ArticlePDF Available

The Benefits of Facebook “Friends:” Social Capital and College Students’ Use of Online Social Network Sites

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study examines the relationship between use of Facebook, a popular online social network site, and the formation and maintenance of social capital. In addition to assessing bonding and bridging social capital, we explore a dimension of social capital that assesses one's ability to stay connected with members of a previously inhabited community, which we call maintained social capital. Regression analyses conducted on results from a survey of undergraduate students (N=286) suggest a strong association between use of Facebook and the three types of social capital, with the strongest relationship being to bridging social capital. In addition, Facebook usage was found to interact with measures of psychological well-being, suggesting that it might provide greater benefits for users experiencing low self-esteem and low life satisfaction.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Benefits of Facebook ‘Friends:’ Social
Capital and College Students’ Use of
Online Social Network Sites
Nicole B. Ellison
Charles Steinfield
Cliff Lampe
Department of Telecommunication, Information Studies, and Media
Michigan State University
This study examines the relationship between use of Facebook, a popular online social
network site, and the formation and maintenance of social capital. In addition to
assessing bonding and bridging social capital, we explore a dimension of social capital
that assesses one’s ability to stay connected with members of a previously inhabited
community, which we call maintained social capital. Regression analyses conducted on
results from a survey of undergraduate students (N = 286) suggest a strong association
between use of Facebook and the three types of social capital, with the strongest rela-
tionship being to bridging social capital. In addition, Facebook usage was found to
interact with measures of psychological well-being, suggesting that it might provide
greater benefits for users experiencing low self-esteem and low life satisfaction.
doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Introduction
Social network sites (SNSs) such as such as Friendster, CyWorld, and MySpace allow
individuals to present themselves, articulate their social networks, and establish or
maintain connections with others. These sites can be oriented towards work-related
contexts (e.g., LinkedIn.com), romantic relationship initiation (the original goal of
Friendster.com), connecting those with shared interests such as music or politics
(e.g., MySpace.com), or the college student population (the original incarnation of
Facebook.com). Participants may use the sites to interact with people they already
know offline or to meet new people. The online social network application analyzed
in this article, Facebook, enables its users to present themselves in an online profile,
accumulate ‘‘friends’ who can post comments on each other’s pages, and view each
other’s profiles. Facebook members can also join virtual groups based on common
interests, see what classes they have in common, and learn each others’ hobbies,
interests, musical tastes, and romantic relationship status through the profiles.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1143
Facebook constitutes a rich site for researchers interested in the affordances of
social networks due to its heavy usage patterns and technological capacities that bridge
online and offline connections. We believe that Facebook represents an understudied
offline to online trend in that it originally primarily served a geographically-bound
community (the campus). When data were collected for this study, membership was
restricted to people with a specific host institution email address, further tying offline
networks to online membership. In this sense, the original incarnation of Facebook
was similar to the wired Toronto neighborhood studied by Hampton and Wellman
(e.g., Hampton, 2002; Hampton & Wellman, 2003), who suggest that information
technology may enhance place-based community and facilitate the generation of social
capital.
1
Previous research suggests that Facebook users engage in ‘‘searching’ for
people with whom they have an offline connection more than they ‘‘browse’ for
complete strangers to meet (Lampe, Ellison, & Steinfield, 2006).
Online SNSs support both the maintenance of existing social ties and the for-
mation of new connections. Much of the early research on online communities
assumed that individuals using these systems would be connecting with others out-
side their pre-existing social group or location, liberating them to form communities
around shared interests, as opposed to shared geography (Wellman, Salaff, Dimitrova,
Garton, Gulia, & Haythornthwaite, 1996). A hallmark of this early research is the
presumption that when online and offline social networks overlapped, the direction-
ality was online to offline—online connections resulted in face-to-face meetings. For
instance, Parks and Floyd (1996) report that one-third of their respondents later met
their online correspondents face-to-face. As they write, ‘‘These findings imply that
relationships that begin on line rarely stay there’’ (n.p.).
Although this early work acknowledged the ways in which offline and online
networks bled into one another, the assumed online to offline directionality may not
apply to today’s SNSs that are structured both to articulate existing connections and
enable the creation of new ones. However, because there is little empirical research
that addresses whether members use SNSs to maintain existing ties or to form new
ones, the social capital implications of these services are unknown.
An Overview of Facebook
Created in 2004, by 2007 Facebook was reported to have more than 21 million
registered members generating 1.6 billion page views each day (Needham & Company,
2007). The site is tightly integrated into the daily media practices of its users: The
typical user spends about 20 minutes a day on the site, and two-thirds of users log in
at least once a day (Cassidy, 2006; Needham & Company, 2007). Capitalizing on its
success among college students, Facebook launched a high school version in early
September 2005. In 2006, the company introduced communities for commercial
organizations; as of November 2006, almost 22,000 organizations had Facebook
directories (Smith, 2006). In 2006, Facebook was used at over 2,000 United States
colleges and was the seventh most popular site on the World Wide Web with respect
to total page views (Cassidy, 2006).
1144Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
Much of the existing academic research on Facebook has focused on identity
presentation and privacy concerns (e.g., Gross & Acquisti, 2005; Stutzman, 2006).
Looking at the amount of information Facebook participants provide about them-
selves, the relatively open nature of the information, and the lack of privacy controls
enacted by the users, Gross and Acquisti (2005) argue that users may be putting
themselves at risk both offline (e.g., stalking) and online (e.g., identify theft). Other
recent Facebook research examines student perceptions of instructor presence and self-
disclosure (Hewitt & Forte, 2006; Mazer, Murphy, & Simonds, 2007), temporal pat-
terns of use (Golder, Wilkinson, & Huberman, 2007), and the relationship between
profile structure and friendship articulation (Lampe, Ellison, & Steinfield, 2007).
In contrast to popular press coverage which has primarily focused on negative
outcomes of Facebook use stemming from users’ misconceptions about the nature of
their online audience, we are interested in situations in which the intended audience
for the profile (such as well-meaning peers and friends) and the actual audience are
aligned. We use Facebook as a research context in order to determine whether offline
social capital can be generated by online tools. The results of our study show that
Facebook use among college-age respondents was significantly associated with meas-
ures of social capital.
Literature Review
Social Capital: Online and Offline
Social capital broadly refers to the resources accumulated through the relationships
among people (Coleman, 1988). Social capital is an elastic term with a variety of
definitions in multiple fields (Adler & Kwon, 2002), conceived of as both a cause and
an effect (Resnick, 2001; Williams, 2006). Bourdieu and Wacquant (1992) define
social capital as ‘‘the sum of the resources, actual or virtual, that accrue to an
individual or a group by virtue of possessing a durable network of more or less
institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance and recognition’’ (p. 14). The
resources from these relationships can differ in form and function based on the
relationships themselves.
Social capital has been linked to a variety of positive social outcomes, such as
better public health, lower crime rates, and more efficient financial markets (Adler &
Kwon, 2002). According to several measures of social capital, this important resource
has been declining in the U.S. for the past several years (Putnam, 2000). When social
capital declines, a community experiences increased social disorder, reduced partici-
pation in civic activities, and potentially more distrust among community members.
Greater social capital increases commitment to a community and the ability to
mobilize collective actions, among other benefits. Social capital may also be used
for negative purposes, but in general social capital is seen as a positive effect of
interaction among participants in a social network (Helliwell & Putnam, 2004).
For individuals, social capital allows a person to draw on resources from other
members of the networks to which he or she belongs. These resources can take the
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1145
form of useful information, personal relationships, or the capacity to organize
groups (Paxton, 1999). Access to individuals outside one’s close circle provides
access to non-redundant information, resulting in benefits such as employment
connections (Granovetter, 1973). Moreover, social capital researchers have found
that various forms of social capital, including ties with friends and neighbors, are
related to indices of psychological well-being, such as self esteem and satisfaction
with life (Bargh & McKenna, 2004; Helliwell & Putnam, 2004).
Putnam (2000) distinguishes between bridging and bonding social capital. The
former is linked to what network researchers refer to as ‘weak ties,’ which are loose
connections between individuals who may provide useful information or new per-
spectives for one another but typically not emotional support (Granovetter, 1982).
Alternatively, bonding social capital is found between individuals in tightly-knit, emo-
tionally close relationships, such as family and close friends. After briefly describing the
extant literature on these two forms of social capital and the Internet, we introduce an
additional dimension of social capital that speaks to the ability to maintain valuable
connections as one progresses through life changes. This concept, ‘maintained social
capital,’’ permits us to explore whether online network tools enable individuals to keep
in touch with a social network after physically disconnecting from it.
Social Capital and the Internet
The Internet has been linked both to increases and decreases in social capital. Nie
(2001), for example, argued that Internet use detracts from face-to-face time with
others, which might diminish an individual’s social capital. However, this perspective
has received strong criticism (Bargh & McKenna, 2004). Moreover, some researchers
have claimed that online interactions may supplement or replace in-person interac-
tions, mitigating any loss from time spent online (Wellman, Haase, Witte, & Hampton,
2001). Indeed, studies of physical (e.g., geographical) communities supported by
online networks, such as the Netville community in Toronto or the Blacksburg
Electronic Village, have concluded that computer-mediated interactions have had
positive effects on community interaction, involvement, and social capital (Hampton &
Wellman, 2003; Kavanaugh, Carroll, Rosson, Zin, & Reese, 2005).
Recently, researchers have emphasized the importance of Internet-based linkages
for the formation of weak ties, which serve as the foundation of bridging social
capital. Because online relationships may be supported by technologies like distri-
bution lists, photo directories, and search capabilities (Resnick, 2001), it is possible
that new forms of social capital and relationship building will occur in online social
network sites. Bridging social capital might be augmented by such sites, which
support loose social ties, allowing users to create and maintain larger, diffuse net-
works of relationships from which they could potentially draw resources (Donath &
boyd, 2004; Resnick, 2001; Wellman et al., 2001). Donath and boyd (2004) hypoth-
esize that SNSs could greatly increase the weak ties one could form and maintain,
because the technology is well-suited to maintaining such ties cheaply and easily.
Based on this prior work, we propose the following hypothesis:
1146Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
H1: Intensity of Facebook use will be positively associated with individuals’ perceived bridging
social capital.
In Putnam’s (2000) view, bonding social capital reflects strong ties with family
and close friends, who might be in a position to provide emotional support or access
to scarce resources. Williams (2006) points out that little empirical work has explic-
itly examined the effects of the Internet on bonding social capital, although some
studies have questioned whether the Internet supplements or supplants strong ties
(see Bargh & McKenna, 2004, for a review). It is clear that the Internet facilitates new
connections, in that it provides people with an alternative way to connect with others
who share their interests or relational goals (Ellison, Heino, & Gibbs, 2006; Horrigan,
2002; Parks & Floyd, 1996). These new connections may result in an increase in social
capital; for instance, a 2006 Pew Internet survey reports that online users are more
likely to have a larger network of close ties than non-Internet users, and that Internet
users are more likely than non-users to receive help from core network members
(Boase, Horrigan, Wellman, & Rainie, 2006). However, it is unclear how social
capital formation occurs when online and offline connections are closely coupled,
as with Facebook. Williams (2006) argues that although researchers have examined
potential losses of social capital in offline communities due to increased Internet use,
they have not adequately explored online gains that might compensate for this. We
thus propose a second hypothesis on the relationship between Facebook use and
close ties:
H2: Intensity of Facebook use will be positively associated with individuals’ perceived bonding
social capital.
Online social network tools may be of particular utility for individuals who
otherwise have difficulties forming and maintaining both strong and weak ties. Some
research has shown, for example, that the Internet might help individuals with low
psychological well-being due to few ties to friends and neighbors (Bargh & McKenna,
2004). Some forms of computer-mediated communication can lower barriers to
interaction and encourage more self-disclosure (Bargh, McKenna, & Fitzsimons,
2002; Tidwell & Walther, 2002); hence, these tools may enable connections and
interactions that would not otherwise occur. For this reason, we explore whether
the relationship between Facebook use and social capital is different for individuals
with varying degrees of self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1989) and satisfaction with
life (Diener, Suh, & Oishi, 1997; Pavot & Diener, 1993), two well-known and vali-
dated measures of subjective well-being. This leads to the two following pairs of
hypotheses:
H3a: The relationship between intensity of Facebook use and bridging social capital will vary
depending on the degree of a person’s self esteem.
H3b: The relationship between intensity of Facebook use and bridging social capital will vary
depending on the degree of a person’s satisfaction with life.
H4a: The relationship between intensity of Facebook use and bonding social capital will vary
depending on the degree of a person’s self esteem.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1147
H4b: The relationship between intensity of Facebook use and bonding social capital will
vary depending on the degree of a person’s satisfaction with life.
Maintained Social Capital and Life Changes
Social networks change over time as relationships are formed or abandoned. Partic-
ularly significant changes in social networks may affect one’s social capital, as when
a person moves from the geographic location in which their network was formed and
thus loses access to those social resources. Putnam (2000) argues that one of the
possible causes of decreased social capital in the U.S. is the increase in families
moving for job reasons; other research has explored the role of the Internet in these
transitions (Cummings, Lee, & Kraut, 2006; Wellman et al., 2001). Wellman et al.
(2001), for example, find that heavy Internet users rely on email to maintain long
distance relationships, rather than using it as a substitute for offline interactions with
those living nearby.
Some researchers have coined the term ‘‘friendsickness’’ to refer to the distress
caused by the loss of connection to old friends when a young person moves away to
college (Paul & Brier, 2001). Internet technologies feature prominently in a study of
communication technology use by this population by Cummings, Lee, and Kraut
(2006), who found that services like email and instant messaging help college stu-
dents remain close to their high school friends after they leave home for college. We
therefore introduce a measure focusing specifically on the maintenance of existing
social capital after this major life change experienced by college students, focusing on
their ability to leverage and maintain social connections from high school.
Young adults moving to college need to create new networks at college. However,
they often leave friends from high school with whom they may have established rich
networks; completely abandoning these high school networks would mean a loss of
social capital. Granovetter (1973, 1982) has suggested that weak ties provide more
benefit when the weak tie is not associated with stronger ties, as may be the case for
maintained high school relationships. To test the role of maintained high school
relationships as weak, bridging ties, we adapted questions about general bridging
relationships, such as those in Williams (2006), to be specific to maintained relation-
ships with high school acquaintances as opposed to close friends. We call this con-
cept ‘‘maintained social capital.’ In keeping with the thrust of our prior hypotheses
about the role of Facebook and bridging social capital, we propose the following:
H5: Intensity of Facebook use will be positively associated with individuals’ perceived
maintained social capital.
Method
A random sample of 800 Michigan State University (MSU) undergraduate students
was retrieved from the MSU registrar’s office. All 800 students were sent an email
invitation from one of the authors, with a short description of the study, information
1148Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
about confidentiality and incentives, and a link to the survey. Two reminder emails
were sent to those who had not responded. Participants were compensated with a $5
credit to their on-campus spending accounts. The survey was hosted on Zoomerang
(http://www.zoomerang.com), an online survey hosting site, and was fielded in April
2006. Only undergraduate users were included in our sampling frame. A total of 286
students completed the online survey, yielding a response rate of 35.8% (see Table 1
for sample demographics). Demographic information about non-responders was
not available; therefore we do not know whether a bias existed in regards to survey
participation. However, when we compare the demographics of our sample to infor-
mation we have about the MSU undergraduate population as a whole, our sample
appears to be representative with a few exceptions. Female, younger, in-state, and
on-campus students were slightly overrepresented in our sample.
2
Measures
Our instrument included four broad types of measures, which are discussed in more
detail below. We collected information about demographic and other descriptive
variables, including gender, age, year in school, local vs. home residence, ethnicity,
Table 1 Sample demographics (N = 286)
Mean or % (N) S.D.
Gender:
male 34% (98)
female 66% (188)
Age 20.1 1.64
Ethnicity:
white 87% (247)
non-white 13% (36)
Income
1
3.18 2.04
Year in school
2
2.55 1.07
Home residence:
In-state 91% (259)
out-of-state 09% (25)
Local residence:
on campus 55% (157)
off campus 45% (127)
Member of fraternity or sorority 08% (23) 1.01
Hours of Internet use per day
2
2 hours 56 min. 1:52
Facebook members 94% (268)
Notes:
1
represents household income; 1 = under $20,000, 2 = $20,000–$34,999, 3 = $35,000–
$49,999, 4 = $50,000–$74,999, 5 = $75,000 or more;
2
1 = first year, 2 = sophomore, 3 = junior,
4 = senior;
3
converted from ordinal scale using mid-point of response category (e.g., 1–2
hours = 1 hour 30 minutes).
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1149
a measure of Internet use adapted from LaRose, Lai, Lange, Love, and Wu (2005),
and whether respondents were Facebook members or not. (These items are reflected
in Table 1 above.) We also included Facebook usage measures, such as time spent
using Facebook and items designed to assess whether Facebook was used to meet new
people or to establish an online connection to pre-existing connections. Our instru-
ment also included measures of subjective well-being and as well as three social
capital measures, which served as our dependent variables.
Measures of Facebook Usage
Facebook Intensity
The Facebook intensity scale (Cronbach’s alpha = .83) was created in order to obtain
a better measure of Facebook usage than frequency or duration indices. This measure
includes two self-reported assessments of Facebook behavior, designed to measure
the extent to which the participant was actively engaged in Facebook activities: the
number of Facebook ‘‘friends’’ and the amount of time spent on Facebook on
a typical day. This measure also includes a series of Likert-scale attitudinal questions
designed to tap the extent to which the participant was emotionally connected to
Facebook and the extent to which Facebook was integrated into her daily activities
(see Table 2 for item wording and descriptive statistics).
Table 2 Summary statistics for Facebook intensity
Individual Items and Scale Mean S.D.
Facebook Intensity
1
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83) 20.08 0.79
About how many total Facebook friends do you have at
MSU or elsewhere? 0 = 10 or less, 1 = 11–50, 2 = 51–100,
3 = 101–150, 4 = 151–200, 5 = 201–250, 6 = 251–300,
7 = 301–400, 8 = more than 400
4.39 2.12
In the past week, on average, approximately how many
minutes per day have you spent on Facebook?
0 = less than 10, 1 = 10–30, 2 = 31–60, 3 = 1–2 hours,
4 = 2–3 hours, 5 = more than 3 hours
1.07 1.16
Facebook is part of my everyday activity 3.12 1.26
I am proud to tell people I’m on Facebook 3.24 0.89
Facebook has become part of my daily routine 2.96 1.32
I feel out of touch when I haven’t logged onto
Facebook for a while
2.29 1.20
I feel I am part of the Facebook community 3.30 1.01
I would be sorry if Facebook shut down 3.45 1.14
Notes:
1
Individual items were first standardized before taking an average to create scale due to
differing item scale ranges.
2
Unless provided, response categories ranged from 1 = strongly
disagree to 5 = strongly agree.
1150Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
Facebook Usage: Elements in Profile and Perceptions of Who Has Viewed Profiles
We asked respondents to indicate which of several salient aspects of the profile (such
as relationship status, high school, and mobile phone number) they included when
constructing their profile. The instrument asked respondents to indicate who they
thought had viewed their profile, such as high school friends, classmates, or family
members. These items offer insight into the degree to which respondents used Face-
book to maintain existing connections or meet new people.
Use of Facebook to Meet New People vs. Connect with Existing Offline Contacts
In order to further investigate whether usage was more motivated by prior offline
contacts or the potential to form new online contacts, we developed several items
reflecting each of these paths (see Table 3). In the former case, the items measured
whether respondents used Facebook to look up someone with whom they shared
some offline connection, such as a classmate or a friend (Cronbach’s alpha = .70). In
the latter case, our instrument included several items that tapped the use of Facebook
to make new friends without any reference to an offline connection, but these did not
correlate highly, and our final analysis incorporated only a single item measure: using
Facebook to meet new people.
Measures for Psychological Well-Being
Self-Esteem
Self-esteem was measured using seven items from the Rosenberg self-esteem scale
(Rosenberg, 1989). The answers to these questions were reported on a 5-point Likert
scale and exhibited high reliability (see Table 4).
Satisfaction with Life at MSU
The scale of satisfaction with life at MSU was adapted from the Satisfaction with Life
Scale (SWLS) (Diener, Suh, & Oishi, 1997; Pavot & Diener, 1993), a five-item
instrument designed to measure global cognitive judgments of one’s life. We
Table 3 Summary statistics for Facebook use for prior contacts and meeting new people
Individual Items and Scales
1
Mean S.D.
Off to Online: Use Facebook to connect with offline contacts
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70)
3.64 0.79
I have used Facebook to check out someone I met socially 3.99 1.05
I use Facebook to learn more about other people in my classes 3.26 1.20
I use Facebook to learn more about other people living near me 2.86 1.22
I use Facebook to keep in touch with my old friends 4.42 0.86
On to Offline: I use Facebook to meet new people
(single item measure)
1.97 1.03
Note:
1
Individual items ranged from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, scales
constructed by taking mean of items.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1151
amended each item slightly to refer specifically to the MSU context, on the assump-
tion that restricting participants was more appropriate given our hypotheses and
more likely to elicit accurate answers. The reliability test for this 5-point Likert scale
showed a relatively high reliability (see Table 4).
Measures of Social Capital
Our three measures of social capital—bridging, bonding, and maintained social
capital—were created by adapting existing scales, with wording changed to reflect
the context of the study, and creating new items designed to capture Internet-specific
social capital (Quan-Haase and Wellman, 2004). The full set of social capital items
was factor analyzed to ensure that the items reflected three distinct dimensions (see
Table 5).
Bridging Social Capital
This measure assessed the extent to which participants experienced bridging social
capital, which is believed to be better-suited for linking to external assets and for
information diffusion (Putnam, 2000). According to Williams (2006), ‘‘members of
weak-tie networks are thought to be outward looking and to include people from
a broad range of backgrounds. The social capital created by these networks generates
broader identities and generalized reciprocity’’ (n.p.). We therefore adapted five
items from Williams’ (2006) bridging social capital subscale and created three addi-
tional items intended to measure bridging social capital in the MSU context to create
our bridging social capital scale (Cronbach’s alpha = .87). One item, ‘‘MSU is a good
Table 4 Summary statistics and factor analysis results for self-esteem and satisfaction with
MSU life items
Individual Items and Scales
1
Mean S.D.
Self Esteem Scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87) 4.30 0.55
I feel that I’m a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others 4.50 0.60
I feel that I have a number of good qualities 4.54 0.57
All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure (reversed) 4.27 0.86
I am able to do things as well as most other people 4.29 0.63
I feel I do not have much to be proud of (reversed) 4.26 0.89
I take a positive attitude toward myself 4.17 0.75
On the whole, I am satisfied with myself 4.07 0.84
Satisfaction with MSU Life Scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87)
2
3.55 0.74
In most ways my life at MSU is close to my ideal. 3.42 0.96
The conditions of my life at MSU are excellent. 3.54 0.91
I am satisfied with my life at MSU. 3.85 0.84
So far I have gotten the important things I want at MSU. 3.74 0.81
If I could live my time at MSU over, I would change almost nothing. 3.18 1.05
Notes:
1
Individual items ranged from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, scales
constructed by taking mean of items.
2
1152Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
place to be,’’ was included because it loaded on the same factor and tapped into an
outcome of bridging social capital.
Bonding Social Capital
Bonding was assessed using five items from the bonding subscale of the Internet
social capital scales developed and validated by Williams (2006). Responses were
reported on a five-point Likert scale. These items were adapted to the MSU context
(Cronbach’s alpha = .75.)
Maintained Social Capital
This original scale was inspired by our pilot interviews,
3
media coverage of Facebook,
and anecdotal evidence that suggested that keeping in touch with high school friends
was a primary use of Facebook. These items were adapted from traditional measures
of social capital which assess an individual’s ability to mobilize support or action
(Cronbach’s alpha = .81) but focus on the ability to get assistance from a previously
inhabited community.
Findings
We first present some basic descriptive data to characterize Facebook users and
uses and provide insight into whether Facebook is used more to meet new people
or to maintain or strengthen relationships with offline connections. In a short
period of time, Facebook has garnered a very strong percentage of users on college
campuses. In our sample, 94% of the undergraduate students we surveyed were
Facebook members. We investigated whether members and non-members dif-
fered significantly along various demographic characteristics, but we lacked
confidence in these findings given the extremely low number of non-Facebook
users. The remainder of our analyses are based only on data from Facebook
members.
Facebook members report spending between 10 and 30 minutes on average using
Facebook each day and report having between 150 and 200 friends listed on their
profile (Table 2). From Table 3 we see that respondents also report significantly
more Facebook use involving people with whom they share an offline connection—
either an existing friend, a classmate, someone living near them, or someone they
met socially (mean = 3.64)—than use involving meeting new people (mean = 1.97)
(t = 26.14, p ,.0001).
Further insight into Facebook usage patterns can be gleaned from Figures 1 and
2, which show what elements respondents report including in their Facebook profile
and who they believe has seen their profiles, respectively. The fact that nearly all
Facebook users include their high school name in their profile (96%) suggests that
maintaining connections to former high school classmates is a strong motivation for
using Facebook. Not surprisingly, 97% report that high school friends had seen their
profile. Ninety percent or more also reported that other friends as well as people in
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1153
Table 5 Summary statistics and factor analysis results for social capital items
Individual Items and Scales
2
Mean S.D. Factor Loadings
1
Bridging
Social
Capital
Maintained
Social
Capital
Bonding
Social
Capital
Bridging Social Capital Scale
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87)
3.81 0.53
I feel I am part of the MSU community 3.78 0.80 0.70 20.24 0.13
I am interested in what goes on at
Michigan State University
3.98 0.64 0.73 20.10 0.13
MSU is a good place to be 4.22 0.78 0.73 20.12 0.18
I would be willing to contribute
money to Michigan State
University after graduation
3.35 0.95 0.66 20.04 0.13
Interacting with people at MSU
makes me want to try new things
3.74 0.68 0.60 20.04 0.15
Interacting with people at MSU
makes me feel like a part
of a larger community
3.81 0.68 0.72 20.09 0.23
I am willing to spend time to
support general MSU activities
3.70 0.77 0.76 20.10 0.16
At MSU, I come into contact with
new people all the time
4.05 0.69 0.54 20.17 0.13
Interacting with people at MSU
reminds me that everyone in the
world is connected
3.65 0.88 0.60 20.07 0.04
Bonding Social Capital Scale
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75)
3.72 0.66
There are several people at MSU
I trust to solve my problems
3.22 1.01 0.17 20.07 0.60
If I needed an emergency loan
of $100, I know someone at
MSU I can turn to
3.75 1.09 0.02 20.18 0.76
There is someone at MSU I can turn
to for advice about making very
important decisions
3.98 0.85 0.27 20.09 0.76
The people I interact with at MSU
would be good job references for me
3.88 0.79 0.32 0.07 0.63
I do not know people at MSU well
enough to get them to do anything
important (reversed)
3.78 0.87 0.13 20.23 0.61
(continued)
1154Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
their classes had seen their profile, further suggesting an offline component to Face-
book use.
4
As Figure 2 suggests, students view the primary audience for their profile to be
people with whom they share an offline connection. This is suggested as well by the
responses to items about how they use Facebook. Mean scores for the offline-to-
online scale were significantly higher than those for the single-item online-to-offline
measure (p ,.0001). This suggests that students use Facebook primarily to maintain
existing offline relationships or to solidify what would otherwise be ephemeral,
temporary acquaintanceships. There was a slight tendency for newer students to
use Facebook to meet new people more than for juniors and seniors to do so (see
Figure 3), but across all four years in school, respondents reported greater use of
Facebook for connecting with existing offline contacts.
In order to explore our research hypotheses regarding the relationship between
Facebook use and the various forms of social capital, we conducted regression
analyses. In each regression, we controlled for demographic, subjective well-being
and Internet use factors, in order to see if usage of Facebook accounted for variance
in social capital over and above these other independent variables.
Table 5 Continued
Individual Items and Scales
2
Mean S.D. Factor Loadings
1
Bridging
Social
Capital
Maintained
Social
Capital
Bonding
Social
Capital
Maintained Social Capital Scale
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81)
3.77 0.67
I’d be able to find out about events
in another town from a high school
acquaintance living there
3.59 0.88 0.20 20.58 0.05
If I needed to, I could ask a high school
acquaintance to do a small favor
for me
3.92 0.89 0.06 20.86 0.18
I’d be able to stay with a high school
acquaintance if traveling to a
different city
3.85 0.94 20.02 20.85 0.15
I would be able to find information
about a job or internship from a
high school acquaintance
3.58 0.89 0.11 20.79 0.02
It would be easy to find people to
invite to my high school reunion
3.90 0.88 0.29 20.56 0.14
Notes:
1
Principal components factor analysis with varimax rotation, explaining 53% of the
variance.
2
Individual items ranged from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, scales
constructed by taking mean of items.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1155
In order to test Hypothesis 1, we first investigated the extent to which demo-
graphic factors, psychological well-being measures, and general Internet use pre-
dicted the amount of bridging social capital reported by students; the adjusted R
2
for this model was .38. We then entered the Facebook intensity variable, which
Figure 1 Self-reported elements in respondents’ Facebook profiles
Figure 2 Perceived audience for respondents’ Facebook profiles
1156Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
raised the adjusted R
2
to .43. An additional pair of analyses further explored
whether Facebook intensity interacted with the self-esteem and satisfaction with
MSU life scales (see Table 6). The key finding is that, after first controlling for
demographic factors, psychological well-being measures, and general Internet use,
the extent to which students used Facebook intensively still contributed signifi-
cantly (scaled beta
5
= .34, p ,.0001), supporting Hypothesis 1. Interestingly,
general Internet use was not a significant predictor of bridging social capital,
suggesting that only certain kinds of uses of the Internet support the generation
and maintenance of bridging social capital. The significance of these variables did
not change when the interaction terms were added. We also explored whether
gender and year in school interacted with Facebook intensity, in order to see if
gender or time at MSU accounted for variation in the association between bridging
social capital and Facebook use. These interactions were not significant and are not
included in the table.
Overall, our independent factors accounted for nearly half of the variance in
bridging social capital. The results suggest that Facebook is indeed implicated in
students’ efforts to develop and maintain bridging social capital at college, although
we cannot assess causal direction. Few demographic factors matter, although white
students are somewhat more likely to have bridging social capital than non-white
students (scaled beta = .08, p ,.05). Among the psychological measures, the extent
of students’ satisfaction with life at MSU was strongly correlated with bridging social
capital (scaled beta = .66, p ,.0001).
Figure 3 Offline-to-online vs. online-to-offline mean scores by year in school
Notes: Off to Online average = 3.64 vs. On to Offline = 1.97, t = 26.14, p ,.0001
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1157
To explore Hypotheses 3a and 3b, the interaction between Facebook use and the
psychological measures was examined (Figures 4 and 5). Both hypotheses, which
predicted that the relationship between Facebook use and bridging social capital
would vary based upon the degree of self-esteem and satisfaction with life, are
supported. Students reporting low satisfaction and low self-esteem appeared to gain
in bridging social capital if they used Facebook more intensely, suggesting that the
affordances of the SNS might be especially helpful for these students.
As shown in Table 7, bonding social capital was also significantly predicted by
the intensity with which students used Facebook (scaled beta = .37, p ,.001 in
model 2). Other factors that related to bonding social capital were ethnicity (being
white, scaled beta = .16, p ,.01, model 2), year in school (scaled beta = .22, p ,.01,
model 2), living on campus (scaled beta = .13, p ,.01, model 2), self-esteem (scaled
Table 6 Regressions predicting the amount of bridging social capital from demographic,
attitudinal, and Facebook variables
Independent Variables
1
Model 1: Control Factors,
Facebook Intensity, and
Facebook X
Self-Esteem Interaction
Model 2: Control Factors,
Facebook Intensity,
and Facebook X Satisfaction
with MSU Life Interaction
Scaled Beta p Scaled Beta p
Intercept 3.80 **** 3.85 ****
Gender: male 20.02 20.03
Gender: female 0.02 0.03
Ethnicity: white 0.08 * 0.07
Ethnicity: nonwhite 20.08 * 20.07
Income 0.04 0.05
Year in school 0.00 0.01
State residence: in-state 20.05 20.07
State residence: out-of-state 0.05 0.07
Local residence: on campus 20.04 20.03
Local residence: off campus 0.04 0.03
Fraternity/sorority member 20.01 20.03
Not member of fraternity/sorority 0.01 0.03
Hours of Internet use per day 20.03 20.01
Self-esteem 0.20 *** 0.22 ****
Satisfaction with life at MSU 0.66 **** 0.61 ****
Facebook (FB) intensity 0.34 **** 0.31 ****
Self-esteem by FB intensity
4
20.35 **
Satisfaction by FB intensity 20.51 ***
N = 269 F = 18.83, ****
Adj. R
2
= .44
F = 19.92, ****
Adj. R
2
= .46
Notes:
1
Nominal factors expanded to all levels.
2
Continuous factors centered by mean, scaled
by range/2.
3
* p ,.05, ** p ,.01, *** p ,.001, **** p ,.0001.
4
Only one interaction term
was entered at a time in each regression.
1158Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
beta = .23, p ,.01, model 2), and satisfaction with MSU life (scaled beta = .40, p ,
.001, model 2). General Internet use was not a significant predictor of bonding social
capital, and the interactions between Facebook use and the two psychological meas-
ures were not significant. As in the bridging social capital analysis, gender and year in
school did not interact significantly with Facebook use in predicting bonding social
capital. The adjusted R
2
for the control factors alone was .19; adding Facebook
Intensity raised this statistic to .22. Again, the same variables were significant when
the interactions were added. Overall, the included variables accounted for almost one
quarter of the variance in students’ reported bonding social capital.
Figure 4 Interaction of Facebook use intensity and satisfaction with MSU life on bridging
social capital
Figure 5 Interaction of Facebook intensity and self-esteem on bridging social capital
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1159
Finally, entering only our control factors accounted for 13% of the variance in
maintained social capital (Table 8). Adding Facebook intensity raised the R
2
to .17
and revealed the same strong connection to Facebook intensity (scaled beta = .36,
p,.001), even after controlling for the number of years at college (and thus, away
from high school) and general Internet use. Interestingly, general Internet use was
also a significant predictor of maintained social capital (scaled beta = .26, p ,.05),
suggesting that other Internet applications are useful in this case. Ethnicity (being
white, scaled beta = .23, p ,.001) and self-esteem (scaled beta = .30, p ,.001) were
the other significant variables in this regression. None of the interactions were
significant. Together, the independent variables accounted for 16% to 17% of the
variance in the maintained social capital measure.
Table 7 Regressions predicting the amount of bonding social capital from demographic,
attitudinal, and Facebook variables
Independent Variables
1
Model 1: Control Factors,
Facebook Intensity,
and Facebook
X Self-Esteem Interaction
Model 2: Control Factors,
Facebook Intensity,
and Facebook X Satisfaction
with MSU Life Interaction
Scaled Beta p Scaled Beta p
Intercept 3.73 **** 3.76 ****
Gender: male 0.07 0.06
Gender: female 20.07 20.06
Ethnicity: white 0.17 ** 0.16 **
Ethnicity: nonwhite 20.17 ** 20.16 **
Income 0.07 0.07
Year in school 0.23 *** 0.23 ***
State residence: in-state 20.09 20.10
State residence: out-of-state 0.09 0.10
Local residence: on campus 0.13 ** 0.14 **
Local residence: off campus 20.13 ** 20.14 **
Fraternity/sorority member 20.07 20.08
Not member of fraternity/sorority 0.07 0.08
Hours of Internet use per day 20.01 0.01
Self-esteem 0.22 ** 0.24 **
Satisfaction with life at MSU 0.40 *** 0.37 ***
Facebook (FB) intensity 0.37 **** 0.34 ***
Self-esteem by FB intensity
4
20.32
Satisfaction by FB intensity 20.26
N = 269 F = 7.60, ****
Adj. R
2
= .23
F = 7.48, ****
Adj. R
2
= .22
Notes:
1
Nominal factors expanded to all levels.
2
Continuous factors centered by mean, scaled
by range/2.
3
* p ,.05, ** p ,.01, *** p ,.001, **** p ,.0001.
4
Only one interaction term
was entered at a time in each regression.
1160Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
Discussion
Returning to our original research question, we can definitively state that there is
a positive relationship between certain kinds of Facebook use and the maintenance
and creation of social capital. Although we cannot say which precedes the other,
Facebook appears to play an important role in the process by which students form
and maintain social capital, with usage associated with all three kinds of social capital
included in our instrument.
Although representation of non-users is low in our sample, when we compare
members vs. nonmembers, we see no real difference in demographics, with the
exception of class year and age (which is strongly correlated with class year). This
Table 8 Regressions predicting the amount of maintained social capital from demographic,
attitudinal, and Facebook variables
Independent Variables
1
Model 1: Control Factors,
Facebook Intensity,
and Facebook
X Self-Esteem Interaction
Model 2: Control Factors,
Facebook Intensity,
and Facebook
X Satisfaction with
MSU Life Interaction
Scaled Beta p Scaled Beta p
Intercept 3.57 **** 3.60 ****
Gender: male 20.02 20.02
Gender: female 0.02 0.02
Ethnicity: white 0.23 *** 0.23 ***
Ethnicity: nonwhite 20.23 *** 20.23 ***
Income 0.08 0.08
Year in school 20.09 20.08
State residence: in-state 0.06 0.05
State residence: out-of-state 20.06 20.05
Local residence: on campus 20.06 20.05
Local residence: off campus 0.06 0.05
Fraternity/sorority member 20.02 20.03
Not member of fraternity/sorority 0.02 0.03
Hours of Internet use per day 0.26 * 0.27 *
Self-esteem 0.30 *** 0.31 ***
Satisfaction with life at MSU 20.02 20.04
Facebook (FB) intensity 0.37 *** 0.36 ***
Self-esteem by FB intensity
4
20.11
Satisfaction by FB intensity 20.29
N = 269 F = 5.40, ****
Adj. R
2
= .16
F = 5.57, ****
Adj. R
2
= .17
Notes:
1
Nominal factors expanded to all levels.
2
Continuous factors centered by mean, scaled
by range/2.
3
* p ,.05, ** p ,.01, *** p ,.001, **** p ,.0001.
4
Only one interaction term
was entered at a time in each regression.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1161
is most likely due to the fact that Facebook is a relatively recent phenomenon, and we
would expect senior students to be less likely to join. The high penetration and lack
of any systematic difference between members and non-members suggests that Face-
book has broad appeal, does not exclude particular social groups, and has not had
a noticeable effect on participants’ grades.
Our participants overwhelmingly used Facebook to keep in touch with old
friends and to maintain or intensify relationships characterized by some form of
offline connection such as dormitory proximity or a shared class. For many, Face-
book provided a way to keep in touch with high school friends and acquaintances.
This was demonstrated through the fact that the most commonly included infor-
mation on users’ profiles was likely to be relevant for existing acquaintances trying to
find them (e.g., their high school) and that nearly all users felt that their high school
friends had viewed their profile, and through respondents’ self-reported types of use
(connecting with offline contacts as opposed to meeting new people). This offline to
online movement differs from the patterns observed by early researchers examining
computer-mediated communication and virtual communities. Due to the structure
of the site, which blocks entry to those without a school email address and then
places individuals into communities based on that email address, Facebook serves
a geographically-bound user base.
6
Our first dimension of social capital—bridging—assessed the extent to which
participants were integrated into the MSU community, their willingness to support
the community, and the extent to which these experiences broadened their social
horizons or worldview. Our findings suggest that certain kinds of Facebook use
(articulated by our Facebook intensity items) can help students accumulate and
maintain bridging social capital. This form of social capital—which is closely linked
to the notion of ‘‘weak ties’’—seems well-suited to social software applications, as
suggested by Donath and boyd (2004), because it enables users to maintain such ties
cheaply and easily. Although more research is needed to understand the nature of
this trend, we suspect that Facebook serves to lower the barriers to participation so
that students who might otherwise shy away from initiating communication with or
responding to others are encouraged to do so through Facebook’s affordances.
Participants’ reports about who is viewing their profile provide insight into this
dynamic. As depicted in Figure 2, students report that the primary audiences for
their profiles are high school friends and people they know from an MSU context.
This implies that highly engaged users are using Facebook to crystallize relationships
that might otherwise remain ephemeral. Haythornthwaite (2005) discusses the
implications of media that ‘‘create latent tie connectivity among group members
that provides the technical means for activating weak ties’’ (p. 125). Latent ties are
those social network ties that are ‘‘technically possible but not activated socially’’
(p. 137). Facebook might make it easier to convert latent ties into weak ties, in that
the site provides personal information about others, makes visible one’s connections
to a wide range of individuals, and enables students to identify those who might be
useful in some capacity (such as the math major in a required calculus class), thus
1162Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
providing the motivation to activate a latent tie. These weak ties may provide addi-
tional information and opportunities, which are expressed as dimensions of bridging
social capital that speak to interaction with a wide range of people and the more
tolerant perspective this might encourage. Facebook seems well-suited to facilitate
these experiences, in that detailed profiles highlight both commonalities and differ-
ences among participants.
We also found an interaction between bridging social capital and subjective
well-being measures. For less intense Facebook users, students who reported low
satisfaction with MSU life also reported having much lower bridging social capital
than those who used Facebook more intensely. The same was true for self-esteem.
Conversely, there was little difference in bridging social capital among those who
reported high satisfaction with life at MSU and high self-esteem relative to Facebook
use intensity. One explanation consistent with these interaction effects is that Face-
book use may be helping to overcome barriers faced by students who have low
satisfaction and low self-esteem. Because bridging social capital provides benefits
such as increased information and opportunities, we suspect that participants who
use Facebook in this way are able to get more out of their college experience. The
suggestion that Facebook use supports a ‘‘poor get richer’’ hypothesis, as opposed to
the ‘‘rich get richer’’ findings reported in other contexts (Kraut, Kiesler, Boneva,
Cummings, Helgeson, & Crawford, 2002), may be of special interest to Internet
researchers.
Bonding social capital was also predicted by high self-esteem, satisfaction with
university life, and intense Facebook use, although overall, the regression model
predicting bonding social capital accounted for less of the variation for this depen-
dent variable than for bridging social capital. However, Facebook appears to be much
less useful for maintaining or creating bonding social capital, as indicated by the fact
that the bonding model only accounted for 22% of the variance (versus 46% in the
bridging social capital models). We might expect Facebook usage to have less of an
impact on bonding than bridging social capital given the affordances of this service.
It can lower barriers to participation and therefore may encourage the formation of
weak ties but not necessarily create the close kinds of relationships that are associated
with bonding social capital. Yet the strong coefficient for Facebook intensity suggests
that Facebook use is important for bonding social capital as well. One explanation is
that it may help individuals to maintain pre-existing close relationships, just as it can
be used as a low-maintenance way to keep tabs on distant acquaintances. For
instance, in our pilot interviews, students discussed the ‘‘birthday’’ feature of Face-
book, which prompted them to send birthday greetings to friends with minimal
effort.
Finally, Facebook intensity predicted increased levels of maintained social cap-
ital, which assessed the extent to which participants could rely on high school
acquaintances to do small favors. For college students, many of whom have
moved away for the first time, the ability to stay in touch with these high school
acquaintances may illustrate most clearly the ‘‘strength of weak ties’’ outlined by
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1163
Granovetter (1973, 1982). These potentially useful connections may be valuable
sources of new information and resources. Additionally, the ability to stay in touch
with these networks may offset feelings of ‘‘friendsickness,’’ the distress caused by the
loss of old friends.
Limitations to this study include the fact that we examined only one community.
Because the college years are a unique developmental period in the life cycle and
because the MSU Facebook community is closely coupled with the geographically
bounded MSU community, we are not able to generalize these findings to other
kinds of communities or social network tools. It may be that the positive outcomes
linked to Facebook use discussed here are limited to this special case in which the
offline community is bounded spatially and to the unique nature of the undergrad-
uate experience. Future research could explore Facebook use in other contexts, such
as organizations and high schools. Because we used a one-time survey, we cannot
establish causality. Additionally, the extremely low incidence of non-members, non-
White, or international students in our sample hampered our ability to assess the
effects of Facebook membership on these groups. Finally, respondents may have
misreported behavioral or demographic information, as we used self-reported rather
than direct measures of Facebook use and other variables.
To address these concerns, future research should approach Facebook use and
the generation of social capital via multiple methodologies. Profile capture and
analysis would allow researchers to marry survey responses with direct behavioral
measures. Additionally, experimental interventions would support causal claims;
these interventions could be in the form of a survey, with pre- and post-test data
collected from the site itself. Collecting longitudinal data over a series of years,
tracking incoming first-year students and following them after they graduate, is also
a necessary next step.
Conclusions
Our empirical results contrast with the anecdotal evidence dominating the popular
press. Although there are clearly some image management problems experienced by
students as reported in the press, and the potential does exist for privacy abuses, our
findings demonstrate a robust connection between Facebook usage and indicators of
social capital, especially of the bridging type. Internet use alone did not predict social
capital accumulation, but intensive use of Facebook did.
The strong linkage between Facebook use and high school connections suggests
how SNSs help maintain relations as people move from one offline community to
another. It may facilitate the same when students graduate from college, with alumni
keeping their school email address and using Facebook to stay in touch with the
college community. Such connections could have strong payoffs in terms of jobs,
internships, and other opportunities. Colleges may want to explore ways to encour-
age this sort of usage.
1164Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
Online social network sites may play a role different from that described in early
literature on virtual communities. Online interactions do not necessarily remove
people from their offline world but may indeed be used to support relationships
and keep people in contact, even when life changes move them away from each
other. In addition to helping student populations, this use of technology could
support a variety of populations, including professional researchers, neighborhood
and community members, employees of companies, or others who benefit from
maintained ties.
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank Dean Chuck Salmon and the College of Communication
Arts and Sciences at Michigan State University for their generous support of this
research.
Notes
1 ‘‘Netville’’ residents with broadband Internet connections and access to a local online
community discussion board were more likely to be involved with their neighbors than
were their non-wired peers: They recognized three times as many and talked to twice as
many (Hampton & Wellman, 2003).
2 Differences were as follows: 54% of the MSU student population is female vs. 66% of
our respondents; 58% of MSU students live off-campus vs. 45% of our respondents;
11% of MSU students are out of state vs. 9% of our respondents.
3 We interviewed one graduate and six undergraduate students about their Facebook use;
the data were used to inform survey construction and study design.
4 We asked Facebook users whether or not they had set the privacy settings on their
accounts to control who viewed their profiles. More than two thirds (70%) either did
not know (suggesting that they left the default setting of all members of the MSU
network) or said that their profile was visible by the entire MSU network. Only 13%
limited access only to their friends, while the rest blocked only certain individuals.
Figure 2 does not take respondents’ privacy settings into account.
5 A scaled beta is similar to a standardized regression coefficient in that the coefficients
are adjusted so that they correspond to factors that are scaled to have a mean of zero and
range of two. This makes it easier to compare effect sizes when factors have different
scales.
6 In May of 2006, Facebook began establishing company sites and allowed members to
choose their networks. Nonetheless, college Facebook communities remain defined by
those who have a school email account.
References
Adler, P., & Kwon, S. (2002). Social capital: Prospects for a new concept. Academy of
Management Review,27(1), 17–40.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1165
Bargh, J., & McKenna, K. (2004). The Internet and social life. Annual Review of Psychology,
55(1), 573–590.
Bargh, J. A., McKenna, K. Y., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2002). Can you see the real me? Activation
and expression of the ‘‘true self’’ on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues,58(1), 33–48.
Boase, J., Horrigan, J. B., Wellman, B., & Rainie, L. (2006). The strength of Internet ties. Pew
Internet and American Life Project. Retrieved May 20, 2006 from http://
www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Internet_ties.pdf
Bourdieu, P., & Wacquant, L. (1992). An Invitation to Reflexive Sociology. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Cassidy, J. (2006, May 15). Me media. The New Yorker, 50–59.
Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of
Sociology,94(Supplement), S95–S120.
Cummings, J., Lee, J., & Kraut, R. (2006). Communication technology and friendship during
the transition from high school to college. In R. E. Kraut, M. Brynin, & S. Kiesler (Eds.).
Computers, Phones, and the Internet: Domesticating Information Technology (pp. 265–278).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Diener, E., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (1997). Recent findings on subjective well-being. Indian Journal
of Clinical Psychology,24(1), 25–41.
Donath, J., & boyd, d. (2004). Public displays of connection. BT Technology Journal,22(4), 71.
Ellison, N., Heino, R., & Gibbs, J. (2006). Managing impressions online: Self-presentation
processes in the online dating environment. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication,11(2), article 2. Retrieved June 30, 2007 from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/
vol11/issue2/ellison.html
Golder, S. A., Wilkinson, D., & Huberman, B. A. (2007, June). Rhythms of social interaction:
Messaging within a massive online network. In C. Steinfield, B. Pentland, M. Ackerman, &
N. Contractor (Eds.), Communities and Technologies 2007: Proceedings of the Third
International Conference on Communities and Technologies (pp. 41–66). London: Springer.
Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology,78(6),
1360–1380.
Granovetter, M. S. (1982). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. In P. V.
Mardsen & N. Lin (Eds.), Social Structure and Network Analysis (pp. 105–130). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Gross, R., & Acquisti, A. (2005, November). Information revelation and privacy in online social
networks. Paper presented at the WPES’05, Alexandria, Virginia.
Hampton, K. (2002). Place-based and IT mediated ‘‘community.’’ Planning Theory and
Practice,3(2), 228–231.
Hampton, K., & Wellman, B. (2003). Neighboring in Netville: How the Internet supports
community and social capital in a wired suburb. City & Community,2(4), 277–311.
Haythornthwaite, C. (2005). Social networks and Internet connectivity effects. Information,
Communication, & Society,8(2), 125–147.
Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of well-being. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society,359(1449), 1435–1446.
Hewitt, A., & Forte, A. (2006, November). Crossing boundaries: Identity management and
student/faculty relationships on the Facebook. Paper presented at CSCW, Banff, Alberta,
Canada.
1166Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
Horrigan, J. B. (2002). Online communities: Networks that nurture long-distance
relationships and local ties. Pew Internet and American Life Project. Retrieved June 26,
2007 from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Communities_Report.pdf
Kavanaugh, A., Carroll, J. M., Rosson, M. B., Zin, T. T., & Reese, D. D. (2005). Community
networks: Where offline communities meet online. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication 10(4), article 3. Retrieved June 26, 2007 from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/
vol10/issue4/kavanaugh.html
Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J., Helgeson, V., & Crawford, A. (2002). Internet
paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues,58(1), 49–74.
Lampe, C., Ellison, N., & Steinfield, C. (2006). A Face(book) in the crowd: Social searching vs.
social browsing. Proceedings of the 2006 20th Anniversary Conference on Computer
Supported Cooperative Work (pp. 167–170). New York: ACM Press.
Lampe, C., Ellison, N., & Steinfield, C. (2007). A familiar Face(book): Profile elements as
signals in an online social network. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human
Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 435–444). New York: ACM Press.
LaRose, R., Lai, Y.-J., Lange, R., Love, B., & Wu, Y. (2005). Sharing or piracy? An exploration
of downloading behavior. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,11(1), article 1.
Retrieved May 14, 2006 from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol11/issue1/larose.html
May, M., & Kwong, K. H. (2007). YHOO: Yahoo! may regret not paying up for Facebook.
Retrieved May 10, 2007 from http://www.needhamco.com/Research/Documents/
CPY25924.pdf
Mazer, J. P., Murphy, R. E., & Simonds, C. J. (2007). I’ll see you on ‘‘Facebook:’’ The effects of
computer-mediated teacher self-disclosure on student motivation, affective learning, and
classroom climate. Communication Education,56(1), 1–17.
Nie, N. H. (2001). Sociability, interpersonal relations, and the Internet: Reconciling
conflicting findings. American Behavioral Scientist,45(3), 420–35.
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication,1(4). Retrieved July 14, 2006 from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol1/issue4/
parks.html
Paul, E., & Brier, S. (2001). Friendsickness in the transition to college: Precollege predictors
and college adjustment correlates. Journal of Counseling and Development,79(1), 77–89.
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological
Assessment,5(2), 164–172.
Paxton, P. (1999). Is social capital declining in the United States? A multiple indicator
assessment. American Journal of Sociology,105(1), 88–127.
Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling Alone. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Quan-Haase, A., & Wellman, B. (2004). How does the Internet affect social capital? In M.
Huysman & V. Wulf (Eds.), Social Capital and Information Technology (pp. 113–135).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Resnick, P. (2001). Beyond bowling together: Sociotechnical capital. In J. Carroll (Ed.), HCI
in the New Millennium (pp. 247–272). Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image (Rev. ed.). Middletown, CT:
Wesleyan University Press.
Smith, J. (2006). Updated lists of all companies and regions on Facebook. Retrieved May 9, 2007
from http://www.insidefacebook.com/2006/11/15
Southey, R. (2001). Poems of Robert Southey. London: Adamant Media Corporation.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association1167
Stutzman, F. (2006, April). An evaluation of identity-sharing behavior in social network
communities. Paper presented at the iDMAa and IMS Code Conference, Oxford, Ohio.
Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). Computer-mediated communication effects on
disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one another a bit
at a time. Human Communication Research,28(3), 317–348.
Wellman, B., Haase, A. Q., Witte, J., & Hampton, K. (2001). Does the Internet increase,
decrease, or supplement social capital? Social networks, participation, and community
commitment. American Behavioral Scientist,45(3), 436.
Wellman, B., Salaff, J., Dimitrova, D., Garton, L., Gulia, M., & Haythornthwaite, C. (1996).
Computer networks as social networks: Collaborative work, telework, and virtual
community. Annual Review of Sociology,22, 213–238.
Williams, D. (2006). On and off the ‘net: Scales for social capital in an online era. Journal of
Computer-Mediated Communication,11(2), article 11. Retrieved August 29, 2006 from
http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol11/issue2/williams.html
About the Authors
Nicole Ellison is an assistant professor in the Department of Telecommunication,
Information Studies, and Media at Michigan State University. Her research explores
issues of self-presentation, relationship development, and identity in online environ-
ments such as weblogs, online dating sites, and social network sites.
Address: 403 Communication Arts and Sciences, East Lansing, MI 48824 USA
Charles Steinfield is Professor and Chair of the Department of Telecommunication,
Information Studies, and Media at Michigan State University. His research interests
include the uses of online social networks, individual and organizational collabora-
tion via ICT, and e-commerce.
Address: 409 Communication Arts and Sciences Building, East Lansing, MI 48824,
USA
Cliff Lampe is an assistant professor in the Department of Telecommunication,
Information Studies, and Media at Michigan State University. His research interests
include the social practices and architecture of online communities, online rating
systems, social software, and user-generated content.
Address: 419 Communication Arts and Sciences Building, East Lansing, MI 48824,
USA
1168Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 12 (2007) 1143–1168 ª2007 International Communication Association
... In the past, consumers primarily shopped in physical stores (Hsiao, 2009), but now social media platforms have become key shopping destinations. Social media, including popular sites like Facebook and Instagram, allows users to connect and interact, with Facebook and Instagram being particularly dominant in global usage (Steinfield et al., 2007). ...
Article
The objective of study to examine the relationships between key factors—Informativeness, E-WOM, Ad Perceived Value, Entertainment, and Consumer Buying Decisions. It focuses on how these factors influence consumer choices when buying fashion products online. The research targeted individuals who have used social media platforms for online shopping in the Kathmandu Valley, with a sample size of 282 participants. Convenience sampling was used to select participants. The study used correlational and causal-comparative research designs, gathering primary data through a survey questionnaire. Hypothesis testing and regression analysis were applied to understand the relationships between the factors influencing consumer buying decisions. The regression model revealed a moderate positive correlation between the predictor variables and consumer buying decisions, showing that these factors together explained a significant portion of the variation in buying decisions. The results indicate that Perceived Value of Ads and Entertainment have a moderate positive effect on consumer buying decisions. Participants who found ads valuable and entertaining were more likely to make a purchase. E-WOM and Informativeness also showed statistically significant, but slightly weaker, correlations with buying decisions. These findings emphasize the importance of creating advertisements that are both valuable and entertaining, providing useful insights for marketing strategies aimed at influencing consumer behavior.
... With reference to Heath et al. (2009) concerns discussed earlier, we would not discount the opinion that such an approach is coercive at some subtle level but would argue that ethically we do not feel it disadvantaged participants but allowed the research to be of value in understanding young people's relationship with football and its mediated forms. What is more controversial is the research project's role in potentially fuelling young people's 'need for popularity' that Ellison et al. (2007) identify as a risk of online interaction. This, however, was seen by community football coaches working with the young participants in the research as an attractive selling point in encouraging young people to be involved: ...
Article
Full-text available
Building on the increasing academic interest in collaborative participatory research, this paper introduces Performative Participatory Research (PPR) as a way of meaningfully engaging (young) people in research and ensuring useful outcomes when timescales are short. Our case involved a project exploring young people's engagements with sport, particularly how consumption of football underpins different forms of solidarity, belonging and community. The paper focuses on three activities that were used to not only build rapport and trust but also generate novel perspectives on the subject: a pre-match press conference, a player profile film and the design of a social media campaign. We argue that PPR generates positive outcomes and valuable data when looking to address disempowered or difficult to engage groups in research. Whilst our focus was on the connection between football and celebrity in the lives of young people, the methods could be adapted to understand other cultural interests with other demographics. ARTICLE HISTORY
... Ideologies are particularly relevant in digital relational spheres where cultural identity has become more symbolic, disembodied, and grounded in shared interests, aesthetics, and outlooks on the world (Ito et al., 2009;Manago et al., 2015). Social media (SM) facilitate cultural groups unconstrained by physical propinquity, allowing people who are motivated by desires to connect with like-minded others to form potentially expansive relational networks consisting of "loose ties" through which they coconstruct meanings, language, and other digital artifacts that resonate with their lived experiences (Ellison et al., 2007;Ito et al., 2009). Hip-hop as a cultural identity around the globe is a prime illustration of this phenomenon (Morgan & Warren, 2011;Nguyen & Ferguson, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
The spread of digital communication around the globe has raised questions about the nature of digitally mediated cultural identity and how worldviews are constructed in the context of permeable and dynamic communities less tethered to physical geography. To expand research on the impacts of digital communication on cultural identity development among adolescents in the Majority World, the present study compared the worldviews of indigenous Maya adolescents before and after the Internet and mobile devices became widely used in their community. Adolescents were interviewed in 2009 (N = 80; 40 girls, Mage = 16.94) and in 2018 (N = 79; 44 girls, Mage = 15.91) using eight vignettes that were developed from ethnographic work in the community and designed to elicit participants’ cultural beliefs and values. In each story, one character articulates a traditional, collectivistic worldview, and another articulates a Western, individualistic worldview present in the community. Participants were asked who they agreed with and why, and responses were analyzed quantitatively (pattern of character endorsements) and qualitatively (frameworks of meaning). Analysis of covariance showed no differences in character endorsements across the two cohorts. Schooling, not the use of mobile devices or social media, uniquely predicted alignment with individualistic characters in regression analyses. Although individualistic values did not increase, qualitative analyses of frameworks of meaning showed that adolescents in the two cohorts differed in how they integrated individualistic and collectivistic perspectives. The study demonstrates the importance of locally relevant mixed methods for understanding changes in the contents of cultural identity over historical time.
... When we are waiting for our meal in a restaurant or waiting for a bus at a bus station, we often involuntarily use our cellphones to seek possibly interesting information on social media platforms or idly browse status updates of online friends. Today, social media is popular around the world, which not only effectively enriches people's leisure time, but also provides a novel approach to establishing and maintaining interpersonal relationships (Carr et al., 2016;Ellison et al., 2007;Godard and Holtzman, 2024). Prior literature suggests that social media has become a part of our lives and produced a widespread and profound effect on our physical health and psychological wellbeing (Krause et al., 2021;O'Day and Heimberg, 2021;Padín et al., 2021;Steers et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Prior literature suggests that fitness posts exposure on social media increases female body image concerns. However, little research has been conducted to examine the effect of fitness posts exposure on female body esteem. Objective Given that, two studies were conducted to investigate how fitness posts exposure on social media exerted an influence on female body esteem. Method and results By using a questionnaire survey ( n = 270), we in Study 1 measured participants’ frequency of fitness posts exposure on social media, body esteem, body surveillance, and appearance contingent self-worth. The results showed that fitness posts exposure was significantly and positively related to female body esteem, and body surveillance played a mediating role between them. And this mediation was further moderated by appearance contingent self-worth. Simple slope analysis showed that fitness posts exposure had a significantly positive prediction on body surveillance at the high level of appearance contingent self-worth, but the prediction of fitness posts exposure was not significant at the low level of appearance contingent self-worth. Study 2 was a lab experiment, in which we randomly assigned 180 female undergraduates to the fitness-appearance exposure condition, the fitness-performance exposure condition, and the travel image exposure condition. Then, we asked participants to report body surveillance and body esteem. The results showed that participants in the fitness-appearance exposure condition reported higher body surveillance and lower body esteem than participants in the other two conditions. Consistent with Study 1, Study 2 revealed the mediating role of body surveillance between exposure condition and body esteem. Conclusion Fitness posts exposure produces a negative influence on female body esteem via the mediation of body surveillance, and this pattern is more obvious among women with high appearance contingent self-worth. Appearance-relevant content involved in fitness posts, rather than pure fitness performance, harms female body esteem.
Article
Bedtime procrastination is prevalent, particularly among adolescents. This study employed self-regulation theory and social cognitive theory as its theoretical framework and used structural equation modeling to explore relationships among self-efficacy, fear of missing out (FoMO), social media use, and bedtime procrastination. 468 Chinese college students (54% male, 46% female) completed self-report questionnaires online. The results showed a significant negative correlation between self-efficacy and bedtime procrastination. This association was partially explained by increased FoMO and social media use. Fear of missing out was only related to bedtime procrastination among females. Our study highlights that FoMO and social media use may be important for understanding and mitigating bedtime procrastination, specifically in those with lower self-efficacy.
Article
We investigated the relationship between social media and loneliness in early adolescents in Perú across 15 months of COVID‐19 lockdowns (grades 6–8; 56% female in May 2020). Cross‐sectional analyses with data from May 2020 ( n = 1613) found that higher social media intensity was associated with higher feelings of loneliness. In a longitudinal follow‐up study ( n = 455), we investigated bidirectional associations in May 2020, November 2020 and July 2021, and found that feelings of loneliness in girls were associated with a subsequent increase in social media intensity. Social media intensity was not associated with changes in feelings of loneliness in either gender. Our findings with adolescents in low‐ and middle‐income urban settings in Perú underscore the importance of longitudinal research and contribute to understanding these issues globally.
Article
Full-text available
The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) was developed to assess satisfaction with the respondent’s life as a whole. The scale does not assess satisfaction with life domains such as health or finances but allows subjects to integrate and weight these domains in whatever way they choose. Normative data are presented for the scale, which shows good convergent validity with other scales and with other types of assessments of subjective well-being. Life satisfaction as assessed by the SWLS shows a degree of temporal stability (e.g., 54 for 4 years), yet the SWLS has shown sufficient sensitivity to be potentially valuable to detect change in life satisfaction during the course of clinical intervention. Further, the scale shows discriminant validity from emotional well-being measures. The SWLS is recommended as a complement to scales that focus on psychopathology or emotional well-being because it assesses an individuals’ conscious evaluative judgment of his or her life by using the person’s own criteria.
Chapter
A multidisciplinary examination of the interplay between social capital—the value derived from social ties—and information technology. The concept of social capital, or the value that can be derived from social ties created by goodwill, mutual support, shared language, common beliefs, and a sense of mutual obligation, has been applied to a number of fields, from sociology to management. It is only lately, however, that researchers in information technology and knowledge management have begun to explore the idea of social capital in relation to their fields. This collection of thirteen essays by computer scientists, sociologists, communication specialists, economists, and others presents a multidisciplinary look at this particular intersection of information technology and social science and the need to adopt a sociotechnical perspective.For the most part the contributors take a positive view of the interplay of social capital, knowledge sharing, and community building. Some essays look at specific instances, including the on-line and face-to-face relationships of a community of athletes, the building of social capital among Iranian NGOs, and the Internet-based communities created by the open-source movement, while others discuss more general ideas of civic and personal communities. The last four essays examine computer applications that augment social capital, including topic- and member-centered communications spaces such as the Expert Finder and the Loops system and virtual repositories of knowledge such as the Answer Garden and Pearls of Wisdom.
Article
In this chapter I review empirical studies directly testing the hypotheses of my 1973 paper "The Strength of Weak Ties" (hereafter "SWT") and work that elaborates those hypotheses theoretically or uses them to suggest new empirical research not discussed in my original formulation. Along the way, I will reconsider various aspects of the theoretical argument, attempt to plug some holes, and broaden its base.
Article
In this study friendsickness (preoccupation with and concern for the loss of or change in precollege friendships) is seen as a significant source of distress for college students, affecting college adjustment. A short‐term longitudinal study of college students (N = 70) examined associations between friendsickness, precollege predictors, and dimensions of college adjustment 10 weeks into the first semester. As hypothesized, friendsickness was associated with precollege social concerns, discrepancy between precollege expectations and college experiences, more precollege friends in the college social network, and loneliness and poor self‐esteem in college. Implications for precollege prevention of and college intervention efforts for friendsickness are discussed.