ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Long-Term Adaptation to Sway-Yoked Visual Motion and Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation on Visual and Vestibular Control of Posture

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Human postural control is a multimodal process involving visual and vestibular information. The aim of the present study was to measure individual differences in the contributions of vision and vestibular senses to postural control, and to investigate if the individual weights could be modulated by long-term adaptation to visual motion or galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS). Since GVS is a less expensive technique than a motion platform and can be wearable, it is a promising virtual reality (VR) technology. We measured the postural sway of observers induced by a visual motion or GVS before and after a 7-day adaptation task. We divided participants into four groups. In visual adaptation groups, visual motions were presented to either enhance voluntary body movement (enhancing vision group) or inhibit voluntary body movement (inhibiting vision group). In GVS adaptation groups, GVS was applied to enhance voluntary body movement (enhancing GVS group) or inhibit voluntary body movement (inhibiting GVS group). The adaptation to enhancing body-movement-yoked visual motion decreased the GVS-induced postural sway at a low motion frequency. The adaptation to the enhancing GVS slightly increased the GVS-induced postural sway and decreased the visually-induced sway at a low motion frequency. The adaptation to the inhibiting GVS increased the GVS-induced postural sway and decreased the visually-induced sway at a high motion frequency. These data suggest that long-term adaptation can modify weights of vision and vestibular senses to control posture. These findings can be applied to training or rehabilitation systems of postural control and also to adaptive virtual-reality systems.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Michiteru Kitazaki*
Department of Information Science
and Engineering
Toyohashi University of Technology
Toyohashi, Aichi 441-8580, Japan
Takuya Kimura
Department of Knowledge-Bas ed
Information Engineering
Toyohashi University of Technology
Japan
Effects of Long-Term Adaptation
to Sway-Yoked Visual Motion and
Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation on
Visual and Vestibular Control of
Posture
Abstract
Human postural control is a multimodal process involving visual and vestibular informa-
tion. The aim of the present study was to measure individual differences in the contri-
butions of vision and vestibular senses to postural control, and to investigate if the indi-
vidual weights could be modulated by long-term adaptation to visual motion or
galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS). Since GVS is a less expensive technique than a
motion platform and can be wearable, it is a promising virtual reality (VR) technology.
We measured the postural sway of observers induc ed by a visual motion or GVS
before and after a 7-day adaptation task. We divided participants into four groups. In
visual adaptation groups, visual motions were presented to either enhance voluntary
body movement (enhancing vision group) or inhibit voluntary body movement (inhibi-
ting vision group). In GVS adaptation groups, GVS was applied to enhance voluntary
body movement (enhancing GVS group) or inhibit voluntary body movement (inhibi-
ting GVS group). The adaptation to enhancing body-movement-yoked visual motion
decreased the GVS-induced postural sway at a low motion frequency. The adaptation
to the enhancing GVS slightly increased the GVS-induced postural sway and decreased
the visually-induced sway at a low motion frequency. The adaptatio n to the inhibiting
GVS increased the GVS-induced postural sway and decreased the visually-induced
sway at a high motion frequency. These data suggest that long-term adaptation can
modify weights of vision and vestibular senses to control posture. These findings can
be applied to training or rehabilitation systems of postural control and also to ada ptive
virtual-reality systems.
1 Introduction
1.1 Human Postural Control from Vision and Vestibular
Senses
Human posture is a multimodal process controlled by visual, vestibular,
and proprioceptive information. Visually-induced postural sway has been widely
studied. Spontaneous postural sway decreases by 50–60% with a stable visual
environment (Travis, 1945; Edwards, 1946), while a moving room can affect
the posture of both adults and infants (Lee & Lishman, 1975; Lee & Aronson,
Presence, Vol. 19, No. 6, December 2010, 544–556
ª 2011 by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology *Correspondence to mich@cs.tut.ac.jp.
544
PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
1974). When a visual field contains a large visual motion,
body sway of observers occurs at an identical frequency
to the visual motion (Lestienne, Soechting, & Berthoz,
1977). Thus, when we use a cyclic visual motion, the
sway occurs at the same frequency of the visual motion.
Recently, galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) has
been investigated for vestibularly-induced postural sway.
When a small current is applied to the left and right mas-
toid processes, observers incline in the direction of the
anodal ear (Day, 1999). When GVS is applied to a walk-
ing observer, the walking trajectory is affected (Fitzpa-
trick, Wardman, & Taylor, 1999). GVS has also been
applied to some virtual reality systems. For instance, vir-
tual acceleration of self-motion was induced by GVS
with simultaneous presentation of visual motions
(Maeda et al., 2005; Maeda, Ando, & Sugimoto, 2005).
GVS was also applied to enhance music experience for
virtual-reality entertainment (Nagaya et al., 2006) and to
transfer body balance as a vestibular telepresence system
(Yoshida, Ando, Maeda, & Watanabe, 2008). Since the
GVS is a small system and is inexpensive, it is advanta-
geous in comparison with other larger systems such as a
motion platform, and is becoming increasing popular in
the virtual reality field.
1.2 Plasticity of the Human Perception-
Action Syst em
The human perception-action coordination system
is known to rapidly adapt to new situations. The most fa-
mous and traditional paradigm to investigate the adapta-
tion of the perception-action system is inverted vision
with a prism scope (Stratton, 1896). When wearing the
prism scope, the observer’s perceptual world is inverted,
causing staggering. After prolonged adaptation (1–4
weeks) the perceptual world returns to a proper orienta-
tion, and the observer is able to walk, run, and ride a
bicycle. A more moderate version of this system is dis-
torted optical stimulation (Welch, 1969), in which man-
ual pointing to a visual target fails and is shifted to the
distorted position when the observer observes the world
through the distortion prism. After adapting to the
prism, the observer can correctly point to the target.
These studies suggest that the human perception-
action system is plastic and adaptive to a new environ-
ment. When observers are moving, they sense both vis-
ual and vestibular sensations. In daily life, visual informa-
tion and vestibular information are consistent. For
example, when we walk 5 m, visual information and ves-
tibular information are equivalent and have a value of
5 m. In some studies, the gain of visual motion when an
observer actually moved was manipulated, and the pro-
longed adaptation to a new gain resulted in modification
of the perceptual stable gain in the direction of the
adapted gain (Wallach & Canal, 1976; Becklen, Wallach,
& Nitzberg, 1984; Wallach, 1987). Using VR techni-
ques, adaptation to a visual-vestibular conflict for 45 min
was reported to modify the vestibular sensation, but not
the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR; Ivanenko, Viaud-
Delmon, Siegler, Israel, & Berthoz, 1998; Viaud-
Delmon, Ivanenko, Grasso, & Israel, 1999). The degree
of this adaptation is stronger for males than females
(Viaud-Delmon, Ivanenko, Berthoz, & Jouvent, 1998).
In a previous study, we manipulated the gain of visual
motion when an observer rotates his head when wearing
a head-mounted display (HMD), and examined adaptive
changes in the most stable gain perceived by the observer
(Kitazaki & Shimizu, 2005). We found that the stable
gain changed after active adaptation for 2 min, and that
the adaptation for visual stability is related to relatively
higher information processing, at least after the fusion of
binocular sources, but is specific to or modulated by the
retinal location. In clinical studies, Jenkins and col-
leagues reported that the VOR gradually changes for
120 days after vestibular ablation (Jenkins, Cohen, &
Kimball, 2000; Jenkins, 1985).
1.3 Relevance to Virtual Reality
We propose that combining GVS and visual stimu-
lation with the plasticity of the perceptual-action system
of the brain will provide new virtual reality applications;
for instance, an adaptive telepresence system for mobile
observers who wear an HMD for vision and a GVS sys-
tem for vestibular sensation, and who receive visual and
vestibular sensation from other people at distance loca-
tions. The telepresence system needs to send multimodal
Kitazaki and Kimura 545
information such as vision and vestibular sensation.
However, they may not necessarily be sent with high re-
solution. If the quality of vision is lower than the vestib-
ular information, users of the system should utilize the
vestibular information more than the degraded visual in-
formation. If users rely more on the vestibular informa-
tion with a long-term usage/adaptation, users would
gradually adapt to the telepresence system appropriately.
The aim of the present study was to measure individ-
ual differences in the contributing weights of vision and
vestibular senses to postural control, and to investigate
whether the individual weights could be modulated by a
long-term active adaptation to visual motion and GVS.
2 General Methods
2.1 Participants
Twenty-four graduate and undergraduate students
participated with written informed consent. All partici-
pants were male and 20–23 years old. Since a previous
study reported that the degree of vestibular adaptation is
stronger for males than females (Viaud-Delmon et al.,
1998), we employed only male participants. This study
was approved by the Committee for Human-Subject
Studies of the Toyohashi University of Technology.
2.2 Apparatus
All experiments were conducted in a semi-dark
room, as shown in Figure 1(a). Visual stimuli were pre-
sented on a rear-projection screen (width height, 2.43
m 1.82 m) by a 3-CRT projector (Barco Cine7/II;
1024 768 pixels, 60 Hz refresh rate). GVS was gener-
ated by a D/A device (National Instruments PCI-6704),
and applied through the left and right mastoid processes
with a pair of disposable Ag/AgCI electrodes (Ambu
Blue Sensor P-00-S; Figure 1[b]).
The head movement and center of gravity for the
indexes of postural sway were measured for all partici-
pants. The head motion was measured by a magnetic
3D motion tracker (Polhemus FASTRAK) at 60 Hz.
The center of gravity was measured by a force plate
(NEC EB1101) and recorded through an A/D device
(National Instruments PCI-6024) to a computer. The
computer controlled visual stimuli and GVS measured
both the head motion and the center of gravity. Thus,
the timing of stimulus presentation and recording pos-
tural data were synchronized.
2.3 Experimental Design
We employed a typical experimental paradigm to
investigate adaptation: pretests and posttests with an ad-
aptation (Figure 2). First, all 24 participants performed
an experiment to measure postural sway induced by vis-
ual stimulus and GVS (pretest). Next, the participants
adapted to one of four perception-action combinations
for 7 days. For five participants, visual motion was pre-
sented to enhance voluntary body movement (enhancing
body-movement-yoked visual motion). For six partici-
Figure 1. Experimental apparatus. (a) Experimental scene in a dark
room. (b) Disposable electrode at left mastoid.
546 PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
pants, visual motion was presented to inhibit the move-
ment (inhibiting body-movement-yoked visual motion).
For six participants, GVS was applied to enhance volun-
tary body movement (enhancing body-movement-yoked
GVS). For seven participants, GVS was applied to inhibit
voluntary body movement (inhibiting body-movement-
yoked GVS). After the adaptation period, all participants
repeated an identical postural-sway experiment to test
the effect of adaptation (posttest). The changes of visu-
ally- and GVS-induced postural sways before and after
the adaptation reflect the effect of adaptation and the
plasticity of the multimodal postural control system.
The individual variance of the adaptation methods was
examined as between-subject conditions (four partici-
pant groups), while the effect of adaptation was investi-
gated with within-subject design by comparing the
results of the pretest with those of the posttest. The
numbers of participants in the four adaptation groups
were unequal as some participants canceled prior to per-
forming experiments.
3 Visually- and GVS-Induced Postural
Sways (Pre test)
3.1 Visual an d GVS Stimuli
The modality of the motion stimulus was the most
critical independent variable. Either visual stimulus or
GVS was presented to a participant for each trial with a
within-subject design. For the visual stimulus, we pre-
sented a lateral motion of random dots on the front-par-
allel screen (width height, 58.38 50.58 in visual
angle) at 1.5 m viewing distance. Five thousand random
dots moved laterally (leftward and rightward) back and
forth with a sinusoidal speed modulation (travel dis-
tance, 11.68 to 5.88 left and right from the center). Each
dot was a square (0.458 0.458) and red (0.76 cd/m
2
)
on a black background. The density of the random dots
on the background was 15.9% (see Figure 3).
For the GVS, we presented a weak current (minimum
0.1 mA, maximum 0.5 mA) through left and right mas-
toids; the current modulation was sinusoidal and similar
to the visual motion. Although the resolution of current
modulation was limited to 0.1 mA steps by the appara-
tus, the postural sway of participants was natural (see
Figure 4[a]). The motion frequencies of visual motion
and GVS were varied as an independent variable (0.1,
0.2, and 0.3 Hz) with within-subject design. Since the
Figure 2. Diagram of experimental design.
Figure 3. Visual stimuli. (a) Schematic stimulus of random dot visual
motion. (b) Visual motion varied with motion frequency. Motion of ran-
dom dots plotted against time for 0.1, 0.2, and 0.3 Hz conditions.
Figure 4. Sample data of postural sway and frequency analysis.
(a) Postural sway data of a trial of a subject and the GVS current were
plotted against time. (b) FFT was applied to the left data, and the
sway power was plotted against the sway frequency.
Kitazaki and Kimura 547
duration of a trial was always 90 s, the 9, 18, and 27
cycles of visual or GVS motions were presented for 0.1,
0.2, and 0.3 Hz conditions, respectively. A fixation point
(a cross, 4.68 4.68) was placed at the center of the dis-
play, to which participants were asked to fix their eyes. In
the visual motion condition, moving random dots were
presented around the fixation point and, in the GVS
condition, only the fixation point was presented on a
black background.
3.2 Procedure
A session contained a condition combination of
two stimulus modalities (Vision or GVS) and three
motion frequencies (0.1, 0.2, or 0.3 Hz), where these
six conditions/trials were presented in a random order.
Each participant conducted 10 sessions. Thus, every
condition was repeated 10 times.
3.3 Results
All participants showed postural sways induced by
visual and GVS motions. An example of postural sway
induced by GVS for a trial of a subject is shown in Figure
4(a). We applied FFT to each trial data, and extracted
postural-sway powers at the frequency of the visual or
GVS motion (see Figure 4[b]). The postural sway of an
observer typically occurs at the same frequency as the
stimulus motion or modulation. We obtained two types
of indexes for postural sway: center of gravity and head
motion. However, as the results of these two indexes
were similar, only the center of gravity data are pre-
sented.
3.3.1 Individual Differences. Postural-sway
powers were presented at the same frequency of the
stimulus motion for visual- and GVS-modality condi-
tions (see Figure 5), and individual data (n ¼ 24) were
plotted to observe large individual differences. The indi-
vidual differences were larger for visually-induced sways
than for GVS-induced sways. The absolute power of the
most sensitive participant was 1,000 times larger than
less sensitive participants for vision, and 100 times larger
for GVS.
To determine the ratios of contributing weights of
vision and vestibular for each subject’s postural control,
correlations of visually-induced sways and GVS-induced
sways were plotted (see Figure 6), where each dot repre-
sents a subject. Data were divided into three motion-fre-
quency conditions. The horizontal axis indicates power
of visually-induced postural sways at the stimulus fre-
quency, and the vertical axis indicates power of GVS-
induced sways. There were significant positive correla-
tions between visually- and GVS-induced sways
(r ¼ 0.58 and p ¼ .0030 for 0.1 Hz, r ¼ 0.52 and p ¼
.0089 for 0.2 Hz, and r ¼ 0.53 and p ¼ .0081 for 0.3
Hz). Thus, subjects who were sensitive to the visual
motion were also sensitive to the GVS for the induced
postural sway. Since many subjects were plotted in the
upper-left side of the center oblique line, the GVS-
induced sways were larger than the visually-induced sway
for many subjects.
3.3.2 Effects of Sensory Modality and Motion
Frequency. Data were normalized using the individual
averages as absolute values of the postural sways were
large. Normalized data (relative value to the individual
average) are shown in Figure 7. Data are averages of all
24 participants with error bars of the standard errors.
The GVS-induced postural sway was larger than the visu-
ally-induced postural sway, particularly at low frequen-
cies. The GVS-induced sway decreased with a high fre-
quency. A repeated measures ANOVA (two ways; two
modalities three frequencies) showed a significant
Figure 5. Individual results of visually- and GVS-induced postural sways
(pretests). Sway powers induced by visual and GVS stimuli were plotted
against stimulus frequency conditions for all subjects.
548 PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
main effect of the modality (p ¼ .0002) and the fre-
quency (p ¼ .0087), and an interaction between them
(p ¼ .0211). These data were identical to those pre-
sented in our previous study (Kitazaki & Kimura, 2008).
4 Long-Term Adaptation
4.1 Adaptation Groups
Participants were randomly divided into four
groups prior to pretests: the inhibiting body-movement-
yoked vision group (six participants), the enhancing
body-movement-yoked vision group (five participants),
the inhibiting body-movement-yoked GVS group (seven
participants), and the enhancing body-movement-yoked
GVS group (six participants).
4.2 Methods
The apparatus was identical to the pretests except
that the force plate was excluded. The motion of each
participant’s head was monitored by a motion tracker,
and visual motion or GVS were continuously manipu-
lated online depending on their motion at 60 Hz. In the
enhancing vision adaptation, when the participant
inclined rightward, the random dots moved rightward to
induce more rightward postural sway (and vice versa). In
the inhibiting vision adaptation, when the participant
Figure 6. Correlation between visually- and GVS-induced postural sways (pretests). Each dot repre-
sents a subject. The horizontal axis indicates powers of visually-induced postural sways at the stimulus
frequency, and the vertical axis indicates powers of GVS-induced sways.
Figure 7. Averaged results of visually- and GVS-induced postural sways
(pretests). Individually normalized data of sway power induced by visual
and GVS stimuli were averaged and plotted against the stimulus motion
frequency. Error bars indicate standard errors.
Kitazaki and Kimura 549
inclined rightward, the random dots moved leftward.
For these two conditions, the manipulation of visual
motion was the opposite. In the enhancing GVS adapta-
tion, when the participant inclined rightward, the GVS
induced a rightward postural sway. In the inhibiting
GVS adaptation, when the participant inclined leftward,
the GVS induced a rightward postural sway. For these
two conditions, the manipulation of GVS was the oppo-
site.
Participants were asked to sway their body laterally
back and forth with a 30 cm travel distance (15 cm left
and right from the center) at 0.2 Hz. To guide this body
motion, two vertical bars in red and green were pre-
sented on the screen (see Figure 8). The red bar moved
left and right with sinusoidal speed modulation at 0.2
Hz. The green bar represented the current (online) posi-
tion of the participant’s head. We asked participants to
move the green bar on the red bar as accurately as possi-
ble. Each trial continued for 60 s. Each subject per-
formed 10 trials a day, and continued for 7 days. Thus,
every participant performed 70 trials in total.
5 Effects of Long-Term Adaptation on
Induced Postural Sway (Posttest)
5.1 Methods
The methods were identical to the pretest. All par-
ticipants performed the same experiment.
5.2 Results
5.2.1 Amplitude of Postural Sways. Identical
analyses to those of the pretest were performed. Data
were plotted for inhibiting vision (see Figure 9), enhanc-
ing vision (see Figures 10 and 11), inhibiting GVS (see
Figure 12), and enhancing GVS (see Figures 13 and 14)
groups. In Figures 9, 10, 12 and 13, the left graph repre-
sents pretest data of the same group subjects, while the
center graph represents posttest data. To determine indi-
vidual changes before and after the adaptation, the dif-
ference of the induced postural sway before and after the
adaptation was calculated by subtracting the pretest data
Figure 8. Display for the adaptation experiment.
Figure 9. Effects of adaptation in the inhibiting vision group. (Left) Pre-
test data of the inhibiting vision group in the same format as in Figure 7.
(Center) Posttest data of the inhibiting vision group. (Right) The differ-
ence between the posttest data and the pretest data were plotted to
see adaptation effects.
Figure 10. Effects of the adaptation in the enhancing vision group.
The graph format matches that in Figure 9.
550 PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
from the posttest data; these data are plotted in the
right-side graph of Figures 9, 10, 12 and 13.
There were no effects of the adaptation for the inhibi-
ting vision adaptation condition (see Figure 9), while the
visually-induced sway slightly increased and the GVS-
induced sway decreased at low frequency (0.1 Hz) for
the enhancing vision adaptation condition (see Figure
10). Repeated measures ANOVA for the data from Fig-
ure 9 (right) and Figure 10 (right) were performed to
examine the effects of the adaptation (three ways: two
adaptation groups two modalities three frequen-
cies). There was a significant three-way interaction
between adaptation (inhibiting/enhancing) modality
frequency (p ¼ .0491), which supports the observa-
tion that the enhancing vision adaptation only decreased
GVS-induced sway and increased visually-induced sway
at the low frequency. Single-sample t-tests were per-
formed to test whether power changes of postural sways
were different from zero for each condition; there was a
near-significant effect suggesting that GVS-induced sway
was decreased at 0.1 Hz (p ¼ .079). Next, paired t-tests
were performed to test the difference between the GVS-
induced sways and the visually-induced sways for each
frequency condition; there was only a weak tendency for
a difference between GVS- and visually-induced sways
Figure 11. Effects of adaptation in the enhancing vision group at
0.1 Hz in correlation plots. Correlations of powers of GVS- and visually-
induced sways were plotted following the format of Figure 6. Both pre-
test and posttest data were plotted. Arrows indicate individual changes
before and after adaptation.
Figure 12. Effects of the adaptation in the inhibiting GVS group. The
graph format follows that of Figure 9.
Figure 13. Effects of the adaptation in the enhancing GVS group. The
graph format follows that of Figure 9.
Figure 14. Effects of the adaptation in the inhibiting GVS group at 0.3
Hz in correlation plots. The graph format follows that of Figure 11.
Kitazaki and Kimura 551
with the enhancing vision adaptation at 0.1 Hz (p ¼
.1441).
For the 0.1 Hz condition of the enhancing vision
group before and after the adaptation, each subject’s
sway power was plotted to determine the visually-
induced sways and GVS-induced sways (see Figure 11);
these data were not normalized by individual averages.
For four of the five subjects, contributing weights of
vision increased while those of GVS decreased.
For the inhibiting GVS adaptation condition, the
GVS-induced sway increased particularly at the high fre-
quency (0.3 Hz), while the visually-induced sway slightly
decreased (see Figure 12). For the enhancing GVS adap-
tation condition, the GVS-induced sway slightly
increased and the visually-induced sway decreased at the
low frequency (see Figure 13).
Repeated measures ANOVA for the data from Figure
12 (right) and 13 (right; three ways: two adaptation
groups two modalities three frequencies) was per-
formed to test the effects of the adaption; however, there
were no effects as the individual differences were too
large. Next, single-sample t-tests were performed to
determine whether the power changes of postural sway
were different from zero for each condition. There was a
near-significant effect of increased GVS-induced sway at
0.3 Hz by inhibiting GVS adaptation (p ¼ .054). Paired
t-tests were performed to test the difference between
GVS-induced sway and visually-induced sway for each
frequency condition. For the inhibiting GVS condition,
there was a significant effect of the adaptation (p ¼ .026)
at 0.3 Hz where the GVS-induced sway increased signifi-
cantly more than the visually-increased sway. For the
enhancing GVS condition, there was a near significant
effect of the adaptation (p ¼ .060) at 0.1 Hz, where the
GVS-induced sway increased more than the visually-
induced sway.
For the 0.3 Hz condition of the inhibiting GVS group
and the 0.1 Hz condition of the enhancing GVS group
before and after the adaptation, each subject’s sway
power was plotted to determine ratios of the visually-
induced sways and the GVS-induced sways (see Figures
14 and 15, respectively). For five of the seven subjects in
the inhibiting GVS group, the contributing weights of
GVS increased and those of vision decreased (see Figure
14). For three of the six subjects in the enhancing GVS
group, contributing weights of GVS increased and those
of vision decreased (see Figure 15).
5.2.2 Phase D elay of Postural Sways. The
phases of postural sway at the same frequency of visual
or GVS stimuli were calculated, and pretests, posttests,
and their difference were plotted in Figures 16, 17, 18,
and 19. Postural sway delays to stimulus motions, partic-
ularly GVS-induced sways, have a large delay for
30–1208 (Kitazaki & Kimura, 2008), which may affect
the delay of postural sways. Similar ANOVAs (three
Figure 15. Effects of the adaptation in the enhancing vision group at
0.1 Hz in correlation plots.
Figure 16. Effects of adaptation on phase delay in the inhibiting vision
group. The phase delays were plotted following the format of Figure 9.
(Left) Pretest data of the inhibiting vision group. (Center) Posttest data.
(Right) The difference between the posttest data and the pretest data.
552 PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
ways: two adaptation groups [inhibiting/enhancing]
two modalities three frequencies) were performed for
visual adaptations and GVS adaptations. Single-sample
t-tests and paired t-tests were also performed, similar to
the power/amplitude analysis. There was no effect of ad-
aptation in the inhibiting vision group (see Figure 16).
For the enhancing vision group, the delay of visually-
induced sway increased at the middle frequency (0.2 Hz;
see Figure 17; single-sample t-test, p ¼ .064; paired t-
test p ¼ .076). However, there was no significant effect
of the ANOVA.
For the inhibiting GVS group, the delay of visually-
induced sways increased at the high frequency (0.3 Hz;
see Figure 18; single-sample t-test, p ¼ .079). For the
enhancing GVS group, the delay of visually-induced
sway decreased at the middle frequency (0.2 Hz; see Fig-
ure 19; single-sample t-test, p ¼ .090). For the three-
way ANOVA, there was a significant interaction of
modalities and frequencies (p ¼ .035), suggesting that
both inhibiting and enhancing GVS adaptations
increased the delay of visually-induced sway increase at
the high frequency and decrease at the low frequency.
6 Discussion
We performed a long-term (7-day) adaptation
experiment using a body-movement-yoked visual
motion and GVS. Participants received visual motions or
GVS to either inhibit or enhance their voluntary sway.
The enhancing vision adaptation decreased the GVS-
induced sway and slightly increased the visually-induced
sway only at the low frequency (0.1 Hz). The adaptation
to the inhibiting GVS increased the GVS-induced pos-
tural sway and decreased the visually-induced sway, par-
ticularly at a high motion frequency (0.3 Hz). The adap-
tation to the enhancing GVS slightly increased the GVS-
induced postural sway and decreased the visually-
induced sway, particularly at a low motion frequency
(0.1 Hz). These results are summarized in Table 1.
For the delay of postural sway, the enhancing vision
adaptation increased the delay of visually-induced sway
at the frequency of adaptation (0.2 Hz). The adaptation
to the inhibiting GVS increased the delay of visually-
induced postural sway at a high motion frequency (0.3
Hz), while the adaptation to the enhancing GVS slightly
decreased the visually-induced postural sway at the fre-
quency of adaptation (0.2 Hz).
Figure 19. Effects of the adaptation on phase delay in the enhancing
GVS group. The graph format matches that of Figure 16.
Figure 17. Effects of adaptation on phase delay in the enhancing
vision group. The graph format follows that of Figure 16.
Figure 18. Effects of the adaptation on phase delay in the inhibiting
GVS group. The graph format follows that of Figure 16.
Kitazaki and Kimura 553
6.1 Modification of Sway Amplitudes
and Modality Contributi ons
The enhancing vision adaptation increased the
weight of the visual control of posture relative to that of
the vestibular control, which is reasonable as the enhanc-
ing vision adaptation seemed to make visual information
more reliable for the purpose of controlling posture. We
suggest that the effect was limited to the lower frequency
as the difference of the weights of vision and vestibular
senses are large at the low frequency (see Figure 7).
The inhibiting GVS adaptation increased the weight
of the vestibular control of posture relative to that of the
visual control at a high motion frequency. Although this
result is controversial, speculatively, our visual and ves-
tibular postural control may not be good at higher fre-
quencies (Stoffregen, 1986; van Asten, Gielen, &
van der Gon, 1988; Kitazaki & Kimura, 2008). By
exposure to inhibiting GVS, a negative aftereffect might
occur at the frequency vulnerable to posture controls,
then GVS-induced sway would increase after adaptation.
The enhancing GVS adaptation increased the weight of
the vestibular control of posture relative to that of the
visual control at 0.1 Hz, which is similar to our findings
of the enhancing vision adaptation.
6.2 Modification of Sway Delay
For all conditions and groups, the delay of the
GVS-induced sway was not affected by the adaptation.
Thus, the temporal system of the vestibular control of
posture seems robust. The delay of the visually-induced
sway was increased by the adaptations to enhancing
vision at 0.2 Hz, and decreased by the adaptation to
enhancing GVS at 0.2 Hz. If the enhancing vision adap-
tation makes visual information more reliable to control
postures, the delay should be decreased. However, our
results were the opposite. We speculate that the temporal
aspects of visual control of posture are slightly sensitive
to the adaptation, but cannot be accounted for by the
modification of modality contributions. Further studies
on this area are required.
6.3 Potential Contributions to VR
Application
Our data suggest that the long-term adaptation to
the body-movement-yoked visual motion and GVS can
modify weights on vision and vestibular senses to control
posture. This reflects great plasticity in the perception-
action system. Our findings can be applied to an adaptive
virtual-reality system or a training or rehabilitation sys-
tem of postural control in the future. For the adaptive
telepresence system for mobile observers, our results
suggest that enhancing visual motion and GVS can mod-
ify the weights of visual and vestibular sensation for pos-
tural control. If the quality of vision is lower than the
vestibular information, the amplitude of the vestibular
information should be increased with the GVS. For
long-term usage/adaptation, users would gradually rely
more on the vestibular information, and the degraded
visual information would not deteriorate the perform-
ance. However, inhibiting visual motion would have no
effect for the adaptive system. There is also a limitation
with respect to the effective frequency. The adaptation
Table 1. Summary of Adaptation Effects*
[Visual motion] [GVS]
[Enhanced] þVisual sway
GVS sway at low frequency
Visual sway
þGVS sway at low frequency
[Inhibited] No effect Visual
þGVS sway at high frequency
*The first and second columns indicate modalities of adaptation, and the first and second rows indicate the direction
of the adaptation (enhanced or inhibited). If postural sway is increased after adaptation, þ is indicated. If postural
sway is decreased after adaptation, – is indicated.
554 PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
effects with enhancing visual motion and GVS mainly
appear at low motion frequency. Thus, the proposed sys-
tem may be effective for slow self-motion rather than fast
movements.
If vestibular sensitivity worsens more than vision due
to aging or accidental disorder, then visual information
should be used to control posture more than the vestib-
ular information. We propose the use of an AR applica-
tion to enhance optic flow to increase the visual weight
for postural control as a training or rehabilitation system.
Optic flow can be presented by an HMD and the vestib-
ular information can be presented by GVS. Since both
the HMD and GVS are small and wearable, the AR train-
ing system can be used at home, and will not impair the
quality of life of the user. After use of this system for a
period, users will rely more on visual information rather
than vestibular information. As our proposed applica-
tions work gradually and adaptively, the effects may by
optimized by quitting adaptation at an appropriate time
while evaluating adaptation effects continuously. How-
ever, our findings are still preliminary, and further
experiments are required with a larger number of partici-
pants.
7 Conclusions
We measured the postural sway of observers
induced by a visual motion or GVS before and after a
7-day adaptation task. Adaptations to enhanced visual
motion and GVS that were yoked to the observer’s vol-
untary self-motion caused an increase in visually-induced
postural sway and GVS-induced postural sway, respec-
tively. Thus, the long-term adaptation can modify
weights of vision and vestibular senses over postural con-
trol. These findings can be applied to a training or reha-
bilitation system of postural control, and also as an
adaptive virtual-reality system.
Acknowledgm ents
This research was supported by the Nissan Science Foundation,
a Grant-in-aid for Young Scientists (B), and The Global COE
Program (Frontiers of Intelligent Sensing) by MEXT Japan.
References
Becklen, R., Wallach, H., & Nitzberg, R. (1984). A limitation
of position constancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 10, 713–723.
Day, B. L. (1999). Galvanic vestibular stimulation: New uses
for an old tool. Journal of Physiology, 517, 631.
Edwards, A. S. (1946). Body sway and vision. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology, 36, 526–535.
Fitzpatrick R. C., Wardman D. L., & Taylor J. L. (1999).
Effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation during human
walking. Journal of Physiology, 517, 931–939.
Ivanenko, Y. P., Viaud-Delmon, I., Siegler, I., Israel, I., & Ber-
thoz, A. (1998). The vestibulo-ocular reflex and angular dis-
placement perception in darkness in humans: Adaptation to
a virtual environment. Neuroscience Letters, 241, 167–170.
Jenkins, H. A. (1985). Long-term adaptive changes of the ves-
tibulo-ocular reflex in patients following acoustic neuroma
surgery. Laryngoscope, 95(10), 1224–1234.
Jenkins, H. A., Cohen, H. S., & Kimball, K. T. (2000). Long-
term vestibulo-ocular reflex changes in patients with vestibu-
lar ablation. Acta Otolaryngology, 120(2), 187–191.
Kitazaki, M., & Kimura, T. (2008). Frequency-phase analysis
of postural sway induced by visual motion and galvanic ves-
tibular stimulation. Journal of Vision, 8(6), 1046.
Kitazaki, M., & Shimizu, A. (2005). Visual-motor adaptation
to stabilize perceptual world: Its generality and specificity.
Proceedings of 15th International Conference on Artificial
Reality and Telexistence, 85–90.
Lee, D. N., & Aronson, E. (1974). Visual proprioceptive con-
trol of standing in human infants. Perception & Psychophysics,
15, 529–532.
Lee, D. N., & Lishman, J. R. (1975). Visual proprioceptive con-
trol of stance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 1, 87–95.
Lestienne, F., Soechting, J., & Berthoz, A. (1977). Postural
readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes.
Experimental Brain Research, 28, 363–384.
Maeda, T., Ando, H., Amemiya, T., Nagaya, N., Sugimoto,
M., & Inami, M. (2005). Shaking the world: Galvanic ves-
tibular stimulation as a novel sensation interface. ACM
SIGGRAPH 2005 Emerging Technologies. doi: 10.1145/
1187297.1187315
Maeda, T., Ando, H., & Sugimoto, M. (2005). Virtual acceler-
ation with galvanic vestibular stimulation in a virtual reality
environment. Proceedings of IEEE Virtual Reality 2005,
289–290.
Kitazaki and Kimura 555
Nagaya, N., Yoshidzumi, M., Sugimoto, M., Nii, H., Maeda,
T., Kitazaki, M., & Inami, M. (2006). Gravity Jockey: A
novel music experience with galvanic vestibular stimulation.
Proceedings of the 2006 ACM SIGCHI International Confer-
ence on Advances in Computer Entertainment Technology.
doi: 10.1145/1178823.1178881
Stoffregen, T. A. (1986). The role of optical velocity in the
control of stance. Perception & Psychophysics, 39(5), 355–
360.
Stratton, G. M. (1896). Some preliminary experiments on
vision without inversion of the retinal image. Psychological
Review, 3, 611–617.
Travis, R. C. (1945). An experimental analysis of dynamic and
static equilibrium. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35,
216–234.
van Asten, W. N., Gielen, C. C., & van der Gon, J. J. (1988).
Postural movements induced by rotations of visual scenes.
Journal of the Optical Society of America, A5(10), 1781–
1789.
Viaud-Delmon, I., Ivanenko, Y. P., Berthoz, A., & Jouvent, R.
(1998). Sex, lies and virtual reality. Nature Neuroscience, 1,
15–16.
Viaud-Delmon, I., Ivanenko, Y. P., Grasso, R., & Israel, I.
(1999). Non-specific directional adaptation to asymmetrical
visual-vestibular stimulation. Cognitive Brain Research, 7,
507–510.
Wallach, H. (1987). Perceiving a stable environment when one
moves. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 1–27.
Wallach, H., & Canal, T. (1976). Two kinds of adaptation in
the constancy of visual direction. Perception & Psychophysics,
19, 445–449.
Welch, R. B. (1969). Adaptation to prism-distorted vision: The
importance of target-pointing. Perception & Psychophysics, 5,
305–309.
Yoshida, T., Ando, H., Maeda, T., & Watanabe, J. (2008).
Externalized sense of balance using galvanic vestibular stimu-
lation. Proceedings of 12th Annual Meeting of the Association
for the Scientific Study of Consciousness, 249–250.
556 PRESENCE: VOLUME 19, NUMBER 6
... In many situations they provide largely redundant information so that loss of one is not critical. In situations where one sensory channel becomes critical for balance or another become false or unreliable, the CNS might selectively attend or ignore specific channels through a process of "reweighting" [2][3][4][5][6]. Current concepts of motor control are based on forward sensory and motor models [7][8] whereby the CNS responds to the sensed difference between the predicted sensory inflow for an action and the actual sensed inflow. ...
... When applied as a binaural bipolar stimulus it evokes a pattern of afferent firing that signals natural head rotation in roll [12][13]. When the CNS is repeatedly exposed to daily or weekly sessions of pseudorandom or sinusoidal vestibular stimulation, the stimulation initially causes severe postural disturbances, but this effect dissipates and balance returns to baseline levels after 7-8 sessions [2][3]. These results appear to reflect a reweighting of sensory inputs [2][3]. ...
... When the CNS is repeatedly exposed to daily or weekly sessions of pseudorandom or sinusoidal vestibular stimulation, the stimulation initially causes severe postural disturbances, but this effect dissipates and balance returns to baseline levels after 7-8 sessions [2][3]. These results appear to reflect a reweighting of sensory inputs [2][3]. Because vestibulo-ocular reflexes remain affected despite repeated exposure to these same artificial vestibular stimuli, reweighting likely involves slow cerebellar processes rather than actual changes at the level of the vestibular end-organs or reflexes [2]. ...
Article
Full-text available
To determine how the vestibular sense controls balance, we used instantaneous head angular velocity to drive a galvanic vestibular stimulus so that afference would signal that head movement was faster or slower than actual. In effect, this changed vestibular afferent gain. This increased sway 4-fold when subjects (N = 8) stood without vision. However, after a 240 s conditioning period with stable balance achieved through reliable visual or somatosensory cues, sway returned to normal. An equivalent galvanic stimulus unrelated to sway (not driven by head motion) was equally destabilising but in this situation the conditioning period of stable balance did not reduce sway. Reflex muscle responses evoked by an independent, higher bandwidth vestibular stimulus were initially reduced in amplitude by the galvanic stimulus but returned to normal levels after the conditioning period, contrary to predictions that they would decrease after adaptation to increased sensory gain and increase after adaptation to decreased sensory gain. We conclude that an erroneous vestibular signal of head motion during standing has profound effects on balance control. If it is unrelated to current head motion, the CNS has no immediate mechanism of ignoring the vestibular signal to reduce its influence on destabilising balance. This result is inconsistent with sensory reweighting based on disturbances. The increase in sway with increased sensory gain is also inconsistent with a simple feedback model of vestibular reflex action. Thus, we propose that recalibration of a forward sensory model best explains the reinterpretation of an altered reafferent signal of head motion during stable balance.
... Finally, the question of whether the proprioceptive weight remains constant during sway referencing is still open. Indeed, several research papers emphasize the importance of dynamic re-weighting to adapt to the environment have shown changes in sensory weights, such as up-weighting relevant sensory cues and down-weighting irrelevant ones [20,27]. Accordingly, during sway referencing, the weight of the irrelevant signals from the proprioceptive sensors may have been reduced. ...
Article
Full-text available
Postural control characteristics have been proposed as a predictor of Motion Sickness (MS). However, postural adaptation to sensory environment changes may also be critical for MS susceptibility. In order to address this issue, a postural paradigm was used where accurate orientation information from body sensors could be lost and restored, allowing us to infer sensory re-weighting dynamics from postural oscillation spectra in relation to car-sickness susceptibility. Seventy-one participants were standing on a platform (eyes closed) alternating from static phases (proprioceptive and vestibular sensors providing reliable orientation cues) to sway referenced to the ankle-angle phases (proprioceptive sensors providing unreliable orientation cues). The power spectrum density (PSD) on a 10 s sliding window was computed from the antero-posterior displacement of the center of pressure. Energy ratios (ERs) between the high (0.7–1.3 Hz) and low (0.1–0.7 Hz) frequency bands of these PSDs were computed on key time windows. Results showed no difference between MS and non-MS participants following loss of relevant ankle proprioception. However, the reintroduction of reliable ankle signals led, for the non-MS participants, to an increase of the ER originating from a previously up-weighted vestibular information during the sway-referenced situation. This suggests inter-individual differences in re-weighting dynamics in relation to car-sickness susceptibility.
... There is considerable evidence that sensory re-weighting occurs after exposure to conditions where the normal relationships between sensory cues are disrupted experimentally. Kitazaki and Kimura (2010) reported down-weighting of vestibular cues in postural stabilization when the cues were rendered irrelevant for estimating spatial orientation of the body. Others have shown similar effects over a period of weeks (Dilda et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Sensory dynamics can be re-shaped by environmental interaction, allowing adaptation to altered or unfamiliar conditions that would otherwise provoke challenges for the central nervous system. One such condition occurs in virtual reality, where sensory conflict is thought to induce cybersickness. Although the sensory re-weighting process is likely to underlie adaptation to cybersickness, evidence of a link between sensory re-weighting dynamics and cybersickness is rare. Here, we characterize the relationship between sensory re-weighting in a balance control task and cybersickness. Participants were exposed to visual oscillation while standing in tandem stance. The sway path length of the center of pressure (COP) was measured and averaged for each level of visual oscillation, and a ratio was computed between high and low oscillation magnitudes to reflect the relative contributions of multiple sensory sources of information concerning balance control. Results showed a significant relationship between the magnitude dependency of sway and common sub-scales of cybersickness: disorientation [r(21) = 0.45, p = 0.028] and oculomotor discomfort [r(21) = 0.45, p = 0.033]. We conclude that participants who reported less cybersickness were better-able to down-weight visual information at high magnitude oscillations, thus demonstrating a lower dependency between sway and visual magnitude. The results confirm the utility of balance control as an indicator of cybersickness, and support the role of multisensory re-weighting in determining an individual's tolerance to VR applications.
Article
Background: The vestibular system encodes head motion in space which is naturally accompanied by other sensory cues. Electrical stimuli, applied across the mastoid processes, selectively activate primary vestibular afferents which has spurred clinical and biomedical applications of electrical vestibular stimulation (EVS). When properly matched to head motion, EVS may also manipulate the closed-loop relationship between actions and vestibular feedback to reveal the mechanisms of sensorimotor recalibration and learning. New method: We designed a portable, low-cost real-time EVS system using an Arduino microcontroller programmed through Simulink that provides electrical currents based on head angular motion. We used well-characterized vestibular afferent physiological responses to head angular velocity and electrical current to compute head-motion equivalent of real-time modulatory EVS currents. We also examined if our system induced recalibration of the vestibular system during human balance control. Results: Our system operated at 199.9997Hz(±0.005Hz) and delivered head-motion-equivalent electrical currents with ∼10ms delay. The output driving the current stimulator matched the implemented linear model for physiological vestibular afferent dynamics with minimal background noise (<0.2% of ±10V range). Participants recalibrated to the modulated closed-loop vestibular feedback using visual cues during standing balance, replicating earlier findings. Comparison with existing methods: EVS is typically used to impose external perturbations that are independent of one's own movement. We provided a solution using open-source hardware to implement a real-time, physiologybased, and task-relevant vestibular modulations using EVS. Conclusions: Our portable, low-cost vestibular modulation system will make physiological closed-loop vestibular manipulations more accessible thus encouraging novel investigations and biomedical applications of EVS.
Conference Paper
Human perception and action adaptively change depending on everyday experiences of or exposures to sensory information in changing environments. I aimed to know how our perception-action system adapts and changes in modified virtual-reality (VR) environments, and investigated visuo-motor adaptation of position constancy in a VR environment, visual and vestibular postural control after 7-day adaptation to modified sensory stimulation, and learning of event related cortical potential during motor imagery for application to a brain-machine interface. I found that human perception system, perception-action coordination system, and underlying neural system could change to adapt a new environment with considering quantitative sensory-motor relationship, reliability of information, and required learning with real-time feedback. These findings may contribute to develop an adaptive VR system in a future, which can change adaptively and cooperatively with human perceptual adaptation and neural plasticity.
Article
Full-text available
Examined position constancy in human vision with 86 college students. When the eyes track a moving object, the image of a stationary target shifts on the retina colinearly with the eye movement. A compensation process called position constancy prevents this image shift from causing perceived target motion commensurate with the image shift. Four experiments were conducted to investigate whether position constancy operates when the image shift and the eye movement are not colinear. Exp I investigated the induced motion of a target that, while subject to induction, moved perpendicularly to its induced motion. In Exp II, the perceived target motion differed significantly from the objective target motion. In Exps III and IV, the perceived target motions were also in close agreement with the presumed image paths. Overall results indicate that position constancy can occur when, during the eye movement, the target undergoes a motion of its own. Results indicate that position constancy failed to operate when the direction of the target motion formed an angle with the direction of the eye movement. (12 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Though the retinal image changes drastically with the movement of our head or eyes, our perceptual world is stable. It is called visual stability or position constancy. When we observe a virtual-reality environment through a head-mounted display with moving our head, the perceptual world is stabilized by the appropriate visual-motor gain without significant system delay. It is known that when the gain is changed the visual stability adaptively changes. We aimed to know the generality and specificity of this adaptation to stabilize the perceptual world: What is limitation for the ability of our adaptive visual-motor system? We found that the visual-motor adaptation occurred irrespective of the amount of visual information or active/passive movement. Even though the eye for adapting and that for test were different, the adaptation was intact. When the retinal location (left or right hemi-field) for test was different from that for adaptation, the adaptation still occurred, but it was less than in the case of same retinal location. Thus, the visual-motor adaptation for visual stability is concerned with relatively higher information processing, at least after the fusion of binocular sources, but is specific to or modulated by the retinal location.
Article
Full-text available
Conducted 2 experiments with 24 undergraduates and 30 15-29 yr olds. Results show that vision functions proprioceptively as an integral component of the control system for maintaining a stance. In several stances, Ss' body sway could be controlled by moving their surroundings (i.e., by manipulating their visual proprioceptive information about body sway). A previous experiment had shown the same for toddlers. Results also show that visual proprioceptive information is generally more sensitive than mechanical proprioceptive information from the vestibular system and the ankles and feet and is yet more sensitive if the person is facing a nearby object. Consequently, vision normally improves balance in normal standing and especially in less practiced stances where ankle-foot proprioception is impoverished; indeed, if it is too impoverished, balance is dependent on visual proprioception. This suggests that, in learning a new stance, visual proprioception normally plays a lead role in tuning up ankle-foot proprioception and muscular control. (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Postural control is influenced by vision and vestibular senses. The purpose of this study was to investigate temporal properties of visual, vestibular and cross-modal controls of posture. Visual motion of random-dots (5000dots, lateral cyclic motion, sinusoidal speed modulation, travel distance 11.6deg) was presented on a large-screen (width 2.43 × height 1.82m) by a rear-projector. Galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) was applied through left and right mastoid processes (0.1–0.5mA, sinusoidal amplitude modulation). Both visual motion and GVS induced lateral (leftward-rightward) postural sway back and forth. Observers' center of pressure was measure by a force plate at 60Hz. In Experiment 1, either visual motion or GVS was presented for each trial. We varied the frequency of visual motion and GVS modulation at 0.1, 0.2, and 0.3Hz. Thus, the independent variables were the stimulus modality (Vision or GVS) and the frequency (0.1, 0.2, 0.3Hz). The sway data were analyzed using FFT, and its power and phase at the stimulus frequency were calculated. In results, GVS induced much stronger sway than visual motion. The GVS-induced sway was weaker for 0.3Hz modulation than lower frequencies. Phase (lag) of induced sway increased as stimulus frequency became higher (Vision: 13, 15, 20deg for 0.1, 0.2, 0.3Hz, GVS: 28, 97, 124deg). These results suggest that temporal lag of sway behind sensory stimuli is not accounted by phase constant, but better understood by time constant (250ms for vision, 1100ms for GVS) or their combination. In Experiment 2 for the cross-modal sway, both visual motion and GVS were presented at the same frequency (0.2Hz) with different phase lags (0–360deg at 45deg step). We found the Visual-and-GVS-induced sway was strongest when vision lagged 90deg behind GVS, and weakest when GVS lagged 90deg behind vision. These results suggest that the vision and GVS are integrated in a weak fusion model to control posture.
Conference Paper
This study describes the relation between the vection produced by optical flow and that created by galvanic vestibular stimulation. Vection is the illusion of self motion and is most often experienced when an observer views a large screen display containing a translating pattern. This illusion has only limited fidelity and duration unless it is reinforced by confirming vestibular information. Galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) can directly produce the sensation of vection.
Conference Paper
We developed a novel sensation interface device using galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS). GVS alters your balance. Our device can induce vection (virtual sense of acceleration) synchronized with optic flow or musical rhythms. The device can also induce lateral walking towards the anode while human walking.
Article
Examined the theories of upright vision. Reports on some preliminary experiments conducted on the inverted visual field. One S was used to study whether the inversion of image is necessary for seeing things in an upright position. The S wore an optical contrivance to substitute an upright retinal position for the normal inverted one. The instrument was worn for 21.5 h over a period of three days. For the time that the instrument was not worn, the S was blindfolded. Initially all the images were inverted, things were seen in one way and thought about in a different way, because of memory and past experience. However, with practice the S learnt to adapt to the new visual field. On removal of the glasses, normal vision was restored. Concluded that difficulty in seeing things upright due to upright retinal images is due to previous experience. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
An experimental analysis of the factors involved in the two kinds of equilibrium and of the interrelationship of their components yielded the following major results: The dynamic component of equilibration is unrelated to the static component. There is practically no correlation between balancing skill on the stabilometer and ability manually to maintain orientation of a rotation chair in continuous displacement. Weight is of relatively greater importance than height in dynamic stabilometer performance, and distance from the center of gravity to the foot base line shows high correlation with standing height. When weight is controlled, there is a small sex difference in favor of women's performance on the stabilometer. Both dynamic and static equilibrium show greatly superior performance when visual cues operate. While mild exercise increases body sway significantly as a result of increased respiration and therefore of head movements, it shows little effect on dynamic stabilometer performance. A correlation of .50 between eye-manual co-ordination and balancing skill is believed to show presence of a steadiness factor in the two performances. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A series of 26 patients undergoing translabyrinthine acoustic neuroma surgery was evaluated pre- and postoperatively using rotatory vestibular testing. Patients were tested immediately prior to surgery and followed long-term at frequent intervals for periods of one week to two years, depending on availability and length of time postoperative. Rotatory testing was performed at multiple frequencies of 0.0125, 0.05, and 0.2 Hz, and the vestibulo-ocular reflex was measured using electro-oculographic techniques. Dependent variables of the slow component of the vestibulo-ocular reflex were derived by computer analysis, including gain, phase, DC bias, ampullopetal and ampullofugal responses of the remaining labyrinth, and asymmetry. These variables changed in a predictable fashion as related to the reciprocal of time. Results of this data are fitted with a linear statistical model, validating the changes.