ArticlePDF Available
Framing: Toward Clarification
of
a
Fractured Paradigm
by Robert
M.
Entman, Northwestern University
In response
to
the proposition that communication lacks disciplinary sta-
tus because
of
deficient core knowledge,
I
propose that we turn an osten-
sible weakness into a strength. We should identify our mission as bring-
ing together insights and theories that would otherwise remain scattered
in other disciplines. Because
of
the lack
of
interchange among the disci-
plines, hypotheses thoroughly discredited in one field may receive wide
acceptance in another. Potential research paradigms remain fractured,
with pieces here and there but no comprehensive statement
to
guide re-
search. By bringing ideas together in one location, communication can
aspire
to
become a master discipline that synthesizes related theories and
concepts and exposes them to the most rigorous, comprehensive state-
ment and exploration. Reaching this goal would require a more self-con-
scious determination by communication scholars
to
plumb other fields
and feed back their studies
to
outside researchers.
At
the same time, such
an enterprise would enhance the theoretical rigor
of
communication
scholarship proper.
The idea
of
“framing” offers a case study
of
just the kind
of
scattered
conceptualization
I
have identified. Despite its omnipresence across the
social sciences and humanities, nowhere is there a general statement
of
framing theory that shows exactly how frames become embedded within
and make themselves manifest in a text,
or
how framing influences think-
ing. Analysis
of
this concept suggests how the discipline
of
communica-
tion might contribute something unique: synthesizing a key concept’s dis-
parate uses, showing how they invariably involve communication, and
constructing a coherent theory from them.
Whatever its specific
use,
the concept
of
framing consistently offers a
way
to
describe the power
of
a communicating text. Analysis
of
frames
il-
luminates the precise way in which influence over a human conscious-
ness is exerted by the transfer (or communication)
of
information from
Robert
M.
Entman
is
an associate professor
of
communication studies, journalism, and
po-
litical science and chair
of
the program in Communications, Media, and Public Policy at the
Center for Urban Affairs and Policy Research at Northwestern University, Evanston,
IL.
He
gratefully acknowledges the comments
of
students in his “Mass Communication and Demo-
cratic Theory” seminar, especially Andrew Rojecki.
Copyright
0
1993
Journal
of
Communication
43(4),
Autumn.
0021-9916/93/$5.00
Journal
of
Communzcatzon,
Antumn
199.3
one location-such as a speech, utterance, news report, or novel-to that
consciousness.
(A
representative list
of
classic and recent citations would
include: Edelman, 1993; Entman
&
Rojecki, 1993; Fiske
&
Taylor, 1991;
Gamson, 1992; Goffman, 1974; Graber,
1988;
Iyengar,
1991;
Kahneman
&
Tversky, 1984; Pan
&
Kosicki, 1993; Riker,
1986;
Snow
&
Benford,
1988;
Tuchman, 1978; White, 1987; Zaller,
1992.)
A
literature review suggests
that framing is often defined casually, with much left
to
an assumed tacit
understanding
of
reader and researcher. After all, the words
frame, fram-
ing,
and
.framework
are common outside of formal scholarly discourse,
and their connotation there is roughly the same. The goal here is
to
iden-
tify and make explicit common tendencies among the various uses
of
the
terms and
to
suggest a more precise and universal understanding
of
them.
Of
Frames
and
Framing
Framing essentially involves
selection
and
salience.
To frame is
to
select
some aspects
of
aperceived reality and make them more salient in a com-
municating text, in such a way as to promote aparticularproblem defini-
tion, causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recom-
mendation
for the item described. Typically frames diagnose, evaluate,
and prescribe, a point explored most thoroughly by Gamson (1992). An
example is the “cold war” frame that dominated
U.S.
news
of
foreign af-
fairs until recently. The cold war frame highlighted certain foreign
events-say, civil wars-as problems, identified their source (communist
rebels), offered moral judgments (atheistic aggression), and commended
particular solutions
(U.S.
support
for
the other side).
Frames, then,
dejineproblems-determine
what a causal agent is doing
with what costs and benefits, usually measured in terms
of
common cul-
tural values;
diagnose
causes-identify the forces creating the problem;
make moraljudgments-evaluate
causal agents and their effects; and
suggest remedies-offer
and justify treatments
for
the problems and pre-
dict their likely effects.
A
single sentence may perform more than one
of
these four framing functions, although many sentences in a text may per-
form none
of
them. And a frame in any particular text may not necessarily
include all four functions.
The cold war example also suggests that frames have at least four loca-
tions in the communication process: the communicator, the text, the re-
ceiver, and the culture.
Communicators
make conscious or unconscious
framing judgments in deciding what to say, guided by frames (often
called schemata) that organize their belief systems. The
text
contains
frames, which are manifested
by
the presence
or
absence
of
certain key-
words, stock phrases, stereotyped images, sources
of
information, and
sentences that provide thematically reinforcing clusters
of
facts
or
judg-
ments. The frames that guide the
receiver’s
thinking and conclusion may
or may not reflect the frames in the text and the framing intention
of
the
52
communicator. The
culture
is the stock
of
commonly invoked frames; in
fact, culture might be defined as the empirically demonstrable set
of
com-
mon frames exhibited in the discourse and thinking
of
most people in a
social grouping. Framing in a11 four locations includes similar functions:
selection and highlighting, and use
of
the highlighted elements
to
con-
struct an argument about problems and their causation, evaluation,
and/or solution.
How
Frames
Work
Frames highlight some bits
of
information about an item that is the sub-
ject
of
a communication, thereby elevating them in salience. The word
salience
itself needs
to
be defined:
It
means making
:i
piece
of
informa-
tion more noticeable, meaningful, or memorable
to
audiences. An in-
crease in salience enhances the probability that receivers will perceive
the information, discern meaning and thus process it, and store
it
in meni-
ory (see Fiske
&
Taylor,
1991).
Texts can make bits
of
information more salient by placement
or
repeti-
tion, or by associating them with culturally familiar symbols. However,
even a single nnillustrated appearance
of
a notion in an obscure part
of
the text can
be
highly salient,
if
it
comports with the existing schemata in
a receiver’s
belief
systems.
By
the same token, an idea emphasized in a
text can be difficult for receivers
to
notice, interpret,
or
remember
be-
cause
of
their existing schemata. For our purposes, schemata and closely
related concepts such as categories, scripts, or stereotypes connote men-
tally stored clusters
of
ideas that guide individuals’ processing
of
informa-
tion (see, e.g., Graber, 19881. Because salience is a product
of
the interac-
tion
of
texts and receivers, the presence
of
frames in the text, as detected
by researchers, does not guarantee their influence in audience thinking
(Entman, 1989; Graber, 1988).
Kahneman and Tversky
(1984)
offer perhaps the most widely cited re-
cent example
of
the power
of
framing and the way
it
operates by select-
ing and highlighting some features
of
reality while omitting others. The
authors asked experimental subjects the following:
Imagine thal the
1J.S.
is
preparing
for
the outbreak
of
an unusual Asian
disease, which is expected to kill
600people.
Two alternative programs
to combat the disease have beenproposed. Assume that the exact scien-
tz$c
estimates
of
the consequences qftheprograms are as,follows:
If
Pro-
gram
A
is adopted, 200people will
be
saved. Ifprogram
B
is adopted,
there is a one-thirdprobability that
GOOpeople
will be saved and a two-
thirdsprohahility that no people
will
he
saved. Which
of
the twopro-
grams wouldyou favor?(1984,
p.
243)
In this experiment,
72
percent
of
subjects chose Program
A;
28
percent
53
Toward
ClarzJicatzon
ofa
Fractured
Paradigm
,/ournal
of
Communication,
Autumn
199.3
chose Program B. In the next experiment,
identical options
to
treating the
same described situation were offered, but framed in terms
of
likely
deaths rather than likely lives saved:
“If
Program
C
is adopted,
400
people
will die.
If
Program
D
is adopted, there is a one-third probability that no-
body will die and a two-thirds probability that
600
people will die”
(Kahneman
&
Tversky,
1984,
p. 343). The percentages choosing the op-
tions were reversed by the framing. Program
C
was chosen by
22
percent,
though its twin Program
A
was selected by
72
percent; and Program
D
garnered
78
percent, while the identical Program B received only
28
per-
cent.
As
this example vividly illustrates, the frame determines whether most
people notice and how they understand and remember a problem, as
well as how they evaluate and choose
to
act upon
it.
The notion
of
fram-
ing thus implies that the frame has a common effect on large portions
of
the receiving audience, though
it
is not likely
to
have a universal effect
on
all.
Kahneman and Tversky’s experiments demonstrate that frames select
and call attention
to
particular aspects
of
the reality described, which log-
ically means that frames simultaneously direct attention away from other
aspects. Most frames are defined by what they omit as well as include,
and the omissions
of
potential problem definitions, explanations, evalua-
tions, and recommendations may be as critical as the inclusions in guid-
ing the audience.
tive description and omission
of
the features
of
a situation:
Edelman highlights the way frames exert their power through the selec-
The character, causes, and consequences of any phenomenon become
radically different as changes are made in what is prominently dis-
played, what is repressed and especially in how observations are classi-
jied.
.
.
.
(Uhe social world is
.
. .
a kaleidoscope ofpotential realities,
any
of
which can be readily evoked by altering the ways in which obser-
vations are framed and categorized.
(1993,
p.
232)
Receivers’ responses are clearly affected
if
they perceive and process in-
formation about one interpretation and possess little or incommensurable
data about alternatives. This
is
why exclusion
of
interpretations by frames
is as significant
to
outcomes as inclusion.
Sniderman, Brody, and Tetlock (1991) provide
a
clear instance
of
the
power
of
presence and absence in framing:
The effect of framing is toprime values differentially, establishing the
salience of the one
or
the other. (Thus].
. .
a majority of thepublic sup-
ports the rights ofpersons with
AIDS
when the issue is framed (in
a
sur-
vey question] to accentuate civil liberties considerations-and supports
. . .
mandatory testing when the issue is framed to accentuatepublic
health considerations.
(p.
52)
54
Toumrd
Clanlfication
of
u
Fractured
Puradigm
The text
of
the survey question supplies most people with the considera-
tions they use when they respond
to
the issue
of
AIDS testing (Zaller,
1992).
Often a potential counterframing
of
the subject is mostly or wholly
absent from a text, although,
to
use this instance, an audience member
with a strong civil liberties philosophy might reject mandatory testing
even
if
the poll framed
AIDS
strictly in public health terms.
Frames in Political News
This portrait
of
framing has important implications
for
political communi-
cation. Frames call attention
to
some aspects
of
reality while obscuring
other elements, which might lead audiences to have different reactions.
Politicians seeking support are thus compelled
to
compete with each
other and with journalists over news frames (Entman,
1989;
Riker,
1986).
Framing in this light plays a major role in the exertion
of
political power,
and the frame in a news text is really the imprint
of
power-it registers
the identity
of
actors or interests that competed
to
dominate the text.
many news texts exhibit homogeneous framing at one level
of
analysis,
yet competing frames at another. Thus, in the pre-war debate over
U.S.
policy toward Iraq, there was a tacit consensus among
U.S.
elites not to
argue for such options as negotiation between Iraq and Kuwait. The news
frame included only two remedies, war now or sanctions now with war
(likely) later, while problem definitions, causal analyses, and moral evalu-
ations were homogeneous. Between the selected remedies, however,
framing was contested by elites, and news coverage offered different sets
of
facts and evaluations. The Iraq example reveals that the power
of
news
frames can be self-reinforcing. During the pre-war debate, any critique
transcending the remedies inside the frame (war
soon
versus more time
for sanctions) breached the bounds
of
acceptable discourse, hence was
unlikely
to
influence policy. By conventional journalistic standards, such
views were not newsworthy (Entman
&
Page, in press). Unpublicized, the
views could gain few adherents and generate little perceived or actual ef-
fect on public opinion, which meant elites
felt
no pressure
to
expand the
frame
so
it
included other treatments for Iraqi aggression, such as negoti-
ation. Relatedly, Gamson
(1992)
observes that a frame can exert great
so-
cial power when encoded in a term like
ajfirmatiue action.
Once a term
is widely accepted,
to
use another is
to
risk that target audiences will per-
ceive the communicator as lacking credibility-or
will
even fail
to
under-
stand what the communicator is talking about. Thus the power
of
a frame
can be as great as that
of
language itself.
Reflecting the play
of
power and boundaries
of
discourse over an issue,
Benefits
of
a
Consistent Concept
of
Framing
An understanding
of
frames helps illuminate many empirical and norma-
tive controversies, most importantly because the concept
of
framing di-
55
Journal
of
Communication,
Autumn
1993
rects our attention
to
the details
of
just how a communicated text exerts
its power. The example
of
mass communication explored here suggests
how a common understanding might help constitute framing as a
re-
search paradigm.
A
research paradigm is defined here as a general theory
that informs most scholarship on the operation and outcomes
of
any par-
ticular system
of
thought and action. The framing paradigm could be ap-
plied with similar benefits
to
the study
of
public opinion and voting be-
havior in political science;
to
cognitive studies in social psychology; or
to
class, gender, and race research in cultural studies and sociology,
to
name
a few. Here are some illustrations
of
theoretical debates in the study
of
mass communication that would benefit from an explicit and common
understanding
of
the concept
of
frames.
definition for the notion
of
dominant meaning
that is
so
central
to
de-
bates about polysemy and audience independence in decoding media
texts (Fiske, 1987). From a framing perspective, dominant meaning con-
sists
of
the problem, causal, evaluative, and treatment interpretations with
the highest probability
of
being noticed, processed, and accepted by the
most people.
To
identify a meaning as dominant or preferred is
to
suggest
a
particular framing
of
the situation that is most heavily supported by the
text and is congruent with the most common audience schemata.
A
framing paradigm cautions researchers not
to
take fugitive compo-
nents
of
the message and show how they
might
be interpreted in ways
that oppose the dominant meaning.
If
the text frame emphasizes in a vari-
ety
of
mutually reinforcing ways that the glass is half full, the evidence
of
social science suggests that relatively few in the audience will conclude
it
is half empty.
To
argue that the polysemic properties
of
the message con-
duce
to
such counterframing, researchers must show that real-world audi-
ences reframe the message, and that this reframing is not a by-product
of
the research conditions-for example, a focus group discussion in which
one participant can lead the rest, or a highly suggestive interview proto-
col (Budd, Entman,
&
Steinman, 1990).
plicitly in the text, or retrieve from memory a causal explanation or cure
that is completely absent from the text. In essence, this is just what pro-
fessors encourage their students
to
do habitually. But Zaller (19921, Kah-
neman and Tversky (19841, and Iyengar (19911, among others, suggest
that on most matters
of
social or political interest, people are not general-
ly
so
well-informed and cognitively active, and that framing therefore
heavily influences their responses
to
communications, although Gamson
(1992) describes conditions that can mitigate this influence.
2.
Journalistic objectivity.
Journalists may follow the rules for “objec-
tive” reporting and yet convey a dominant framing
of
the news text that
prevents most audience members from making a balanced assessment
of
a
situation. Now, because they lack
a
common understanding of framing,
journalists frequently allow the most skillful media manipulators
to
im-
1.
Audience autonomy.
The concept
of
framing provides an operational
Certainly people can recall their own facts, forge linkages not made ex-
56
Toward ClariJication
of
a Fractured Paradigm
pose their dominant frames on the news (Entman,
1989;
Entman
&
Page,
in press; Entman
&
Rojecki, 1993).
If
educated
to
understand the differ-
ence between including scattered oppositional facts and challenging a
dominant frame, journalists might be better equipped
to
construct news
that makes equally salient-equally accessible to the average, inattentive,
and marginally informed audience-two or more interpretations
of
prob-
lems. This task would require a far more active and sophisticated role for
reporters than they now take, resulting in more balanced reporting than
what the formulaic norm
of
objectivity produces (Tuchman, 1978).
3.
Content analysis.
The major task
of
determining textual meaning
should be
to
identify and describe frames; content analysis informed by a
theory
of
framing would avoid treating all negative or positive terms or
utterances as equally salient and influential. Often, coders simply
tote
up
all messages they judge as positive and negative and draw conclusions
about the dominant meanings. They neglect
to
measure the salience
of
el-
ements in the text, and fail to gauge the relationships
of
the most salient
clusters
of
messages-the frames-to the audience’s schemata. Unguided
by a framing paradigm, content analysis may often yield data that misrep-
resent the media messages that most audience members are actually pick-
ing up.
4.
Public opinion and normative democratic theory.
In Zaller’s (1992)
account, framing appears
to
be a central power in the democratic
process, for political elites control the framing
of
issues. These frames can
determine just what “public opinion” is-a different frame, according
to
Zaller, and survey evidence and even voting can indicate a different pub-
lic opinion. His theory, along with that
of
Kahneman and Tversky, seems
to
raise radical doubts about democracy itself.
If
by shaping frames elites
can determine the major manifestations
of
“true” public opinion that are
available
to
government (via polls or voting), what can true public opin-
ion be? How can even sincere democratic representatives respond cor-
rectly to public opinion when empirical evidence
of
it
appears
to
be
so
malleable,
so
vulnerable
to
framing effects?
Say there are three ways
to
frame an issue and one generates 40 per-
cent approval, the others
50
percent and
60
percent, respectively.
Ap-
proving the option
with
60 percent support is
not
axiomatically the
most
democratic response because
of
the cyclical majority problem (Riker,
19861, which makes majority rule among several complex options mathe-
matically impossible. Just as important, attempting
to
determine which
of
the differently framed opinions is the closest
to
the public’s “real” senti-
ments appears futile, because
it
would require agreement among con-
tending elites and citizens on which frame was most accurate, fair, com-
plete, and
so
forth.
A
framing paradigm can illuminate,
if
not solve, such
central puzzles in normative democratic theory.
Indeed, the concept
of
framing is important enough in the many fields
of
inquiry that use
it
to
merit a book-length essay. The present effort, con-
strained by space limitations, offers not the definitive word on frames but
57
Journal
of
Communication, Autumn
1993
a preliminary contribution. Equally important, this article exemplifies
how the field
of
communication might develop from its wide ambit and
eclectic approaches a core
of
knowledge that could translate into re-
search paradigms contributing
to
social theory in the largest sense.
References
Budd,
M.,
Entman,
R.
M.,
&
Steinman, C. (1990). The affirmative character
of
U.S. cultural
studies.
Critical Studies in Mass Communication,
7,
169-184.
Edelman,
M.
J.
(1993). Contestable categories and public opinion.
Political Communica-
Entman,
R.
M.
(1989).
Democracy without citizens: Media and the decay ofAmericanpoli-
Entman,
R.
M.,
&
Page,
B.
I.
(in press). The news before the storm: The Iraq war debate
tion,
10(3), 231-242.
tics.
New York: Oxford University Press.
and the limits to media independence. In
W.
L.
Bennett
&
D.
L.
Paletz (Eds.),
Just deserts;
7'be
news
media,
U.S.
foreignpolicy, and the Gulf War.
Chicago: University
of
Chicago
Press.
Entman,
R.
M.,
&
Rojecki, A. (1993). Freezing out the public: Elite and media framing
of
the
Fiske,
J.
(1987).
Television culture.
New York: Routledge.
Fiske,
S.
T.,
&
Taylor,
S.
E.
(1991).
Social cognition.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gamson,
W.
(1992).
Talkingpolitics.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Goffman,
E.
(1974).
Frame analysis.
New York: Free Press
Graber, D.A.
(1988).
Processing the news: Howpeople
tame
the information tide
(2nd ed.).
Iyengar,
S.
(1991).
Is
anyone responsible?
Chicago: University
of
Chicago Press
Kahneman,
D.,
&
Tversky, A. (1984). Choice, values, and frames.
American Psychologist.
Pan,
Z.,
&
Kosicki,
G.
M.
(1993).
Framing analysis: An approach to news discourse.
Poliii-
Riker,
W.
H.
(1986).
The
art
ofpolitical manzpulation.
New Haven: Yale University Press
Sniderrnan, P.
M.,
Brody,
R.
A,,
&
Tetlock, P.
E.
(1991).
Reasoning and choice: Explorations
Snow, D. A,,
&
Benford,
R.
D.
(1988).
Ideology, frame resonance, and participation mobi-
U.S. anti-nuclear movement.
Political Communication,
10(2),
151-167.
New York: Longman.
39,
341-350.
cal Communication,
I0
(11,
55-76,
inpoliticalpsychology.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
lization.
International Social Movement Research,
1,
197-217.
Tuchman,
G.
(1978).
Making
news.
New York: Free Press.
White,
H.
(1987).
The content
of
the form.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Zaller, J.
R.
(1992).
7'be
nature and origins
of
mass opinion.
New York: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
58
... The way media outlets present and interpret the protests can significantly shape public opinion. Positive framing can legitimise the movement, while negative framing can delegitimise it (Entman, 1993). Digital platforms play a crucial role in mobilising support and disseminating information. ...
... In the case of the #EndBadGovernance protests, media coverage can increase the prominence of governance issues. Additionally, Entman (1993) argues that media framing involves emphasising particular aspects of reality to make them more noticeable. Positive media framing can legitimise the protests, whereas negative framing can undermine their credibility. ...
Article
Full-text available
The #EndBadGovernance protests significantly influenced public opinion and national security policies in Nigeria. Support for the movement was stronger among younger generations and urban slum residents, largely due to their greater use of social media and frustration with corruption and poor governance. Media coverage, both traditional and social, played a key role in shaping the narrative, with positive portrayals emphasising government accountability and transparency, thus legitimising the protests, while negative coverage stirred fears of instability. The protests' legitimacy was further reinforced by widespread public participation, which fuelled national discussions on governance reform. However, concerns about public safety led to varied government responses, ranging from dialogue to repression, which had a direct impact on public trust in institutions and state legitimacy. In response, national security policies evolved, incorporating increased surveillance, restrictions on public gatherings, and efforts to address underlying socioeconomic issues. The study, utilising diverse data sources such as mined tweets and articles, highlighted that the #EndBadGovernance movement has become a powerful tool for Nigerians advocating for improved governance. The protests are also connected to broader reform movements, particularly concerning national security. The findings suggest that addressing socioeconomic grievances and fostering transparent communication between the government and the media are essential to restoring public trust and stability. Introduction The #EndBadGovernance protests represent a significant social movement aimed at addressing widespread corruption, poor governance, and systemic inefficiencies within government institutions in Nigeria. These protests, which have gained considerable traction through both physical demonstrations and digital activism, highlight the growing public discontent with government practices and demand for greater transparency, accountability, and reform.
... 2. Khung lý thuyết 2.1. Lý thuyết Đóng khung (Framing Theory) Đóng khung (framing) được hiểu là chọn một số khía cạnh của thực tế đã được con người nhận thức và làm chúng nổi bật hơn trong sản phẩm truyền thông, bằng cách nhấn mạnh vào một vấn đề cụ thể, diễn giải nguyên nhân, đánh giá hoặc đưa ra các khuyến nghị (Entman, 1993). Hai vấn đề của lý thuyết đóng khung được các nhà nghiên cứu quan tâm là khung truyền thông (media frames) và khung cá nhân (individual frames). ...
... Hai vấn đề của lý thuyết đóng khung được các nhà nghiên cứu quan tâm là khung truyền thông (media frames) và khung cá nhân (individual frames). Khung truyền thông (media frames) là thông điệp mà chủ thể truyền thông lựa chọn để truyền tải, còn khung cá nhân (individual frames) là cách mà công chúng giải mã thông điệp (Entman, 1993). Cách giải mã thông điệp của mỗi cá nhân phụ thuộc vào nhiều yếu tố như địa chính trị, văn hóa, trình độ học vấn và sự hiểu biết. ...
Article
Full-text available
Nghiên cứu giới thiệu về lý thuyết báo chí phát triển (development journalism) và các đặc điểm báo chí phát triển tại các quốc gia châu Á - nơi mô hình báo chí này xuất hiện và được thực hành tại nhiều quốc gia. Từ góc tiếp cận này và áp dụng thêm lý thuyết đóng khung (framing theory), nghiên cứu thực hiện thống kê mô tả 543 tin, bài báo về Chương trình Mỗi xã một sản phẩm (OCOP) trên báo Nông thôn Ngày nay trong ba năm (2021, 2022 và 2023) để chỉ ra các khung truyền thông chính được tờ báo hàng đầu về nông nghiệp, nông dân và nông thôn tại Việt Nam sử dụng. Kết quả nghiên cứu cho thấy các khung truyền thông về Chương trình OCOP trên báo Nông thôn Ngày nay thực hiện nhiều chức năng quan trọng của báo chí, có nhiều điểm tương đồng, phản ánh các đặc điểm của báo chí phát triển, ở các mức độ khác nhau. Kết quả nghiên cứu mang tính tham khảo cho các nghiên cứu chuyên sâu về báo chí phát triển ở Việt Nam cũng như thực hành lựa chọn các khung truyền thông khi đưa tin về Chương trình OCOP của nhà báo.
... Dalam proses ini, framing menjadi salah satu strategi utama yang digunakan media untuk mengemas informasi sehingga menghasilkan persepsi tertentu pada khalayak (Goffman, 1974;Kusumaningsih, 2024). Framing tidak hanya memengaruhi cara individu memahami suatu isu, tetapi juga berkontribusi terhadap pembentukan wacana publik yang dapat berdampak luas pada respons sosial dan kebijakan terkait isu tersebut (Entman, 1993;Satrivi & Purnama, 2021). ...
... Dalam penelitian ini, peneliti mengklasifikasikan data berdasarkan tiga aspek utama, yaitu tema berita, penyebutan nama, dan pemilihan kata. Pendekatan ini sejalan dengan analisis framing sebagaimana dikemukakan oleh Entman (1993) yang menyoroti bagaimana media memilih, menyoroti, dan menyusun suatu informasi untuk membentuk persepsi khalayak. Selain itu, kajian Tuchman (1978) menegaskan bahwa framing dalam pemberitaan kasus kekerasan seksual sering kali mempengaruhi posisi dan perspektif terhadap pelaku serta korban. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study analyzes the framing in the news reporting of a sexual harassment case committed by Agus Buntung, a person with a disability. This is done using a descriptive qualitative methodology. The focus of this study is to explain the phenomenon of how the media presents the perpetrators, victims, and related institutions in the development process of this case. Using the Miles and Huberman model, data were collected and analyzed through the stages of data reduction, data presentation, and drawing conclusions. The results of the analysis show that the media uses themes that develop along with the increasing number of victims, depicting the perpetrators as smart and manipulative people even though they have physical limitations. In addition, the irony in the news is reinforced by calling the perpetrator's name with the term "Buntung", which increases public attention to the case. In the news, words such as "increasing victims" and "emotional manipulation" indicate the media's efforts to influence readers' emotions and emphasize the resolution of the case. This study shows how media framing can influence public perceptions of people with disabilities. It also shows how important legal protection and justice are for victims.
... This theory has been widely applied in various fields such as journalism, politics, marketing, public relations, and everyday conversations. In journalism and communication, for instance, framing theory is used to analyze how media organizations construct and present news events, and how these choices influence public perceptions and understandings of those events [5]. Please note that the citation for the application of framing theory in journalism is provided as an example and is not directly related to the original sources mentioned earlier. ...
Article
Full-text available
In modern educational systems, English news viewing and listening courses have emerged as essential elements, providing students with an immersive language learning experience that integrates media literacy and critical thinking, while fostering global perspectives and preparing them to be informed, responsible citizens in an interconnected world. Previous research underscores the effectiveness of English news teaching in enhancing language and cultural understanding, yet highlights a need for stronger integration of values education and more robust research to identify best practices aligning linguistic learning with personal and social development goals. Under the framework of the theory of "News Media and Values Shaping," this paper aims to solve the proposed questions through detailed case studies and aim to play emphasis on conveying positive, healthy, and uplifting values to guide university students in forming correct moral concepts and behavioral norms in English news audio-visual courses.
... These schemes foreground a particular point of emphasis-in our case, either the territoriality principle or regulators' territorial goals-that makes a certain set of interpretations salient (cf. Entman, 1993): using either geographic attributes or regulators' goals as evaluative features to circumscribe regulatory territory's scope. To conceptualize them into attribute-based vs. goalbased territorial categorization schemes, we drew on the cultural-cognitive perspective on categorization Grodal, 2018;Jepperson & Meyer, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Regulators increasingly engage in extraterritorial law enforcement, but its deterrence effects on organizations remain poorly understood. Based on a case study in the Swiss private wealth management industry, we explore the conditions under which U.S. extraterritorial law enforcement provoked cross-border general deterrence, preventing not only the prosecuted Swiss banks but also the unprosecuted ones (i.e., the observers of the events) from violating U.S. law when a peer was prosecuted for similar behavior. Our inductive analysis led us to integrate deterrence theory with a novel cultural–cognitive, organization-centered perspective on regulatory territory: the spatial scope of a regulator’s jurisdictional authority. This integration suggests that unprosecuted banks’ territorial self-categorization—a form of spatial self-categorization that leads them to conclude whether they are located inside or outside what they perceive to be a foreign regulator’s territory—is critical for explaining cross-border general deterrence. Our findings emphasize the significance of the clarity with which the U.S. regulator communicated its regulatory territory and how different types (which we call locals and cosmopolitans) of organizations’ taken-for-granted categorization schemes influenced their rational choices and instrumental actions. We contribute to scholarship on organizations’ legal environment and to organizational research on categories.
Article
Purpose This paper aims to look at the case of esports in Hong Kong by content analysis of news between 2020 and 2022 to explore how esports development was being framed in the news and discuss the implications of the media framing of esports in Hong Kong’s context. Design/methodology/approach A qualitative content analysis approach is adopted to study the news framing of esports in selected news articles between 2020 and 2022. Findings Findings of news framing of esports in Hong Kong suggested that esports were emphasized as an industry in Hong Kong’s context. Even though esports coverage emerged as the Asian Games approached, the absence of sporting elements in the news contributed to the consolidation of understanding of esports as discussed in previous studies, which hinders the social recognition of esports as a sport that could be further developed in the city. Originality/value To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study that focuses on the news framing of esports in Hong Kong, highlighting the important role of media in influencing the public perception of esports and its image in Hong Kong’s context.
Article
Full-text available
This paper studies how artificial intelligence was set to the agenda in the press and social media in France. By simultaneously analysing the framing of AI and the key actors who dominated the discourse on this technology in the national press and on the X and Facebook platforms, the study highlights, on the one hand, the influence of digital companies and government narratives, and on the other, the presence of alternative stakeholder perspectives that diverge from dominant discourses and contribute to political polarisation on AI-related issues such as facial recognition. Our study sheds light on how AI framing can highlight dominant and alternative narratives and visions and may contribute to the consolidation of socio-technical imaginaries in the French public sphere.
Article
During the 2022 FIFA World Cup, players on Iran's national team avoided singing the national anthem before their first match. This was interpreted as an expression of solidarity with the ongoing protests in Iran following the death of Mahsa Amini, a 22-year-old Iranian woman arrested for opposing the country's mandatory hijab requirements. This study focuses on how regular users and government propaganda social media accounts sought to shape the political and sporting narratives surrounding these protests. A total of 20,470 tweets and 33,535 frames composed the sample, revealing stark differences between anti-regime and pro-regime communities in nearly all frames, except for a frame combining football and international politics. The implications of protest framing and the use of social media as a form of resistance and reinterpretation are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
In the American political process, news discourse concerning public policy issues is carefully constructed. This occurs in part because both politicians and interest groups take an increasingly proactive approach to amplify their views of what an issue is about However, news media also play an active role in framing public policy issues. Thus, in this article, news discourse is conceived as a sociocognitive process involving all three players: sources, journalists, and audience members operating in the universe of shared culture and on the basis of socially defined roles. Framing analysis is presented as a constructivist approach to examine news discourse with the primary focus on conceptualizing news texts into empirically operationalizable dimensions—syntactical, script, thematic, and rhetorical structures—so that evidence of the news media's framing of issues in news texts may be gathered. This is considered an initial step toward analyzing the news discourse process as a whole. Finally, an extended empirical example is provided to illustrate the applications of this conceptual framework of news texts.
Article
Full-text available
This article argues that, as imported to the United States from England by scholars like Lawrence Grossberg and John Fiske, cultural studies often loses much of its critical edge. Its misleading affirmation of the power and independence of media audiences derives from several problems. First, it overestimates the freedom of audiences in reception. Second, it minimizes the commodification of audiences as analyzed by a political‐economic approach. Third, it fails to differentiate between mass advertising and specialized media. Fourth, it confuses active reception with political activity. Finally, it takes the exceptional situation of progressive readings promoted within oppositional subcultures as the norm.
Article
Full-text available
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
We discuss the cognitive and the psy- chophysical determinants of choice in risky and risk- less contexts. The psychophysics of value induce risk aversion in the domain of gains and risk seeking in the domain of losses. The psychophysics of chance induce overweighting of sure things and of improbable events, relative to events of moderate probability. De- cision problems can be described or framed in multiple ways that give rise to different preferences, contrary to the invariance criterion of rational choice. The pro- cess of mental accounting, in which people organize the outcomes of transactions, explains some anomalies of consumer behavior. In particular, the acceptability of an option can depend on whether a negative outcome is evaluated as a cost or as an uncompensated loss. The relation between decision values and experience values is discussed. Making decisions is like speaking prose—people do it all the time, knowingly or unknowingly. It is hardly surprising, then, that the topic of decision making is shared by many disciplines, from mathematics and statistics, through economics and political science, to sociology and psychology. The study of decisions ad- dresses both normative and descriptive questions. The normative analysis is concerned with the nature of rationality and the logic of decision making. The de- scriptive analysis, in contrast, is concerned with peo- ple's beliefs and preferences as they are, not as they should be. The tension between normative and de- scriptive considerations characterizes much of the study of judgment and choice. Analyses of decision making commonly distin- guish risky and riskless choices. The paradigmatic example of decision under risk is the acceptability of a gamble that yields monetary outcomes with specified probabilities. A typical riskless decision concerns the acceptability of a transaction in which a good or a service is exchanged for money or labor. In the first part of this article we present an analysis of the cog- nitive and psychophysical factors that determine the value of risky prospects. In the second part we extend this analysis to transactions and trades. Risky Choice Risky choices, such as whether or not to take an umbrella and whether or not to go to war, are made without advance knowledge of their consequences. Because the consequences of such actions depend on uncertain events such as the weather or the opponent's resolve, the choice of an act may be construed as the acceptance of a gamble that can yield various out- comes with different probabilities. It is therefore nat- ural that the study of decision making under risk has focused on choices between simple gambles with monetary outcomes and specified probabilities, in the hope that these simple problems will reveal basic at- titudes toward risk and value. We shall sketch an approach to risky choice that
Article
This is an analysis of the failure of the interaction between the news media and their audiences to create the democratic potential everyone assumes occurs with such interaction. Drawing illustrations mainly from the Carter and Reagan years, the book shows the dilemma facing the news media and their audience today. The book offers a portrait of citizenship in America, defined by the public's changing levels of political knowledge and participation from 1952 to 1984. Politically unsophisticated, the mass audience prefers simple, symbolic news, which means that journalists can offer little of the detached, detailed explorations of policy issues that would provide the public with the information needed to hold government to close account.