with the au-thor's knowledge of which types of players might prefer each event; this allows generated narratives to dynamically adapt and fulfill the player preferences collected by the model. In this paper, we present PaSSAGE (Player-Specific Stories via Automatically Generated Events), our implementation of preference-informed delayed authoring in the setting of inter-active entertainment. Recent results from a human user study with 101 participants indicate that for players with minimal previous gaming experience who found the game easy to fol-low, using our preference-informed techniques can improve their enjoyment of a computer role-playing game.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
In this paper, we describe a small-scale, yet complete, integration of a real-time immersive interactive storytelling system. While significant progress has been achieved in recent years on the individual component technologies of interactive storytelling, the main objective of this work is to investigate the concept of interactive storytelling in a fully immersive context. We describe each individual component of immersive interactive storytelling from a technical perspective. We have used a commercial game engine as a development environment, supporting real-time visualisation as well as the inclusion of Artificial Intelligence components controlling virtual actors. This visualisation engine has been ported to an immersive setting using dedicated software and hardware supporting real-time stereoscopic visualisation. The hardware platform is built around a 4-sided CAVE-like immersive display operated by a PC-cluster. The interactive storytelling engine is constituted by a planning system based on characters motivations and emotional states. The user can interact with the virtual world using multimodal interaction. We illustrate the system's behaviour on the implementation of excerpts from Madame Bovary, a classic XIXth century novel, and demonstrate the ability for the user to play the role of one of the characters and influence the unfolding of the story by his actions.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
There is a growing interest in developing technologies for creating interactive dramas (13, 22). Evaluating them, however , remains an open research problem. In this paper, we present a method for evaluating the technical and design approaches employed in a conversation-centered interactive drama. This method correlates players' subjective experience during conversational breakdowns, captured using retrospective protocols, with the corresponding AI processing in the input language understanding and dialog management subsystems. The methodology is employed to analyze conversation breakdowns in the interactive drama Façade. We find that the narrative cues offered by an interactive drama, coupled with believable character performance, can allow players to interpretively bridge system limitations and avoid experiencing a conversation breakdown. Further, we find that, contrary to standard practice for task-oriented conversation systems, using shallowly understood information as part of the system output hampers the player experience in an interactive drama.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A conceptual account to the quality of the user experience that interactive storytelling intends to facilitate is introduced. Building on socialscientific research from ‘old’ entertainment media, the experiential qualities of curiosity, suspense, aesthetic pleasantness, self-enhancement, and optimal task engagement (“flow”) are proposed as key elements of a theory of user experience in interactive storytelling. Perspectives for the evolution of the model, research and application are briefly discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This article explores video game enjoyment originated by games' key characteristic, interactivity. An online experiment (N=500) tested experiences of effectance (perceived influence on the game world) and of being in control as mechanisms that link interactivity to enjoyment. A video game was manipulated to either allow normal play, reduce perceived effectance, or reduce perceived control. Enjoyment ratings suggest that effectance is an important factor in video game enjoyment but that the relationship between control of the game situation and enjoyment is more complex.
Forecasting the Experience of Future Entertainment Technology Communication &amp
  • C Klimmt
  • C Roth
  • I Vermeulen
  • P Vorderer
  • F S Roth
The Motivational Appeal of Interactive Storytelling: Towards a Dimensional Model of the User Experience
  • C Roth
  • P Vorderer
  • C Klimmt
Roth, C., Vorderer, P. Klimmt, C.: The Motivational Appeal of Interactive Storytelling: Towards a Dimensional Model of the User Experience. In: Iurgel, I., Zagalo, N., Petta, P. (eds.) International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling, ICIDS, Springer, Guimarães (2009)