Content uploaded by Jose Luis Graupera
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jose Luis Graupera on Mar 09, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
International Journal of Applied Sport Sciences (2003), vol 15, 1, 22-35
The assessment of motor coordination in children with the Movement
ABC test: A comparative study among Japan, USA and Spain
Luis Miguel Ruiz
j
Faculty of Sport Sciences
Castilla La Mancha University. (Spain)
José Luis Graupera
Faculty of Sport Sciences
Alcalá de Henares University. (Spain)
Melchor Gutiérrez
Faculty of Psychology
University of Valencia. (Spain)
Motohide Miyahara
School of Physical Education
University of Otago (New Zeland)
Abstract
The Movement Assessment Battery for Children (Movement ABC; Henderson and Sudgen,
1992) is one of the most popular instrument in the assessment of children with movement
coordination problems. It is generally assumed that the published norms for the test are valid for
the use with European children and one of the aims of this study was to compare the results of
Miyahara’s study with Japanese children (53 boys, 49 girls) and the American standardization
(237 boys, 284 girls) with the Spanish sample (202 boys and 183 girls).
The cross-cultural comparisons revealed that there are many differences in performance among
children of these samples. These differences were distributed among tasks and countries in the
two age bands. Gender differences in all samples shown that girls outperform boys in manual
and balance tasks, and boys got better scores in ball skills
This data and its analysis so far suggest different consequences: 1) The question of cultural
differences in motor skill learning and performance; 2) The problem of gender differences in
motor coordination; 3)The norms of the test. As a final consequence it will be necessary to study
this test in a larger and more broadly based sample of Spanish boys and girls for being accepted
as a useful test in the assessment of motor coordination in Spain.
Key words: Movement ABC; Motor Coordination, Assessment, Cross-cultural research.
j
Correspondence author: Faculty of Sport Sciences. Avda. Carlos III s/n. 45071 Toledo (Spain)
2
1. Introduction
Throughout the European Union interest in children with coordination disorders is continuously
growing. Within the school population, it is estimated that approximately 5% to 8% of the
children fail to develop sufficient motor competence to allow normal progress in school in
general and particularly in physical education (Betts & Underwood, 1992; Morris & Whiting,
1971; Sudgen & Wright, 1998).
Assessment of this kind of problems has been a challenge during the last decades, and many
instruments have been developed (see Burton and Miller, 1998).
During the past three decades many scholars in several nations, including Australia, Canada,
Germany, Finland, France, The Netherlands, Sweden, UK, U.S.A., etc., have recognized
children with coordination problems and the difficulties of the definition of this condition and its
assessment and screening by teachers (Cantel, Smyth y Ahonen, 1994; Causgrive y Watkinson,
1994; Cratty, 1994; Degoute, 1979; Flem Maeland, 1992; Geuze and Börger, 1993; Henderson,
Elliman, Knight and Jongmans, 1991; Henderson, Knight, Losse and Jongmans, 1991; Kiphard,
1976; Larkin and Hoare, 1991; Miyahara, 1994; Miyahara and Mobs, 1995; Miyahara et al.,
1998; Rosblad and Gard, 1998; Smits-Englesman, Henderson and Michels, 1998; WHO, 1992).
In these countries research has been conducted to investigate the cause, the prevalence and the
management of motor coordination problems in school settings . In some of these nations,
educational placement and remedial procedures have been established to meet the needs of these
children (Cratty, 1994; Betts y Underwood, 1992; Morris & Whiting, 1971; Gordon and
Mckinlay, 1980; Miyahara, 1996; Whitehall and Underwood, 1991; Cantell, Huovinen,
Männistö, Larkin & Kooistra, 2002).
This was not the case in Spain. During many years there were no interest in children’s motor
coordination problems. The study of Ruiz, Graupera, Gutiérrez, and Mayoral (1997) can be
considered one of the first efforts in this direction. Although the Spanish educational instructions
emphasizes curricular adaptations to attend to special educational needs, there is a dearth of
research and documents in the areas of learning disabilities and motor coordination problems. In
attempt to accumulate data in these areas a comprehensive study is planned to investigate the
motor coordination problems among Spanish school children.
3
Our specific aim in this study were twofold. The first objective was to compare the results of the
Spanish study with Miyahara´s study and the norms of the ABC from the US standardization.
Our second objective was to examine the gender differences between the age-band 2 and 3 (7-8
years; 9-10 years) (Ruiz, Graupera and Miyahara, 2000).
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Three hundred and eighty five children (202 boys and 183 girls) from several urban and rural
Spanish primary schools at Madrid and Valencia participated in the Spanish study. Every
child in these schools between ages of 7 to 9 took part. This research formed part of a more
extensive study about developmental coordination problems and learned helplessness among
Spanish school children (Ruiz, Graupera, Gutiérrez and Mayoral, 1997).
Their results were compared with to the one hundred and two Japanese children (53 boys, 49
girls) and five hundred and twenty one American children (237 boys, 284 girls) with respect
to 8 subtests of the Movement ABC. The number and gender of Spanish children in each age
group are displayed in table 1, together with the comparable information from the Japanese
study and American standardization.
INSERT TABLE 1 HERE
All children of the Spanish sample followed the National Curriculum prescribed by the
Spanish Educational Reform by the Ministry of Education, Culture and Sports, including the
physical education curriculum that maintains the general objectives in each course but permits
a free application between schools.
2.2. The Movement ABC test
The Movement ABC test is designed for use with children aged 4-12+ years. A total of 32
items are divided into four sets of eight, each intended for use with children of specific ages.
The first set of items, labeled Age Band 1 is designed for use with 4-6 year old children, the
second set, Age Band 2 for 7 and 8 years old children, the third for 9 to 10 years old and the
fourth for children 11 years old and older. The test is identical in its structure in each age
band. Three items involve the use of the hands, two items require the children to catch or
throw a bean bag or small ball and three items assess static and dynamic balance. In Table 2
we present the characteristics of test items from age band 2 and age band 3, these age bands
are the object of this article.
4
INSERT TABLE 2 HERE
2.3. Procedures
For each child of the Spanish sample, every task of each age band were administered and
scored by a group of 10 physical education students in their last year of career, using the
criteria described in the Movement ABC Manual. All the participating testers previously had
received training by LMR and JLG in the use of the test in a seminar about motor
coordination problems.
3. Results
3.1. Qualitative observations
The children of the Spanish sample responded very positively to the different tasks contained
in the test. There were no problems in the administration of the test.
A child’s performance on the test can be scored in several ways. Raw scores, such as the
number of seconds taken to complete a task, the number of catches made, etc (see table 2). All
testing took place in the gymnasium or psychomotor education room of the children’s school.
For the present study our primary interest was to compare the results of the Spanish children
(age band 2 and 3) with the results of Miyahara’s study and the American standardization.
3.2. Comparisons among the three countries
The means and standard deviations of the raw scores were calculated for the Spanish sample
and compared with those of the Japanese study and American standardization, task by task,
for each age and gender group in age band 2 and 3 (these analysis were made separately
because each tasks are different in each age band). This data are displayed in Tables 3-4.
INSERT TABLES 3 - 4 HERE
The performance of Japanese, American and Spanish children on each task were analyzed
using the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). The eight tasks were considered as
dependent variables and age, gender and country as a factors. Multivariate tests of
significance were used to test the effects of each factor and all possible interactions with
MABC items in conjunction. These effects were evaluated through univariate F-tests of
significance on each dependent variable. Post-hoc multiple comparisons tests (Bonferroni
criteria) analysis were employed as appropriate trying to establish the differences among
countries (three groups). The fiduciary limit of p< .05 was set for results to be regarded as
significant.
5
3.2.1. Age Band 2 (7-8 years)
MANOVA analysis and multivariate tests of significance (Wilks lambda and approx. F) sums
up the results in this age band. Table 5 shows four significant differences (p<.001) of the three
factors and an interaction between age and country. No other significant differences were
obtained. Differences between samples for each dependent variable are presented below.
INSERT TABLE 5 HERE
Manual dexterity
On the three manual dexterity items there were significant differences. The Japanese sample
got better scores in relation to the American and Spanish on the first task (MABC1: Placing
pegs). American and Japanese children were quicker than the Spanish performing the second
task (MABC2: Threading lace) and the Spanish and the American performed with less error
when they made the third task (MABC3: Flower trail) (Table 8). There were no differences in
terms of gender in these three tasks (Table 6).
When age is considered, children between 7 and 8 years old (see Table 6) improved their
performance in MABC1 and MABC 3. No differences were obtained in MABC2.
It is interesting to talk about the interactions got between age and country. As table 7 shows
there are a significant effect in these three tasks . MABC1 shows a general tendency to
improve between 7 and 8 years; this improvement is more clear in the Spanish sample,
moderate in the American sample and minimal in the Japanese children. MABC2 task
manifest a cross effect because performance is very similar in American children, better in
Japanese at eight years of age and better in the Spanish children at seven years.
INSERT TABLE 6-7-8 HERE
Ball skills
On the two ball skill items, there were no differences among countries. If we consider the
gender, in this case we found differences, boys got better scores than girls in this two tasks. In
we consider the age of children we found a significant improvement in the two tasks
(MABC4: One-hand bounce and catch and MABC5: Throw bean bag in box) at seven and
eight years (Table 6). There is a significant interaction between country and age in task
MABC4. From seven to eight years there is an improvement in the Japanese and American
children but no in the Spanish (Table 7).
6
Static and dynamic balance
On the three items measuring static balance (Stork balance) and dynamic balance (jump into
squares and heel-toe walking), there were three comparisons that reached significant
differences among Japanese and Spanish children, and among American and Spanish in task
six. American and Japanese sample got better scores that the Spanish.
In relation to gender, only one measure reached significant difference. Girls outperform boys
in this task (MABC8: Heel-to-to walking). One tasks, MABC6 (Stork balance) reached
significant differences in relation to age, and children at eight got better scores.
When we consider the interaction of age and country we see that there is only one significant
difference in MABC8 (Heel-to-toe walking). If American children performed better than the
other groups at 8 years and the Spanish manifested a similar tendency, Japanese got their
better scores at 7 years of age.
3.2.1. Age Band 3(9-10 years)
MANOVA analysis and multivariate tests of significance (Wilks lambda and approx. F) sums
up the results in this age band. Table 9 shows significant differences (p<.01) of the three
factors and two interaction between age and country, and gender and country. No other
significant differences were obtained. Differences between samples for each dependent
variable (MABC items) are presented below.
INSERT TABLE 9 HERE
Manual dexterity
On the three manual dexterity items there were significant differences among the three
samples (Table 10). Japanese and American children reached significant differences (better
scores) performing MABC1 (shifting pegs in rows) task than Spanish children. MABC2
(threading nuts on bolt) differentiate among groups, American were better than Japanese and
Spanish, and Japanese better than the Spanish.
Differences between American and Spanish children on task MABC3 (Flower trial) were
minimal although significant, and they performed better than Japanese children (Table 12).
Gender differences were significant when we compare data on task MABC3 (Flower trail)
and in general girls outperform boys (Table 10).
Multivariate analysis found significant interactions in terms of age and country (Table 11).
MABC1 showed a tendency to improve between 9 and 10 years, this difference was more
clear in the Spanish sample than in the other two groups of children. MABC2 showed a cross
effect because performance threading nuts in bolt were better in the American and Spanish
7
children at 10 years, but this betterment were in the Japanese sample at 9 years old.
If we consider the interaction between gender and country, two comparisons reached
statistical significance: MABC1 and MABC2 . There are no differences between boys and
girls in the American sample, but Spanish girls are better than boys, and Japanese boys are
better than girls shifting pegs into rows. Doing the flower trail American and Spanish got
similar results but Japanese girls outperform boys (Table 11).
INSERT TABLES 10-11-12 HERE
Ball skills
On task MABC4 (two-hand catching) significant differences were between the Japanese
sample and the American, and between the American sample an the Spanish. American
children performed better that the Japanese, and the Spanish better than the American (Table
12). No significant differences were found among countries.
In terms of gender, boys outperform girls doing MABC4 (two-hand catch) and MABC5
(throw bean bag in box) (Table 10). No significant differences were found between the two
ages.
There were significant interaction between age and country in the two tasks and a cross effect
on MABC4 task. Japanese and Americans reached better scores at 10 years but the Spanish
children got them at 9 years of age. Japanese and Spanish were better at 9 years on MABC4
task and the American were better at 10 years.
Another significant interaction were the relationship between gender and country. Japanese
and American boys outperform girls catching balls (MABC4), this differences are minimal in
the Spanish sample. Something similar occurs throwing a bean bag to a box (MABC5).
Static and dynamic balance
On these three tasks there were significant differences in terms of country (Table 10).
Japanese and American children got better scores in task MABC6 (one-board balance) and
MABC7 (Hopping in squares) than the Spanish children, but in task 8 (ball balance) the
significant differences were in favor of the Spanish sample, that got better results than the
Japanese and American samples (Table 12).
Only gender by country interaction reached statistical significance in task MABC6 (one-board
balance). Girls performed better that boys among Japanese and American children but worst
in the Spanish sample.
8
4. Discussion
In this paper, we have presented data on how seven to ten years-old Spanish children
perform on the Movement ABC test when compared to the Japanese and the American
children. Different European studies have suggested that MABC norms are satisfactory
(Smits-Engelsman et al., 1998; Rösblad and Grad, 1998) but one of the practical usefulness of
this study is that it suggests the norms provided in the manual are not valid for Spanish
children, and it is necessary to develop new norms in this country.
As in Miyahara’s study the ethnic homogeneity of the Spanish sample it was not a
major issue, as the educational experience. In Spain every child practice no less than two
hours of regular physical education in a coeducational setting. Out of many comparisons
made among Japanese, American and Spanish children, country, gender and age differences
reached significance.
When we examined this differences and its characteristics item by item different
patterns emerged. When Japanese, American and Spanish children are compared in manual
dexterity tasks emerged a interesting cultural and children’s understanding difference. If we
analyzed this data, American and Spanish children reached better scores in the Flower trail
task than the Japanese, and the Spanish sample got worst results on task MABC1 and
MABC2. This situation make us think about how children in different countries and cultures
understand the objective of these tasks. All of these dexterity task demands a compromise
between speed and accuracy. Success performing MABC1 and MABC2 demands speed,
MABC3 demands accuracy. Cause Japanese and American are systematically better in speed
tasks and Spanish children in the accuracy task, make us think about the different
understanding that children manifest when they have to do these tasks. Miyahara et al (1998)
explained differences in terms of a systematic biases of testers or in terms of the nature of
writing systems in the two cultures when they observed the differences between the Japanese
and the American children in his study.
We think that probably is a problem of speed versus accuracy, and it seems as if
Japanese and American understand that doing quickly is better and get better results
performing MABC1 and MABC2. Spanish children understand that doing well and with less
errors it is better, and this produces longer times shifting pegs in a row and threading nuts on
a bolt but they manifest more precision doing the flower trail. Here we found a problem of
culture when we try to adopt tests developing in other cultural contexts.
9
Different studies made since 1930 to 1980, found many gender differences in primary
school children. Girls outperformed boys in agility, static and dynamic balance, manual
dexterity and hoping. Boys reached better scores in ball skills, vertical and horizontal jumps
and speed in running (see Zaichkowsky, Zaichkowsky y Martinek, 1980; Ruiz, 1987). This
test explore some of these abilities and skills, and these data in our three samples shows a
different pattern of change. In our analysis we found that balance differences have changed
because we found that the American and Japanese children got better results doing the static
balance item, while Spanish children were better doing the dynamic balance item of the test.
Cultural influences in motor performance is clear when we consider the interaction
gender by country on static balance (MABC6 task – age - band 3). It is necessary to
remember that girls outperform boys in the Japanese and American sample, but between
Spanish children boys got better results than girls.
Catching and throwing balls performance are different between boys and girls, but it is
interesting to note that gender differences in the two age bands are clear between American
and Japanese, but if we consider Spanish data, this differences only emerge in age band 3,
and specifically on MABC4 and MABC5 . Is it possible to talk about the influence of
coeducation in the Spanish school system?. During the last twenty physical educators in
Spain had made an strong effort in developing equal opportunities for boys and girls in
primary physical education classes., but social influences produces that boys and girls do
different activities outside school as they get older. We can speculate that the kind of games
that girls play in Spain can be the cause of this data. It is very common that girls play games
of skipping and jumping skills more than boys, they prefer games of catching and throwing
skills. It is difficult to explain these differences but the ecological niche is more important
than we can think, in Spain is very usual to see girls hoping and skipping a great variety of
games, and boys playing soccer or basketball. These different activities form the background
of experiences that govern the direction of motor development and made very difficult
establish the real differences in motor performance, probably it would be better to talk about
similarities between boys and girls in motor coordination (Thomas and French, 1985). The
present study indicates that the cultural differences affect motor performance.
We can conclude that this study permits to see the differences among three countries
and three cultures and three continents. These differences are difficult of explaining with this
test, and if we apply the test norms, probably the percentage of Spanish children failing in the
15% were enormous .
10
The Movement ABC is a test that has some problems in its psychometric properties.
Burton and Miller (1998) consider that this test suffers "the weaknesses inherent in motor
ability tests as well as from insufficient evidence of reliability and validity" (pp.177). This is a
problem when we try to compare different studies made in different contexts with different
ways of life and understanding of how to do motor tasks. Tan, Parker and Larkin (2001)
comparing the MABC test with the McCarron Assessment of Neuromuscular Development
(MAND) found that the last one were the more accurate discriminator of motor impairment.
In sum, it will be necessary to study this test in a larger and more broadly based sample
of Spanish boys and girls for being accepted as a useful test in the assessment of motor
coordination in Spain. It seems necessary to develop new investigations and to establish new
norms for Spanish children.
11
References
Betts, M. y Underwood, G.L. (1992) The experience of three low motor ability pupils in
infant physical education. The Bulletin of Physical Education, 28, 3, 45-56
Burton, A.W. and Miller, D.E. (1998) Movement skill assessment. Champaign: Human
Kinetics
Cantell, M.H., Smyth, M.M. y Ahonen, T.P. (1994) Clumsiness in adolescence: Educational,
motor, and social outcomes of motor delay detected at 5 years. Adapted Physical Activity
Quarterly, 11, 114 -129
Cantell, M., Huovinen, T., Männistö, J.-P., Larkin D. & Kooistra, L. (2002) Intervention for
children with developmental coordination disorder. Helsinki: Finnish Educational Ministry and
LIKES Research Center
Cratty, B.J. (1994) Clumsy child syndromes. USA: Harwood Academic Publ.
Causgrove, J. y Watkinson, E.J. (1994) A study of the relationships between physical
awkwardness and children’s perceptions of physical competence. Adapted Physical Activity
Quarterly, 11, 275-283
Degoutte, A. (1979) Contribution a l’etude de la maladresse. Education Physique et Sport, 156,
26-31
Flem Maeland, A. (1992) Identification of children with motor coordination problems.
Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 9, 330-342
Geuze, R. y Börger, H. (1993) Children who are clumsy: Five years later. Adapted Physical
Activity Quarterly, 10, 10-21
Gordon, N. y McKinlay, I. (1980) Helping clumsy child. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingston.
Henderson, S., Knigth, E., Losse, A. y Jongmans, M. (1991) The clumsy child in the school: Are
we doing enougth?. British Journal of Physical Education, 9, 2-9
Henderson, S., Elliman, D., Knight, E. y Jongmans, M. (1991) Clumsiness in children-Do they
grow of it?. A 10 year follow up study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 55-
68
Henderson,S. E. and Sugden, D. (1992). Movement Assessment Battery for Children.
London: Psychological Corporation.
12
Kiphard, E. (1976) Movement disorders and coordination problems in primary school children.
Buenos Aires: Kapelutz. (in Spanish)
Larkin, D. and Hoare, D. (1991) Out of step. Neadlands: The University of Western Australia.
Active Life Foundation
Miyahara, M. (1994) Subtypes of students with learning disabilities based upon gross motor
functions. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 11, 368-381
Miyahara, M. (1996) A meta-analysis of intervention studies on children with developmental
coordination disorders. Corpus, Psyche et Societas, vol.3, 1, 11-18
Miyahara, M. y Möbs, I. (1995) Developmental dyspraxia and developmental coordination
disorder. Neuropsychology Review, 5, 4, 245-268
Miyahara, M., Tsujii, M. Hanai, T., Jongmans, M., Barnett, A., Henderson, S., Hori, M.,
Nakanishi , K. and Kageyama, H. (1996) Sex differences in motor coordination among prymary
school children in Japan and U.S.A. Paper presented at the 10
th
International Conference of the
ISCPES: Cultural Diversity and Congruence in Physical Education and Sport. (pp.181-184)
Miyahara, M., Tsujii, M. Hanai, T., Jongmans, M., Barnett, A., Henderson, S., Hori, M.,
Nakanishi , K. and Kageyama, H. (1998) The Movement Assessment Battery for Children: A
preliminary investigation of its usefulness in Japan. Human Movement Sciences, 17, 679-697
Morris, P.R. and Whiting, H.T.A. (1971) Motor impairment and compensatory education.
London: G. Bell & Sons
Rosblad, B. and Gard, L. (1998) The assessment of children with developmental coordination
disorders in Sweden: A preliminary investigation of the suitability of the Movement ABC.
Human Movement Sciences, 17, 711-719
Ruiz, L.M. (1987) Motor development and physical activities. Madrid: Gymnos, 3
rd
Ed. (in
Spanish)
Ruiz, L.M. (1997) Motor coordination problems and learning difficulties in physical education.
In. N. Garcia (Dir.) Instruction, learning and difficulties. Barcelona: Ed. LU (pp. 397-410) (in
Spanish)
Ruiz, L.M., Graupera, J.L., Gutiérrez, M. and Miyahara, M. (1996) General motor competence:
A comparative study of coordination between Spain, Japan and North America. In J.P.F. Garcia
and M.M. Garcia (Eds.) I Spanish Congress of Sport Sciences Association. Vol. II. Cáceres:
Faculty of Sport Sciences. University of Extremadura. Spain, (pp. 137-140) (in Spanish)
13
Ruiz, L.M., Graupera, J.L., Gutiérrez, M. and Mayoral, A. (1997) Developmental coordination’s
problems and learned helplessness in Spanish school children. Madrid: CIDE, Ministry of
Education and Culture. (in spanish)
Ruiz, L.M., Navarro, F., Graupera, J.L., Linaza, J.L. and Gutierrez, M. (2000) Development,
Motor Behavior and Sport. Madrid: Síntesis (in Spanish)
Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M., Henderson, S.E. and C.G.J. Michels (1998) The assessment of
children with developmental coordination disorders in the Netherlands: The relationships
between the Movement Assessment Battery for Children and the Köperkoordinations Test für
Kinder. Human Movement Sciences, 17, 699-709
Sudgen, D. and Wright, H. (1998) Motor coordination disorders in children. London: Sage
Publi.
Tan, S.K., Parker, H.E. and Larkin, D. (2001) Concurrent validity of motor tests used to identify
children with motor impairment. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18 (2), 168-182
Thomas, J, R. y French, K.E. (1985) Gender differences across age in motor performance: A
Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, vol. 98, 2, 260-282
Whitehall, C. y Underwood, G.L. (1991) A case study of the behavior of pupils of high and low
motor ability in primary school game lessons. The Bulletin of Physical Education, 27, 3, 24-33
World Health Organization (1992) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral
Disorders. Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World Health
Organization.
Zaichkowsky, L, Zaichkowsky, L. y Martinek, T. (1980) Growth and development: The
child and the physical activity. St. Louis: Mosby Co.
14
Table 1: Number of children per age for the Japanese, American and Spanish samples
(age band 2 and age band 3)
Male Female
Age Japan USA Spain Total Japan USA Spain Total
Total
7 12 64 43 119 13 60 39 112 231
8 16 62 51 129 12 78 44 134 263
Total age band 2 28 126 94 248 25 138 83 246 494
9 7 49 39 95 10 81 43 134 229
10 18 62 69 149 14 65 57 136 285
Total age band 3 25 111 108 244 24 146 100 270 514
Total 53 237 202 492 49 284 183 516 1008
14
Table 2
Test items from age band 2 and age band 3of the Movement ABC
DOMAIN ITEM DESCRIPTION
MEASURES
Age Band 2 (7-8 years)
Manual dexterity
MABC1. Placing pegs Place 12 pegs in holes of board (preferred and non preferred hand) Time (seconds)
MABC2 . Threading lace Thread lace through holes in board Time (seconds)
MABC3. Flower trail Trace with a pen 2 curved lines without crossing the lines Number of errors
Ball skills
MABC4. One-catch bounce and catch Bounce a tennis ball on the floor and catch it with the same hand (preferred and
non preferred hand)
Number of
catches
MABC5. Throw bean bag in box Throw bean bag into box with one hand from 2 m Number of hits
Static/dynamic balance
MABC6. Stork Balance Stand on one foot with sole of other foot against side supporting knee for as long
as possible ((preferred and non preferred leg)
Time (seconds)
MABC7. Jump into squares Make 5 continuous jumps (feet together) from square to square Number of jumps
MABC8. Heel-to-toe walking Walk along 4,5 m line, placing heel of one foot against toe of other Number of steps
Age Band 3 (9 –10 years)
Manual dexterity
MABC1. Shifting pegs into rows Move each of 3 rows of pegs up one row Time (seconds)
MABC2. Threading nuts on bolt Screw 3 nuts down a bolt Time (seconds)
MABC3. Flower trail Trace with a pen between 2 curved lines without crossing the lines Number of errors
Ball skills
MABC4. Two-hand catch Throw a ball at wall from 2 m and catch with both hands Number of
catches
MABC5. Throw bean bag in box Throw bean bag into box with one hand from 2,5 m Number of hits
Static/dynamic balance
MABC6. One-board balance Balance on one foot on a balance board Time (seconds)
MABC7. Hopping in squares Make 5 continuous jumps on one foot from square to square Number of jumps
MABC8. Ball balance Walk around stands (2,7 m apart) balancing ball on a board Number of steps
1
Table 3: Mean scores and standard deviation (italics) (age band 2) of the Japanese,
American and Spanish samples.
Male Female
Age Item Japan USA Spain Total Japan USA Spain Total
7
MABC 1
20.19 25.16 31.04 26.78 21.14 23.52 33.30 26.65
5.17 5.56 7.37 6.17 5.04 4.20 8.86 5.92
MABC 2
20.19 19.30 22.27 20.46 21.83 16.92 21.89 19.22
5.17 7.02 6.27 6.56 5.69 4.32 8.63 5.98
MABC 3
4.92 1.84 .62 1.71 5.08 1.44 .33 1.48
6.36 2.15 1.11 2.20 4.57 1.53 .66 1.58
MABC 4
8.96 8.66 9.04 8.83 8.38 8.09 8.26 8.18
.75 1.46 1.45 1.38 1.54 1.59 1.89 1.69
MABC 5
5.17 6.36 6.32 6.23 4.92 5.72 4.76 5.29
1.95 1.91 2.23 2.03 2.47 2.02 2.18 2.13
MABC 6
17.63 14.85 17.50 16.09 18.19 16.60 16.55 16.77
2.95 5.47 4.27 4.78 2.50 4.91 5.06 4.68
MABC 7
4.83 4.81 5.20 4.95 5.00 4.92 4.76 4.87
.53 .71 .83 .74 .00 .65 2.18 1.11
MABC 8
14.75 12.25 13.35 12.90 15.00 13.38 14.51 13.96
.87 4.23 3.14 3.50 .00 2.90 2.16 2.31
8
MABC 1
20.21 22.19 26.34 23.59 19.21 21.42 25.52 22.57
2.63 3.72 6.69 4.76 2.34 4.24 6.59 4.84
MABC 2
18.61 17.85 24.69 20.65 18.26 16.62 25.46 19.67
4.62 5.55 8.44 6.58 3.27 6.03 16.00 9.06
MABC 3
1.94 .62 .74 .83 1.00 .07 .48 .28
2.43 1.13 1.23 1.33 1.13 1.34 .94 1.19
MABC 4
9.31 9.29 8.71 9.06 9.46 8.79 8.38 8.72
.85 1.10 1.97 1.41 .45 1.33 1.77 1.40
MABC 5
7.38 7.27 6.60 7.02 5.67 5.78 5.60 5.71
1.63 2.03 2.27 2.08 1.78 2.05 2.41 2.14
MABC 6
17.72 17.85 17.82 17.82 18.71 17.36 18.13 17.73
4.04 4.31 7.69 5.61 2.62 4.36 3.67 3.98
MABC 7
4.94 4.7
1
4.92 4.82 5.00 4.91 5.60 5.14
.25 .93 1.21 .96 .00 .59 2.41 1.13
MABC 8
13.65 14.24 13.55 13.89 15.00 14.40 14.77 14.58
3.37 2.15 2.69 2.51 .00 1.72 2.96 1.97
2
Table 4: Mean scores and standard deviation (italics) (age band 3) of the Japanese, American and
Spanish samples.
Male Female
Age Item Japan USA Spain Total Japan USA Spain Total
9
MABC 1
11.30 13.34 19.93 15.90 14.70 13.29 18.50 15.07
5.61 2.22 4.41 3.37 3.58 1.60 4.32 2.62
MABC 2
20.55 15.19 19.25 17.25 16.27 17.42 21.71 18.71
10.10 6.46 11.22 8.68 7.56 5.99 11.93 8.01
MABC 3
2.88 .49 .61 .72 1.90 .64 .59 .72
2.10 1.02 .96 1.07 1.73 .91 1.12 1.04
MABC 4
6.43 8.90 8.00 8.35 5.40 6.28 9.30 7.18
3.55 1.61 3.32 2.45 2.91 2.38 .44 1.80
MABC 5
6.43 6.76 6.81 6.76 6.30 4.95 6.09 5.42
2.82 1.09 2.38 1.75 1.42 2.09 2.28 2.10
MABC 6
11.40 10.40 9.03 9.91 13.93 12.90 7.10 11.12
4.03 5.66 6.29 5.80 6.23 5.65 6.13 5.85
MABC 7
5.00 4.68 4.12 4.47 5.00 4.69 4.65 4.70
.00 .97 1.52 1.12 .00 1.02 1.12 .98
MABC 8
.00 .35 2.94 1.39 .10 .22 2.02 .79
.00 1.25 5.79 3.02 .32 .76 4.93 2.07
10
MABC 1
13.02 12.64 17.28 14.83 12.93 12.61 16.87 14.43
2.38 1.57 3.97 2.78 2.24 1.33 5.01 2.97
MABC 2
21.44 12.98 15.11 14.99 19.95 15.28 14.08 15.26
4.52 5.00 7.54 6.12 5.96 4.88 7.51 6.09
MABC 3
2.88 .56 1.25 1.16 1.21 .52 1.33 .93
2.10 .97 2.06 1.61 1.53 .90 2.48 1.63
MABC 4
7.79 8.61 5.85 7.23 6.43 7.63 6.06 6.85
2.23 2.42 3.05 2.69 2.82 2.20 3.01 2.60
MABC 5
6.58 6.66 5.41 6.07 5.07 6.00 5.52 5.70
1.64 1.82 1.93 1.85 1.49 1.93 1.88 1.86
MABC 6
12.05 12.46 7.32 10.03 13.85 14.38 7.19 11.31
6.66 5.68 4.82 5.40 6.80 5.41 4.91 5.34
MABC 7
4.97 4.67 4.78 4.76 4.96 4.88 4.80 4.85
.11 .94 1.15 .94 .13 .58 1.03 .72
MABC 8
.00 .16 1.63 .82 .00 .03 1.47 .63
.00 .79 3.99 2.18 .00 .17 3.84 1.69
1
Table 5. Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Multivariate tests of significance
MABC-Age band 2..
Effect
Wilks
lambda
Approx.
F
Hypoth.
DF
Error DF
Sig. of
F
Age .87 9.22
8
475
.000
Gender .90 6.51
8
475
.000
Country .49 25.07
16
950
.000
Age by Gender .97 1.56
8
475
.135
Age by Country .81 6.48
16
950
.000
Gender by Country .98 .71
16
950
.786
Table 6 : Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Univariate F-tests of significance.
MABC-Age band 2.
Factor effect
Country Gender Age
Item F
(2, 482)
Sig of F F
(1, 502)
Sig of F F
(1, 502)
Sig of F
MABC 1
93.45 .000 .08 .772 30.54 .000
MABC 2
40.21
.000 .18 .673 .04 .842
MABC 3
35.14
.000 2.70 .101 45.66 .000
MABC 4
2.05
.130 8.15 .004 7.78 .005
MABC 5
3.12
.045 22.29 .000 12.85 .000
MABC 6
3.37
.035 .55 .459 4.59 .033
MABC 7
4.3
2
.014 1.37 .243 .70 .402
MABC 8
5.99
.003 12.74 .000 2.58 .109
Table 7 : Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Interaction effects, univariate F-tests of significance.
MABC-Age band 2.
Age by Gender Age by Country Gender by
Country
Item F
(1, 482)
Sig of F F
(2, 482)
Sig of F
F
(2, 482)
Sig of F
MABC 1
1.40 .238 8.69
.000
1.83 .161
MABC 2
.00 .946 5.66
.004
1.50 .224
MABC 3
.77 .380 17.34
.000
.12 .883
MABC 4
1.87 .172 4.68
.010
.35 .707
MABC 5
1.54 .215 1.27
.281
.18 .836
MABC 6
.03 .851 1.06
.348
.72 .487
MABC 7
2.71 .100 1.52
.220
.02 .980
MABC 8
.02 .898 7.41
.001
.82 .439
2
Table 8 : Multiple comparisons by country (Bonferroni, * Significance level .050).
MABC-Age band 2.
Item
Country -
Country
Mean
differences
Std Error Sig.
MABC 1 Japan USA -2.770
*
.840 .003
Spain -8.600
*
.874 .000
Spain USA 5.830
*
.542 .000
MABC 2 Japan USA 2.050 1.114 .199
Spain -4.000
*
1.159 .002
Spain USA 6.050
*
.719 .000
MABC 3 Japan USA 2.230
*
.258 .000
Spain 2.610
*
.269 .000
Spain USA -.380 .167 .069
MABC 4 Japan USA .320 .225 .465
Spain .430 .234 .199
Spain USA -.110 .145 1.000
MABC 5 Japan USA -.370 .316 .726
Spain .010 .329 1.000
Spain USA -.380 .204 .189
MABC 6 Japan USA 1.350 .722 .187
Spain 2.500
*
.752 .003
Spain USA -1.150
*
.466 .042
MABC 7 Japan USA .100 .170 1.000
Spain -.105 .177 1.000
Spain USA .205 .110 .186
MABC 8 Japan USA .930 .392 .054
Spain .520 .407 .607
Spain USA .410 .253 .316
3
Table 9 : Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Multivariate tests of significance
MABC-Age band 3..
Effect
Wilks
lambda
Approx.
F
Hypoth.
DF
Error DF
Sig. of
F
Age
.96
2.58
8
495
.009
Gender
.95
3.44
8
495 .001
Country
.405
35.73
16
990
.000
Age by Gender
.995
.43
8
495 .900
Age by Country
.77
8.74
16
990
.000
Gender by Country
.87
4.60
16
990
.000
Table 10 :. Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Interaction effects, univariate F-tests of significance.
MABC-Age band 3
Factor effect
Country Gender Age
Item F
(2, 502)
Sig of F F
(1, 502)
Sig of F F
(1, 502)
Sig of F
MABC 1
187.56
.000
.45
.505
7.53
.006
MABC 2
10.22
.000
.00
.970
5.17
.023
MABC 3
27.89
.000
6.36
.012
.42
.515
MABC 4
7.11
.001
6.82
.009
1.28
.259
MABC 5
.30
.741
11.98
.001
2.37
.124
MABC 6
45.7
9
.000
2.79
.095
.39
.534
MABC 7
3.64
.027
1.32
.250
1.94
.164
MABC 8
42.54
.000
.63
.428
2.24
.135
Table 11 : Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Univariate F-tests of significance.
MABC-Age band 3.
Age by Gender Age by Country Gender by
Country
Item F
(1, 502)
Sig. of
F
F
(2, 502)
Sig. of F
F
(2, 502)
Sig. of F
MABC 1
1.39
.240
4.44
.012
3.82
.023
MABC 2
.015
.901
7.44
.001
2.62
.074
MABC 3
.63
.428
4.82
.008
4.93
.008
MABC 4
.02
.898
28.86
.000
15.98
.000
MABC 5
.19
.660
8.32
.000
3.30
.038
MABC 6
.01
.903
2.96
.053
4.95
.007
MABC 7
.23
.628
2.02
.133
.65
.523
MABC 8
.18
.673
1.81
.164
.62
.537
4
Table 12 : Multiple comparisons by country (Bonferroni, * Significance level .050).
MABC-Age band 3.
Item
Country -
Country
Mean
differences
Std Error Sig.
MABC 1 Japan USA .12 .338 1.000
Spain -4.83
*
.347 .000
Spain USA 4.95
*
.209 .000
MABC 2 Japan USA 4.45
*
.780 .000
Spain 2.86
*
.802 .001
Spain USA 1.59
*
.484 .003
MABC 3 Japan USA 1.64
*
.153 .000
Spain 1.18
*
.157 .000
Spain USA .46
*
.095 .000
MABC 4 Japan USA -.96
*
.259 .001
Spain -.30 .267 .783
Spain USA -.66
*
.161 .000
MABC 5 Japan USA .10 .205 1.000
Spain .23 .211 .828
Spain USA -.13 .127 .923
MABC 6 Japan USA .17 .605 1.000
Spain 5.30
*
.622 .000
Spain USA -5.13
*
.376 .000
MABC 7 Japan USA .25 .105 .051
Spain .35
*
.108 .004
Spain USA -.10 .065 .372
MABC 8 Japan USA -.16 .305 1.000
Spain -1.89
*
.313 .000
Spain USA 1.73
*
.189 .000