ArticlePDF Available

Hearing in large and small dogs: Absolute thresholds and size of the tympanic membrane

Authors:

Abstract

Comparative studies of hearing have shown that the ability of different species of mammals to hear high-frequency sounds is inversely related to the distance between their ears (i.e., functional interaural distance). In order to determine whether this relation applies to within-species as well as to between-species variation in interaural distance, the audiograms of 4 dogs ranking in size from a Chihuahua to a Saint Bernard were determined. Ss were taught to respond to the presence or absence of a tone. The ears of 15 dogs, including the largest and smallest in this study, were then dissected and measured. Results indicate that there is no significant relation between interaural distance and the high-frequency hearing ability of individual dogs. Further analysis, which included measurements of the tympanic membrane, indicated that neither interaural distance nor area of the tympanic membrane is related to variation in high-frequency hearing, low-frequency hearing, or absolute sensitivity among dogs. (37 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)
... Moreover, behavioral methods have other advantages: comparative studies assessing the difference between physiological and behavioral measures of hearing sensitivity in humans reported higher and more variable responses across subjects when using ABR compared to behavioral methods [24]. In dogs, Markessis and colleagues [25] tried to compare electrophysiological thresholds with the behavioral thresholds obtained by Heffner [26], showing an important difference in the low-frequency threshold, with the behavioral threshold being twice as low as those obtained with the physiological assessment. Moreover, thresholds in the latter were slightly higher in all the other frequencies used. ...
... Moreover, thresholds in the latter were slightly higher in all the other frequencies used. Furthermore, in physiological studies in dogs, the inter-subject variability can be as high as 25 dB SPL [20], while computed data regarding the inter-subject variability from Heffner [26] found a standard deviation of 12 dB SPL at 0.5 kHz. In summary, behavioral assessment seems to grant a better accuracy and stability of thresholds in both humans and dogs. ...
... Despite these advantages, very little emphasis has been placed on devising behavioral methodologies to assess fundamental aspects of dogs' hearing. A handful of studies used a behavioral approach to study dogs' ability to localize sound [27][28][29], but, to the best of our knowledge, only Heffner [26] performed a study using a behavioral procedure to assess hearing threshold. The latter identified the hearing range of four dogs as ranging from 63 Hz to 47 kHz, determining the endpoints when sounds were only perceived if above 60 dB SPL. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is a growing interest in performing playback experiments to understand which acoustical cues trigger specific behavioral/emotional responses in dogs. However, very limited studies have focused their attention on more basic aspects of hearing such as sensitivity, i.e., the identification of minimal intensity thresholds across different frequencies. Most previous studies relied on electrophysiological methods for audiograms for dogs, but these methods are considered less accurate than assessments based on behavioral responses. To our knowledge, only one study has established hearing thresholds using a behavioral assessment on four dogs but using a method that did not allow potential improvement throughout the sessions. In the present study, we devised an assessment procedure based on a staircase method. Implying the adaptation of the assessed intensity on the dogs’ performance, this approach grants several assessments around the actual hearing threshold of the animal, thereby increasing the reliability of the result. We used such a method to determine hearing thresholds at three frequencies (0.5, 4.0, and 20.0 kHz). Five dogs were tested in each frequency. The hearing thresholds were found to be 19.5 ± 2.8 dB SPL at 0.5 kHz, 14.0 ± 4.5 dB SPL at 4.0 kHz, and 8.5 ± 12.8 dB SPL at 20.0 kHz. No improvement in performance was visible across the procedure. While the thresholds at 0.5 and 4.0 kHz were in line with the previous literature, the threshold at 20 kHz was remarkably lower than expected. Dogs’ ability to produce vocalization beyond 20 kHz, potentially used in short-range communication, and the selective pressure linked to intraspecific communication in social canids are discussed as potential explanations for the sensitivity to higher frequencies.
... Precisely, we aimed to identify sounds that may not be eligible for audiometric screening according to the three following characteristics: negative emotional impact, spectral narrowness, and spectral redundancy between sounds. The last two characteristics are very important in behavioral audiometric screening regarding the very large bandwidth of the canine audible field [41]. ...
... The green area shows the [250-4000 Hz] frequency region for which we observed a trend for negative emotional sensitivity to decrease as the sound center frequency/F0 increased. The grey area represents the sound frequencies that were tested in [41] but are above those of our 84-sound corpus (maximum F0 in the corpus = 9988 Hz; maximum center frequency of the 10 dB bandwidth = 11,122 Hz; maximum upper frequency of the 10 dB bandwidth = 17,033 Hz; see Table 1). Figure 3 presents the reports of negative emotional sensitivity as a function of the center frequency of the 10 dB bandwidth for the whole set of 84 sounds. ...
... Below, we attempted to assess whether the present results related to the shape of the canine minimum audible field, which determines how hearing varies over the full frequency range of hearing. In the sole behavioral study of the canine audible field that we are aware of, detection thresholds were measured in four conditioned dogs in an anechoic room [41]. A tone, whose frequency varied between 31 and 45,000 Hz across trials, was played back. ...
Article
Full-text available
While numerous dog behavioral studies use environmental sounds, the dog soundscape remains undescribed. We proposed a list of 79 sounds classified into six categories: Dog, Dog accessories, Human, city and vehicles, Garden, countryside and weather, and Household. In a survey, 620 dog owners scored the frequency of their dog’s exposure to, and thus, the recurrence of, each of the 79 sounds, from never to daily. The survey results also extended to about 25 sounds the number of acknowledged sounds that are likely to elicit stress or fear, that is, negative emotional sensitivity, in dogs. Sound recurrence and emotional sensitivity were not correlated, showing no beneficial effect of frequent exposure to, and no deleterious effect of scarcity of, sound events. We suggest that for the sake of dog welfare, researchers, veterinarians, trainers, and owners may limit dogs’ exposure to the sensitive sounds identified in the study during their dog observations and dog–human interactions. A corpus of 84 sounds was collected. The sounds were spectrally analyzed by determining their F0 and 10 dB bandwidth parameters. At the lowest sound frequencies, where canine hearing is poorest, negative emotional sensitivity was generally low. At the middle and high sound center frequencies/F0s, sensitivity greatly varied from lowest to highest, which is incompatible with both the general assumption and dog auditory detection thresholds. How emotional sensitivity relates to F0 (pitch) and hearing sensitivity remains undetermined. Finally, we suggest that future behavioral audiometric studies of dogs may maximize the spectral spread of each sound while minimizing the spectral overlap between sounds so as to reduce both the testing duration and the risk of inadvertently targeting or, conversely, missing frequency-dependent hearing impairments.
Article
Crop damage and vehicle collisions caused by wild ungulates require effective deterrents. Many commercial acoustic devices use ultrasonic signals, presenting them as aversive to these animals. However, unconvincing results reported in previous studies using (non-frequency modulated) ultrasounds bring their effectiveness as deterrent signals into question. Here, we explored the deterrent effectiveness of ultrasounds in several large mammalian species that are expected to perceive these high frequencies. Specifically, we tested the effect of different ultrasonic signals on red deer, peccaries, and wolves. Animals were exposed to sequences of nine acoustic signals in the 20–30 kHz frequency range, and to a lower-pitched signal (2–3 kHz). While red deer showed no aversion to ultrasonic signals (inaudible for humans), lower frequency (audible for humans) modulations increased aversive responses. Conversely, peccaries showed strong flight responses to several ultrasonic signals. Wolves also reacted to ultrasounds but did not flee. Our study confirms that ultrasonic signals are not suitable for deterring European red deer. However, our results suggest that their effectiveness in other species should be systematically tested, without assuming that their hearing frequency range is a reliable predictor of their behavioural responses to ultrasonic signals.
Chapter
This chapter discusses and defines the concepts that make up the foundation of this work. The first section discusses how behaviors can be defined in veterinary psychiatry and what factors can influence them. The second section deals with the neurophysiological aspect of behaviors by defining the general concepts of emotions, mood, cognition, and the notions of temperament and personality. The last section presents the particularities of communication and cognition in the canine species.
Article
Objective To investigate the prevalence of firework‐associated fear in dogs in Sydney, owner perception of their dog's response to fireworks, perceived efficacy of interventions to manage fearful behaviours and the frequency of dog owners seeking professional advice for these behaviours. Methods Dog owners in the Greater Sydney area were invited to complete an anonymous online survey. Results From 387 valid responses, 44.4% (171 of 385) reported their dogs were fearful of fireworks. The most common fear‐related behaviour was seeking an owner or caretaker (120 of 161, 74.5%). Most owners responded by bringing their dog inside or trying to comfort or reassure their dog. Only 22.5% of owners sought professional advice for their dog's fear of fireworks, but of these, 65.5% considered that advice to be effective. Source and breed group were significantly associated with fear of fireworks (P = 0.011, P = 0.036 respectively). Fear of fireworks was also significantly associated with fear of thunder (P < 0.0001), gunshots (P < 0.0001) and vehicles (P = 0.0009). Conclusion Fear of fireworks and other loud noises negatively impacts canine welfare, yet only a small percentage of owners sought professional advice. There is scope for veterinarians to educate owners and raise awareness about the identification and management of noise‐associated fear and reduce the risk of escalation of fearful behaviours.
Article
Although domestic dogs vary considerably in both body size and skull morphology, behavioural audiograms have previously been found to be similar in breeds as distinct as a Chihuahua and a St Bernard. In this study, we created micro‐CT reconstructions of the middle ears and bony labyrinths from the skulls of 17 dog breeds, including both Chihuahua and St Bernard, plus a mongrel and a wolf. From these reconstructions, we measured middle ear cavity and ossicular volumes, eardrum and stapes footplate areas and bony labyrinth volumes. All of these ear structures scaled with skull size with negative allometry and generally correlated better with condylobasal length than with maximum or interaural skull widths. Larger dogs have larger ear structures in absolute terms: the volume of the St Bernard's middle ear cavity was 14 times that of the Chihuahua. The middle and inner ears are otherwise very similar in morphology, the ossicular structure being particularly well‐conserved across breeds. The expectation that larger ear structures in larger dogs would translate into hearing ranges shifted towards lower frequencies is not consistent with the existing audiogram data. Assuming that the audiograms accurately reflect the hearing of the breeds in question, oversimplifications in existing models of middle ear function or limitations imposed by other parts of the auditory system may be responsible for this paradox.
Article
Circadian biology's impact on human physical health and its role in disease development and progression is widely recognized. The forefront of circadian rhythm research now focuses on translational applications to clinical medicine, aiming to enhance disease diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment responses. However, the field of circadian medicine has predominantly concentrated on human healthcare, neglecting its potential for transformative applications in veterinary medicine, thereby overlooking opportunities to improve non-human animal health and welfare. This review consists of three main sections. The first section focuses on the translational potential of circadian medicine into current industry practices of agricultural animals, with a particular emphasis on horses, broiler chickens, and laying hens. The second section delves into the potential applications of circadian medicine in small animal veterinary care, primarily focusing on our companion animals, namely dogs and cats. The final section explores emerging frontiers in circadian medicine, encompassing aquaculture, veterinary hospital care, and non-human animal welfare and concludes with the integration of One Health principles. In summary, circadian medicine represents a highly promising field of medicine that holds the potential to significantly enhance the clinical care and overall health of all animals, extending its impact beyond human healthcare.
Article
Full-text available
Sensorineural hearing loss (SNHL), caused by pathology in the cochlea, is the most common type of hearing loss in humans. It is generally irreversible with very few effective pharmacological treatments available to prevent the degenerative changes or minimise the impact. Part of this has been attributed to difficulty of translating “proof-of-concept” for novel treatments established in small animal models to human therapies. There is an increasing interest in the use of sheep as a large animal model. In this article, we review the small and large animal models used in pre-clinical hearing research such as mice, rats, chinchilla, guinea pig, rabbit, cat, monkey, dog, pig, and sheep to humans, and compare the physiology, inner ear anatomy, and some of their use as model systems for SNHL, including cochlear implantation surgeries. Sheep have similar cochlear anatomy, auditory threshold, neonatal auditory system development, adult and infant body size, and number of birth as humans. Based on these comparisons, we suggest that sheep are well-suited as a potential translational animal model that bridges the gap between rodent model research to the clinical use in humans. This is especially in areas looking at changes across the life-course or in specific areas of experimental investigation such as cochlear implantation and other surgical procedures, biomedical device development and age-related sensorineural hearing loss research. Combined use of small animals for research that require higher throughput and genetic modification and large animals for medical translation could greatly accelerate the overall translation of basic research in the field of auditory neuroscience from bench to clinic.
Article
Full-text available
We have investigated dogs’ (Canis familiaris) abilities in associating different sounds with appetitive stimuli of different incentive values. The association’s establishment was first tested on family dogs (n = 20) in a problem-solving behavioural paradigm (experiment 1), then in a problem-solving behavioural paradigm as well as an fMRI study on specially trained family dogs (n = 20) (experiment 2). The aim was to show behavioural and parallel neural effects of the association formed between the two sounds and two different associated appetitive stimuli. The latency of solving the problem was considered an indicator of the motivational state. In our first experiment, where only behaviour was studied, we found that dogs were quicker in solving a problem upon hearing the sound associated with food higher in reward value, suggesting that they have successfully associated the sounds with the corresponding food value. In our second experiment, this behaviour difference was not significant. In the fMRI study, the cerebral response to the two sounds was compared both before and after the associative training. Two bilateral regions of interest were explored: the caudate nucleus and the amygdala. After the associative training, the response in the caudate nucleus was higher to the sound related to a higher reward value food than to the sound related to a lower reward value food, which difference was not present before the associative training. We found an increase in the amygdala response to both sounds after the training. In a whole-brain representational similarity analysis, we found that cerebral patterns in the caudate nucleus to the two sounds were different only after the training. Moreover, we found a positive correlation between the dissimilarity index in the caudate nucleus for activation responses to the two sounds and the difference in latencies (i.e. high reward value associated sound condition latency—low reward value associated sound condition latency) to solve the behavioural task: the bigger the difference between the conditions in latency to solve the task, the greater the difference in the neural representation of the two sounds was. In summary, family dogs’ brain activation patterns reflected their expectations based on what they learned about the relationship between two sounds and their associated appetitive stimuli.
Article
Full-text available
Under comparable conditions, free-field behavioral auditory thresholds were obtained from 4 simian Ss at 16 frequencies and from 7 human Ss at 14 frequencies. Human thresholds were generally slightly lower through 4 kc., while above that the simian thresholds were lower. The rhesus monkey retains good sensitivity up to at least 31.5 kc. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Determined pure-tone absolute thresholds for 2 male owl monkeys using a tracking procedure. The owl monkey audibility curve obtained had best sensitivity at 8-10 kHz and +40-db sound pressure level (SPL) cutoffs at 125 Hz and 42 kHz. The high-frequency slope was very sharp, the +70-db SPL cutoff occurring at 46 kHz. A comparison of the nocturnal owl monkey with the closely related but diurnal squirrel monkey revealed that their audibility functions are remarkably similar. Comparison of audibility functions of nocturnal and diurnal primates suggests that there is no inevitable specialization of auditory sensitivity in nocturnal species.
Article
The audibility function of the squirrel monkey was measured with particular interest in determining the high‐frequency cutoff. The animals were trained to lick a tube to produce a tone burst. This response served to fix the animal&apos;s head in the sound field. A barpress within 3 sec of tone onset produced food and was counted as a detection. The squirrel monkey audibility function obtained had a low‐frequency slope of approximately 10 dB/octave, the typical notch at 4 kHz and maximum sensitivity in the 8–10‐kHz region. The high‐frequency side was complex, consisting of a decrease of sensitivity to 16 kHz, a flat portion from 16 to 32 kHz, and a sharp cutoff commencing between 32 and 40 kHz. The 70‐dB‐SPL point occurred at 46 kHz.
Article
A fresh water Amazon River dolphin, Inia geoffrensis, was conditioned to respond to pure tones by pushing a response lever. By using this method an audiogram was obtained for frequencies between 1.0 and 105 kHz. The animal was most sensitive to tones between 75 and 90 kHz. The sensitivity threshold in this area was −50 dB re 1 dyn/cm2. The effective upper limit of hearing was 105 kHz. No reliable response was elicited above 105 kHz using a random presentation and undistorted signal.
Article
Using standard operant conditioning techniques, a killer whale, Orcinus orca Linnaeus, was trained to respond to pure‐tone auditory signals by pushing a response manipulandum. An audiogram was obtained for frequencies between 500 Hz and 31 kHz. Greatest sensitivity to the signal was observed at 15 kHz at a level of −70±5 dB re 1 dyn/cm2. The observed upper limit of hearing was 32 kHz. At no time during training or testing did the animal respond to an undistorted signal above 32 kHz. Frequencies below 500 Hz were not tested, owing to high ambient tank noise levels.
Article
The effects of the type of reinforcer on auditory sensitivity and equal-loudness data were determined in the squirrel monkey. The monkeys, restrained and provided with earphones, were conditioned to depress and hold a bar down in the presence of a stimulus light and then to terminate the holding response after onset of a tone. In Experiment 1, the specified behavior sequence postponed electric shock; in Experiment 2, a food reinforcer was dependent on bar release during the tone. The shape of the auditory sensitivity function and the acuity level at each frequency were the same for the two procedures. The audible frequency range extended from below 0.125 kHz (lowest frequency used) to 46 kHz. Sensitivity was maximum at 8 kHz. Latency of bar release following tone onset served as the basic data for constructing a family of equal-loudness contours. The type of reinforcer appeared not to be a determinant of either the shape of individual loudness contours or the pattern of family of equal-loudness functions. At the lower sound-pressure levels, the equal-loudness contours closely paralleled the threshold curves. At more-intense levels, the contours tended to flatten and depend less on frequency.
Article
Mongolian gerbils were trained to respond to pure tones in a shock-avoidance task. The animals proved to be readily trainable and behaviorally stable. Thresholds were obtained by a method of limits and, in the case of one animal, by generating psychrometric functions. The gerbil responds to frequencies between 0.1 and 60.0 kHz. For pure tones between 1.0 and 16.0 kHz, the threshold of hearing averages 4.6 dB SPL. Between 1.0 and 0.1 kHz, sensitivity declines at a rate of 12 dB/octave. Between 16.0 and 40.0 kHz, threshold increases at a rate of 13 dB/octave, and above 40.0 kHz at a rate of 66 dB/octave. Thresholds derived from psychometric functions were essentially identical to those obtained by the method of limits. Hearing in this species is consistent with its size and anatomical characteristics. Subject Classification: [43]80.50; [43]65.50.
Article
Minimum audible field, underwater audiograms from 1 to 90 kHz were obtained for two ringed seals (Pusa hispida). The audiograms exhibited a uniform sensitivity, to within ± 7 dB, in the frequency range 1 to 45 kHz. Above 45 kHz the threshold increased at a rate of 60 dB per octave. The lowest threshold was −32 dB relative to 1 μbar (68 dB re 1 μPa) at 16 kHz. The audiograms were similar to those of other phocid seals so far studied.
Article
Middle ears (515) from 26 species of the rodent family Heteromyidae — genera Dipodomys, Microdipodops, Perognathus, and Liomys —were studied both grossly and histologically, for qualitative and quantitative comparisons. Middle ear modifications characteristic of each genus are qualitatively described. Quantitative comparisons are made among the 26 species in the study. Some correlations between middle ear size and other measurements are discussed. The middle ear is an acoustical transformer that for best efficiency must match the impedance of the cochlea to the impedance of the air in the external auditory meatus. It accomplishes this by a pressure increase and a velocity decrease through the combined effects of the lever and areal ratios; however, because the important consideration is a matching of two impedances rather than an absolute pressure increase, the pressure transformer ratio is a less informative measure of the middle ear's efficiency than is the impedance transform ratio. The impedance transformer mechanism is explained (from a morphological point of view), and equations are presented. Dipodomys, Microdipodops, and Perognathus have a theoretical transmission (at the resonant frequency) of 94–100% of the incident acoustical energy; Liomys, 78–80%. The areal ratio of stapes footplate to 2/3 tympanic membrane is remarkably constant among the species, varying only from 0.04 to 0.07: in Dipodomys and Microdipodops this small ratio is due to the very large tympanic membrane; in Perognathus and Liomys it is due to the extremely small stapes footplate. The lever ratio of incus to malleus varies from 0.28 to 0.33 in Dipodomys and Microdipodops, from 0.37 to 0.46 in Perognathus, and from 0.55 to 0.60 in Liomys. In addition, the middle ear volumes and the morphology of tympanic membrane, ossicles, ligaments, and muscles, all combine to minimize both mass and stiffness. All these data suggest middle ear mechanisms which are very efficient over a broad frequency range. The middle ear modifications found in heteromyids are adaptive in predator avoidance, especially in areas of little natural cover; nevertheless, contrary to expectations, there is no firm relationship between habitat and the extent of these modifications in the 26 species. However, environment did apparently play an important role in the evolution of the family, and this is discussed.
Article
A free-field underwater audiogram from 0.76 to 100 kHz was obtained for Pagophilus groenlandicus. Areas of increased sensitivity occurred at 2 and 22.9 kHz. The lowest threshold was −32.9 db/μbar at 15.0 kHz. Above 64 kHz the threshold increases at a rate of 40 db/octave. The audiogram was similar to that of the Phoca vitulina. The effects of ambient noise on the audiogram are discussed.