ArticlePDF Available

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

Authors:

Abstract

A single subject, a proficient lucid dreamer experienced with signaling the onset of lucidity (reflective consciousness of dreaming) by means of voluntary eye movements, spent 4 nonconsecutive nights in the sleep laboratory. The subject reported becoming lucid and signaling in 8 of the 18 rapid-eye movement (REM) periods recorded. Ten lucid dream reports were verified by polygraphic examination of signals, providing a total of 12.5 min of signal-verified lucid REM. H-Reflex amplitude was recorded every 5 s, along with continuous recording of electroencephalogram, electrooculogram, electromyogram, electrocardiogram, finger pulse, and respiration. Significant findings included greater mean H-reflex suppression during lucid REM sleep than during nonlucid REM and correlations of H-reflex suppression with increased eye movement density, heart rate, and respiration rate. These results support previous studies reporting that lucid REM is not, as might be supposed, a state closer to awakening than ordinary, or nonlucid, REM; rather, lucid dreaming occurs during unequivocal REM sleep and is characteristically associated with phasic REM activation.
Sleep
12(4):374-378, Raven Press, Ltd., New York
©
1989
Association
of
Professional Sleep Societies
Short Communication
H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic
Activation During Lucid REM Sleep:
A Case Study
* Andrew Brylowski, Lynne Levitan, and Stephen LaBerge
*University
of
Texas Medical School
at
Houston, Houston, Texas,
and
Sleep Research
Center, Stanford University School
of
Medicine, Stanford, California,
U.S.A.
Summary: A single subject, a proficient lucid dreamer experienced with sig-
naling the onset
of
lucidity (reflective consciousness
of
dreaming) by means
of
voluntary eye movements, spent 4 nonconsecutive nights
in
the sleep labora-
tory. The subject reported becoming lucid and signaling in 8
of
the
18
rapid-eye
movement (REM) periods recorded. Ten lucid dream reports were verified by
polygraphic examination
of
signals, providing a total
of
12.5 min
of
signal-
verified lucid REM. H-Reflex amplitude was recorded every 5 s, along with
continuous recording
of
electroencephalogram, electrooculogram, electromyo-
gram, electrocardiogram, finger pulse, and respiration. Significant findings in-
cluded greater mean H-reflex suppression during lucid REM sleep than during
nonlucid REM and correlations
of
H-reflex suppression with increased eye
movement density, heart rate, and respiration rate. These results support pre-
vious studies reporting that lucid REM is not, as might be supposed, a state
closer to awakening than ordinary, or nonlucid, REM; rather, lucid dreaming
occurs during unequivocal REM sleep and
is
characteristically associated with
phasic REM activation. Key Words:
H-Reflex-Lucid
REM sleep.
Electrophysiological studies have shown that
"lucid"
dreams, in which persons are
explicitly aware that they are dreaming while continuing to dream, occur almost ex-
clusively in rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep (1). Further investigations (2,3) have
indicated that lucid REM sleep
is
characterized by higher levels
of
phasic activity and
autonomic activation than ordinary REM sleep, and therefore that it is likely that
lucidity occurs during periods
of
relatively great cortical activation. Contrary to some
earlier suggestions that lucid dreaming might be the result
of
microawakenings, brief
intrusions
of
waking consciousness, in REM periods (4), the findings that lucidity
is
Accepted for publication September
1988.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr.
A.
Brylowski at Department
of
Psychiatry, South-
western Medical School at Dallas,
5323
Harry Hines Blvd., Dallas, TX 75235-9070, U.S.A.
374
H-REFLEX
SUPPRESSION
IN
LUCID
REM
375
associated with more intense, activated REM sleep suggest rather that lucid REM is a
deep sleep state and not nearer to waking than ordinary
REM
sleep.
The primary goal
of
this experiment was to investigate the status
of
H-reflex sup-
pression in lucid dreams. Given that both human (5,6) and feline (7,8) studies have
demonstrated tonic reflex inhibition during phasic
REM
and that lucidity
is
associated
with phasically active REM, it was predicted that the H-reflex during lucid
REM
would
be
at
least as suppressed as it is in ordinary REM.
In the H-reflex analysis, the polygraph records were divided into 5-s epochs instead
of
the standard 30-s epochs (9), enabling correspondingly finer resolution
of
the
state
changes during REM and especially lucid
REM
sleep.
It
was possible to identify mi-
croawakenings and transitions to non-REM (NREM) sleep if any occurred during the
periods
of
lucidity.
METHODS
The subject was a 25-year-old medical student,
an
experienced lucid dreamer who
was familiar with the method
of
signaling from lucid dreams by means
of
eye move-
ments verifiable
on
the polygraph record (1). His sleep was normal during an all-night
polysomnogram without continuous H-reflex monitoring. On 4 nonconsecutive nights
his H-reflex was measured as he slept with the intention to recognize when he was
dreaming and to signal the onset
of
lucidity.
The experimental protocol called for the subject to signal the onset
of
lucidity with
two pairs
of
extreme left-right eye movements and to signal awakening with four pairs
of
left-right eye movements. The subject wrote reports
of
mental activity after each
spontaneous awakening, including the times
of
awakening. Following previously de-
scribed methodology
(I),
reports were scored as signal-verified lucid dreams
if
(a)
the
subject reported that he had become lucid and made the eye movement signals and (b)
the corresponding section
of
the polygraph record showed the reported signals.
A Grass model 8-18D was used to record several channels
of
electroencephalogram,
induding
the
standard central positions used for sleep scoring, vertical and horizontal
electrooculogram, electrocardiogram, nasal airflow, finger pulse, and H-reflex ampli-
tude. Standard procedures for measuring H-reflex were used, modified for continuous
measurement (10,11). The subject's knee was immobilized in a knee immobilizer set
at
100
flexion and a O.I-ms
50-
V stimulus was delivered to the right posterior tibial nerve
every 5 s as the contraction
of
the soleus muscle was monitored. A
TECA
model TE42
electromyogram (EMG) machine provided the electrical stimulus and amplified the
EMG signal, which was sent to the J5 external input
on
the Grass polygraph.
The polygraph records were scored according to the standard criteria (9). The
REM
periods, including the
30
s pre and
post
REM, were subdivided into 5-s epochs, one
epoch for each H-reflex sample. These 5-s epochs were again scored for sleep stages
according to the standard criteria modified for 5-s epochs. An epoch was scored as
Stage 2 if no change in stage had occurred between K-complexes
or
sleep spindles
or
if
a K-complex
or
sleep spindle was present anywhere in the epoch. Also,
an
epoch was
scored as lucid REM only if it contained an eye movement signal
or
lay between epochs
containing signals corresponding with the subject's written report (see Fig.
1).
In addition to sleep staging, several other physiological measures were collected and
analyzed in 5-s epochs. These were H-reflex amplitude (millimeters
of
pen
deflection),
eye movement density (number
of
eye movements
per
epoch), heart rate (beats
per
Sleep, Vol.
12,
No.4,
1989
376
A.
BR YLOWSKI
ET
AL.
C4·A2
~~I~
I
V."""V~"'Ii.rl.~
...
r'~<'f\"\A~
Cz·A2
~I~~~l~~'\i'"
YJ!},,j~NJtA,,,,\~f,MwJ(I!~
.....
~/r'"h\f\.
T3·A1
,~
......
--
....
tv~/'
~,~----~
T4·A2
~V\f''''----V~t
01·A1
02·A2
LOC·A1
ROC·A2
F.
Pulse
EMG
EKG
H·Reflex
Resp
\ \ \
~
A.
Awake
B.
REM
Sleep
---==-----,150
!l
V
5
Sec
FIG. 1.
A:
Awake, eyes closed. Two sets
of
eye movement signals are shown.
B:
REM sleep. Two sets of
eye movement signals are shown, with the onset
of
a lucid dream at the first (arrow). Note the indications of
autonomic activation [skin potential artifact in the electroencephalogram and electrocardiogram (EKG) chan·
nels and finger pulse amplitude suppression] and the profound H-reflex suppression accompanying lucidity.
epoch converted for analysis to beats per minute), and respiration interval (measured
for the breath cycle occupying >50%
of
the
5-s
epoch and converted for analysis to
breaths per minute).
RESULTS
Eighteen REM periods were recorded; eight included portions
of
lucid REM with a
total of
10
lucid dreams. A total
of
150
epochs (12.5
min)
of
lucid REM were collected
across the 4 recording nights out of
2,470 REM epochs
(206
min). The number of epochs
oflucid REM for nights 1-4 was
18
(1.5
min),
18
(1.5
min),
48
(4
min), and
66
(5.5
min),
respectively. The percentage
of
total sleep time spent in REM from nights 1 through 4
was 5.4%
(19
min/349.5 min), 11.1%
(60
min/538.5 min), 18.4%
(64
min/348 min), and
29.4%
(63
min/214.5 min), respectively. The increasing percentages
of
REM are pre-
sumably due to the subject adapting to sleeping under the difficult experimental con-
ditions. Only the epochs within REM periods actually scored as REM were analyzed.
None
of
the portions
of
lucid REM were found to contain any microawakenings or
transitions to NREM.
The mean H-reflex amplitude was determined for the nonlucid epochs and lucid
epochs
of
each REM period that included lucid REM. In all eight cases, the mean
H-reflex amplitude for lucid REM was lower than that for nonlucid REM (binomial test,
Sleep, Vol. 12,
No.4,
1989
H-REFLEX
SUPPRESSION
IN
LUCID
REM
377
p = 0.(04). A one-tailed paired t test
of
the REM period means
of
lucid REM versus
nonlucid also revealed lucid REM H-reflex amplitude
to
be significantly lower than
nonlucid
(t(7)
= 4.43, p < 0.005).
As mentioned above, previous studies have shown that the initiation
Of
lucidity in
REM sleep
is
associated with autonomic activation (2,3). To examine the relationship
between the onset
of
lucidity and H-reflex suppression, the mean values
of
H-reflex
amplitude for the 30 s preceding and the 30 s following lucidity initiation were compared
with the REM period median H-reflex amplitude.
For
both pre- and postlucidity peri-
ods, 8
of
10
mean amplitudes were lower than the all-REM period median amplitudes
(binomial test, p = 0.05). The same type
of
comparison was performed
on
eye move-
ment density in the 30 s pre- and postlucidity initiation.
For
all
10
lucid dreams, the eye
movement density means were higher
just
before and after lucidity onset than the
all-REM period medians (binomial test, p = 0.001), bearing
'Out
the earlier findings on
this measure
of
autonomic arousal.
Direct comparisons
of
the relationship between H-reflex suppression and other au-
tonomic variables were done with Spearman rank correlations
of
H-reflex amplitude
with eye movement density, respiration rate, and heart rate. These tests showed highly
significant (p < 0.001) negative correlations between H-reflex amplitude and all other
physiological variables when the values from nights 1-4 were pooled.
For
the individual
nights,
11
of
12
coefficients showed significant correlations
(p
< 0.05) (see Table
O.
DISCUSSION
The results
of
the above comparisons
of
H-reflex amplitude during lucid and nonlucid
REM sleep clearly show not only that H-reflex is suppressed during REM lucid dreams,
as it is in ordinary REM sleep, but that it tends to be more suppressed during lucid than
nonlucid REM. Increased H-reflex suppression was also strongly associated with pha-
sic REM activity and autonomic activation, two factors that previous studies (2,3) have
shown to also be associated with lucid REM sleep.
The findings presented here demonstrate that lucid dreaming occurs during unam-
biguous REM sleep, without microawakenings or other arousals, and also that lucid
REM sleep
is
associated with relatively high phasic activity, autonomic activation, and
TABLE
1.
Correlations
of
H-reflex amplitude with eye movement density, heart rate, and
respiration rate for the 4 nights individually and pooled using Spearman's
correlation coefficient
Night 1
Night 2
Night 3
Night 4
All nights pooled
Q P < 0.001.
b P <
O.OS.
Eye movement density
-0.3539"
(n
=
192)
-0.4265a
(n =
981)
-0.5475a
(n
=
1,057)
-0.4725a
(n =
970)
-0.3239"
(n
= 3,200)
H-Reflex amplitude with
Heart rate
-0.0538
(n
=
192)
-O.3508a
(n =
980)
-O.0698b
(n
=
I,OS9)
-0.2434
a
(n
=
970)
-O.3198a
(n
= 3,201)
Respiration rate
-0.IS20
b
(n =
191)
-0.184SQ
(n =
980)
-0.3134Q
(n =
I,OS6)
-O.l164Q
(n
==
962)
-0.5119"
(n = 3,189)
Sleep, Vol.
12,
No.4,
1989
378
A.
BR
YLOWSKI
ET
AL.
deep reflex suppression. By showing that the H-reflex
is
even more suppressed during
lucid REM than nonlucid REM, these analyses suggest that lucid REM is not, as might
be supposed, a state closer to awakening than ordinary,
or
nonlucid REM. Addition-
ally, a direct relationship is suggested between the depth
of
H-reflex suppression and
increased phasic activation in REM sleep. Further research is indicated to confirm
these findings with more subjects and to investigate in more detail the relationship
between various modes
of
physiological activation, tonic reflex suppression, and the
mental activity
of
the dreamer.
Acknowledgment: The support
of
The Institute
of
Human Development
is
gratefully acknowl-
edged.
REFERENCES
1.
LaBerge SP, Nagel LE, Dement WC, Zarcone VP. Lucid dreaming verified by volitional communication
during REM sleep. Percept Mot Skills
1981
;52:727-32.
2.
LaBerge SP, Nagel
LE,
Taylor WB, Dement WC, Zarcone VP. Psychophysiological correlates
of
the
initiation
of
lucid dreaming. Sleep
Res
1981
;10: 149.
3. LaBerge S, Levitan L, Dement WC. Lucid dreaming: physiological correlates
of
consciousness during
REM sleep. J Mind Behav 1986;7:251-8.
4.
Schwartz BA, Lefebvre A. Contact veille/P.M.O. II. Les P.M.O. morcelles.
Rev
Electroencephalogr
Neurophysiol Clin 1973;3: 165-76.
5. Hishikawa Y, Sumitsugu N, Matsumoto K, Kaneko
Z.
H-reflex and EMG
of
the mental and hyoid
muscles during sleep, with special reference to narcolepsy. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
1965;18:487-92.
6. Hodes R, Dement WC. Depression of electrically induced reflexes ("H-reflexes") in man during low
voltage EEG sleep. Electroencephala/?r Clin NeurophysioI1964;17:617-29.
7.
Pivik R, Metz J. Phasic EMG inhibition and spinal reflex modulation during synchronized sleep in the
cat. Exp Neural 1975;48:493-501.
8. Pivik R, Metz J. Spinal and lateral geniculate nucleus activity during sleep: quantitative relationships.
Exp NeuroI1982;77:142-62.
9.
Rechtschaffen
A,
Kales A, eds. A manual
of
standardized terminology, techniques,
and
scoring system
for sleep stages
of
human subjects. Los Angeles: University
of
California
at
Los Angeles, Brain Infor-
mation Service/Brain Research Institute, 1968.
10.
Magladery J, McDougal
G.
Electrophysiologic studies
of
nerve and reflex activity in normal man.
I.
Identification of certain reflexes
in
the electromyogram and the conduction
of
peripheral nerve fibers.
Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp 1950;86:265-90.
11. Magladery
J,
Teasdale R, Park
A.
Electrophysiologic studies of nerve and reflex activity in patients with
lesions
of
the nervous system: a comparison
of
spinal motorneuron excitability following afferent nerve
volleys
in
normal persons and patients with upper motor neuron lesions. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp
1952;91 :219-44.
Sleep, Vol. 12,
No.4,
1989
... Lucid dreamers are aware of the hallucinatory character of ongoing perceptions while asleep (1-5), allowing them to voluntarily interact with their internal world models (6) via fully immersive simulations (2,5). LD happens predominantly during rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep, but can also occur during the N1 and N2 sleep stages (7)(8)(9)(10). ...
... Despite five decades of EEG research on the topic, these studies still paint an inconclusive picture of LD neurophysiology (17,18): earlier findings such as increased occipital alpha (8)(9)(10)(11)(12) (19) parietal beta (13)(14)(15)(16)(17)(18)(19) Hz) (20) or frontal gamma (36)(37)(38)(39)(40)(41)(42)(43)(44)(45) underlying LD (21) could not be substantiated after adequate removal of nonneural artifacts (18). One independently replicated finding is that reduction in delta-band (0.5-2.9 Hz (22); 2-4 Hz (18)) activity differentiates LD from non-lucid dreaming, potentially pointing to alterations in sleep depth. ...
... Results of global, sensor-level power analyses are shown in Figure 4A-C. As expected, the waking condition showed a pronounced power increase in the alpha band (8)(9)(10)(11)(12) Hz) compared to non-waking stages. A robust increase in power can also be seen from 20 Hz upwards ( Figure 4A) during waking. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Lucid dreaming (LD) is a state of conscious awareness of the current dream state, predominantly associated with REM sleep. Research progress in uncovering the neurobiological basis of LD has been hindered by low sample sizes, diverse EEG setups, and specific artifact issues like saccadic eye movements and signal non-stationarity. To address these matters, we developed a multi-stage preprocessing pipeline that integrates standardized early-stage preprocessing, artifact subspace reconstruction, and signal-space projection. This approach enhanced data quality by precisely removing saccadic potential effects even in setups with minimal channels. To robustly identify the electrophysiological correlates of LD, we applied this methodology to LD data collected across laboratories (pooled N = 44) and explored sensor- and source-level markers hypothesized to underlie LD. Compared to non-lucid REM sleep, we observed few robust differences on the EEG sensor level, which is in line with recent findings. In contrast, on the source level, gamma1 power (30-36 Hz) showed increases during LD in left-hemispheric temporal areas, which might reflect verbal insight processes. Gamma1 power also increased around the onset of LD eye signaling in right temporo-occipital regions including the right precuneus, in line with its involvement in self-referential thinking. Reductions in beta power (12-30 Hz) during LD in right central and parietal areas including the temporo-parietal junction are potentially associated with a conscious reassessment of the veridicality of the currently perceived reality. Notably, functional connectivity in alpha band (8-12 Hz) increased during LD, in contrast to the reductions typically seen in psychedelic states, highlighting enhanced self-awareness. Taken together, these findings illuminate the electrophysiological correlates of LD state, and may serve as a basis to uncover neural mechanisms at the time point of lucid dream insight.
... During lucid dreaming states, the sleeping body's response to environmental stimuli was found to be further suppressed during lucid dreaming states (Brylowski et al., 1989;Stumbrys, 2011). The neurotransmitter, acetylcholine, which is associated with the occurrence of lucid dreaming, is also found to be associated with the suppression of muscle reflexes responding to neural stimulation (Gott et al., 2024). ...
... This seems to demonstrate that body-environment interaction 5 The further suppressed muscle movements do not necessarily contradict the possibilities of the subjects' signaling to the experimenter and sensory incorporation during lucid dreaming states. For the former, suppressed bodily movements are associated with increased eye movement densities (Brylowski et al., 1989;Gott et al., 2024), making signaling to the experimenter possible during lucid dreaming states. For the latter, although bodily reactions to environmental stimuli were further suppressed (Stumbrys, 2011), neural responses to the environmental stimuli remain possible during lucid dreaming states because of the more active neural activities, which can be associated with sensory incorporation during lucid dreaming states. ...
Article
Full-text available
Enactivism interprets conscious experiences as interaction between the subject’s body and the physical environment (i.e., the body-environment interaction). During dreaming states, however, the body-environment interaction is largely limited. In this case, the phenomenal similarity between dreaming and waking experiences poses a significant challenge to enactivism. This paper proposes an enactivist account of dreaming experiences as a response to this challenge. In particular, this enactivist account explains the phenomenal similarity between dreaming and waking perceptual experiences as an illusion resulting from the disrupted subjective perspective embedded in dreaming experiences due to the limited body-environment interaction. The phenomenal consequence of this disrupted subjective perspective makes dreaming subjects experience the self-generated mental imagery during dreaming states as more realistic and perceptual than the self-generated mental imagery during waking states. In this case, the dreaming experiences are distinguished from waking perceptual experiences in terms of the phenomenal content and distinguished from waking experiences of mental imagery in terms of the phenomenal conceptualization process. The enactivist account of dreaming experiences I propose in this paper manages to explain the illusory phenomenal similarity between dreaming and waking perceptual experiences with the level of body-environment interaction, which makes it more advantageous than previous attempts to make enactivism congruous with dreaming experiences.
... Le SP est normalement caractérisé par la suppression du réflexe H en électromyographie de surface car de la glycine est libérée au niveau post-synaptique médullaire du motoneurone spinal, bloquant la transmission de l'influx moteur (tonus de posture et motricité volitionnelle) du premier au deuxième motoneurone de la voie pyramidale. Dans une étude comparant 150 époques de SP lucide avec 2470 époques de SP non lucide, l'amplitude du réflexe H était plus basse en SP lucide qu'en SP non lucide, c'est à dire que le réflexe H était encore plus atténué que dans le SP normal (BRYLOWSKI et al., 1989). La densité des mouvements oculaires rapides augmentait aussi en période de SP lucide, par rapport au SP normal. ...
... En effet, le SP est normalement caractérisé par la suppression du réflexe H en électromyographie. Dans une étude comparant 150 époques de SP lucide avec 2470 époques de SP non lucide, l'amplitude du réflexe H était plus basse en SP lucide qu'en SP non lucide, c'est à dire que le réflexe H était encore plus atténué que dans le SP normal (BRYLOWSKI et al., 1989). La densité des mouvements oculaires rapides augmentait aussi en période de SP lucide, par rapport au SP normal. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Les expériences de notre nuit sont souvent décrites comme des îlots d'activité mentale, internement générées dans un océan d'inconscience. En sous-texte de cette vision se cachent deux pré-supposés que le sommeil lent est un modèle d'inconscience et que le traitement sensoriel du monde extérieur en sommeil paradoxal ne peut être qu'inconscient. Dans cette thèse, nous avons voulu tester ces pré-supposés avec une approche empruntant à trois littératures complémentaires : celle de la conscience, celle du sommeil sain et pathologique et celle de la philosophie de l'esprit. Dans une première étude nous avons mis en évidence l'existence de "blackout' de nuit : une absence total de rappel de contenu du couche au lever dans l'hypersomnie Idiopathique. Nous pensons que notre démonstration de l'existence du phénomène de blackout est intéressante car elle permet, par contraste, de mettre en évidence l'existence d'une expérience minimale de la nuit, comme les philosophes l'avaient suggéré. Dans deux autres études nous avons montré la capacité de patients narcoleptiques (lucides ou non) à traiter l'extérieur pendant des siestes en utilisant comme réponses les muscles de leurs visages. Cela suggère qu'un traitement conscient dans le sommeil peut avoir lieu en sommeil paradoxal chez ces patients. L'ensemble de ce travail de thèse invite à penser que l'idée selon laquelle on perd conscience pendant que l'on dort serait à réévaluer. En effet, une réelle perte de conscience dans le sommeil, si elle existe, pourrait être plutôt transitoire et négligeable face à la fabuleuse pluralité des processus qui se déroulent en son sein.
... Phasic REM has been associated with increased dream vividness, dream emotion, PGO wave and REM density (Simor et al., 2020) all of which are indirect precursors for lucid dreaming. Furthermore, lucid dreaming has been found to be associated with increased phasic REM activity in its own right, indicated by increased eye movements and physiological activation and suppression of the Hreflex, a muscle reaction to electrical stimulation of innervating fibres (Brylowski et al., 1989). Interestingly, galantamine administration specifically has been shown to decrease REM latency and increase REM duration at the expense of NREM sleep. ...
... 20 The excitability of H-reflex depends on the excitability of the lower and upper motor neuron pools, and basal ganglia, cerebellum, or sleep states modulate it. 21,22 In our study, the amplitude of the SRs was low only in the group with focal hand dystonia, suggesting reduced excitability in this motoneuron pool. Regarding H-reflex, there was no change in its amplitude or H/M ratio in a group of patients with cervical or generalized dystonia in a previous study. ...
Article
We hypothesized that “long latency reflexes” (LLRs), associated segmental reflex (SR), and mixed nerve silent periods (MnSPs) recorded on the distal upper extremity muscles would behave differently in patients with cervical dystonia and focal hand dystonia. We enrolled patients with cervical dystonia, generalized dystonia, focal hand dystonia, and healthy individuals. We recorded SR, LLRs, and MnSPs. The mean amplitude of SR on the affected side of focal hand dystonia was significantly lower ( p = 0.010). The parameters related to LLRs and MnSPs were not different between groups. We suggest, using SR, LLRs, and MnSPs, we could not show an electrophysiological signature specific to dystonia.
... 2.2.1. Lucid Dreaming, REM Sleep, and the Brain Rhythms LD occurs predominantly during REM sleep [48,49]. LD has also been reported during the N1 (sleep onset) and N2 (light sleep) stages but not during the N3 stage (deep sleep) [50,51]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies have begun to understand sleep not only as a whole-brain process but also as a complex local phenomenon controlled by specific neurotransmitters that act in different neural networks, which is called “local sleep”. Moreover, the basic states of human consciousness—wakefulness, sleep onset (N1), light sleep (N2), deep sleep (N3), and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep—can concurrently appear, which may result in different sleep-related dissociative states. In this article, we classify these sleep-related dissociative states into physiological, pathological, and altered states of consciousness. Physiological states are daydreaming, lucid dreaming, and false awakenings. Pathological states include sleep paralysis, sleepwalking, and REM sleep behavior disorder. Altered states are hypnosis, anesthesia, and psychedelics. We review the neurophysiology and phenomenology of these sleep-related dissociative states of consciousness and update them with recent studies. We conclude that these sleep-related dissociative states have a significant basic and clinical impact since their study contributes to the understanding of consciousness and the proper treatment of neuropsychiatric diseases.
... Waking-initiated-lucid-dreams on the other hand, are initiated from waking states such as the transitional state of hypnagogia or as a result of microawakenings arising during the normal course of one's sleep episode. Physiologically, lucid REM states are characterized by greater H-reflex suppression (Brylowski et al., 1989) and heightened levels of nervous system and cortical activation (LaBerge et al., 1986) compared with nonlucid REM. In particular, increased activation of frontopolar and temporoparietal regions including the anterior prefrontal cortex, parietal lobules, and precuneus have been reported during lucid dreaming states (Dresler et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
A lucid dream is a dream during which the dreamer becomes aware of the fact that they are experiencing a dream. The higher awareness and cognitive traits which accompany lucid dreams offer individuals a unique opportunity to use this practice to rehearse waking skills. This scoping review aimed to summarize existing evidence on the effectiveness of lucid dreaming practice (LDP) on the performance of waking skills and used a meta-analytical approach to estimate an overall effect of LDP. A total of seven studies were reviewed. Findings indicate that LDP can improve waking performance of motor tasks of a variety of nature with an overall medium positive effect size of .483 (p = .095). LDP environments appear to be suitable for the practice of waking tasks however practitioners should be aware that perception may at times be distorted from wakefulness. Dream distractions may also be encountered which may impair dream practice and have a negative effect on subsequent performance. A lack of empirical evidence within the literature was identified. Challenges with conducting LDP research are discussed and recommendations for future research are proposed.
... For example, it is common for individuals in a lucid dream to purposefully defy gravity by floating or flying above the landscape [1]. The neural mechanisms of this dramatic shift in dream consciousness are not known, although some preliminary studies suggest that they may arise from hybrid states of awareness that meld REM sleep and wakefulness [2] or from particularly activated REM sleep periods [3][4][5]. One neural feature of REM sleep that may contribute is an activation of vestibular nuclei in the brain stem, especially during phasic episodes when rapid eye movements are prominent [6]. ...
Article
Study Objectives Early research suggests that the vestibular system is implicated in lucid dreaming, e.g., frequent lucid dreamers outperform others on static balance tasks. Further, gravity-themed dreams, such as flying dreams, frequently accompany lucid dreaming. Nonetheless, studies are scarce. Methods We attempted to: 1) replicate previous findings using more sensitive static balance measures and 2) extend these findings by examining relationships with dreamed gravity imagery more generally. 131 participants (80 F; Mage=24.1±4.1yrs) estimated lucid dreaming frequency then completed a 5-day home log with ratings for dream lucidity awareness, control, and gravity sensations (flying, falling). They then performed balance tasks on a sensitive force plate, e.g., standing on one or both feet, with eyes open or closed. Center of pressure (CoP) Displacement and CoP Velocity on each trial measured postural stability. Results Findings partially support the claim of a vestibular contribution to lucid dreaming. Frequent lucid dreamers displayed better balance (lower CoP Velocity) than did other participants on some trials and lucid dreaming frequency was globally correlated with better balance (lower CoP Velocity). Lower CoP Velocity was related to flying sensations in men’s dreams and with more dream control in women’s dreams. However, body height—possibly due to its relationship to sex—and levels of sleepiness confound some of these effects. Conclusion While findings only provide a partial replication of previous work, they nonetheless support an emerging view that the vestibular system underlies basic attributes of bodily self-consciousness, such as feelings of self-agency and self-location, whether such consciousness occurs during wakefulness or dreaming.
Article
During most dreams, the dreamer does not realize that they are in a dream. In contrast, lucid dreaming allows to become aware of the current state of mind, often accompanied by considerable control over the ongoing dream episode. Lucid dreams can happen spontaneously or be induced through diverse behavioural, cognitive or technological strategies. Such induction techniques have spurred research into the potential therapeutic aspects of lucid dreams. In this review, we gather evidence on the link between lucid dreams and conditions like nightmare disorder, depression, anxiety, psychosis, and dissociative states, and explore the possible neurobiological basis of these associations. Furthermore, we delve into contemplative sleep practices that train lucid states during sleep, such as Dream/Sleep Yoga and Yoga Nidrâ. The potential drawbacks of lucid dreaming interventions are outlined, accompanied by an examination of the impacts of lucid dreams on individuals without clinical conditions. By shedding light on these intricate relationships, the review contributes to a deeper understanding of the therapeutic possibilities and implications of lucid dreaming.
Chapter
This chapter aims to elaborate on motor learning in lucid dreams. Lucid dreaming training is the systematic repetition of a sports action with the experienced “dream body” in a lucid dream, with the aim of practicing movement sequences. It does not represent a traditional training method, however, anecdotal reports, questionnaire studies, qualitative studies, and experimental investigations show that training in a lucid dream can improve movement sequences for waking life. The mode of action is explained with the simulation theory, in which cognitive movement executions are understood as a simulation of the actual movement. For lucid dreaming, there are indications at various levels that suggest that movements in the dream represent a simulation. The findings so far are thus quite promising, which allows for some concrete recommendations for sports practice to be derived.
Article
Full-text available
The occurrence of lucid dreaming (dreaming while being conscious that one is dreaming) has been verified for 5 selected subjects who signaled that they knew they were dreaming while continuing to dream during unequivocal REM sleep. The signals consisted of particular dream actions having observable concomitants and were performed in accordance with pre-sleep agreement. The ability of proficient lucid dreamers to signal in this manner makes possible a new approach to dream research--such subjects, while lucid, could carry out diverse dream experiments marking the exact time of particular dream events, allowing derivation of of precise psychophysiological correlations and methodical testing of hypotheses.
Article
Full-text available
Reports of lucid dreaming (i.e., dreaming while being conscious that one is dreaming) were verified for 13 Ss (aged 21–51 yrs) who signaled by means of voluntary eye-movements that they knew they were dreaming while continuing to dream during REM sleep. Physiological analysis of the resulting 76 signal-verified lucid dreams (SVLDs) revealed that elevated levels of automatic nervous system activity reliably occurred both during and 30 sec preceding the onset of SVLDs, implicating physiological activation as a necessary condition for reflective consciousness during REM dreaming. It is concluded that the ability of proficient lucid dreamers to deliberately perform dream actions in accordance with presleep agreement makes possible determination of psychophysiological correspondence during REM dreaming. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Phasic inhibition of the neck or submental musculature during bursts of eye movements (man and cat) and lateral geniculate spikes (cat) during desynchronized sleep is accompanied by spinal monosynaptic reflex inhibition. Phasic EMG suppressions also occur during synchronized sleep in both species, and in man these suppressions are accompanied by total inhibition of the monosynaptic H-reflex. Modulation of spinal monosynaptic activity during phasic EMG suppressions in slow wave sleep has not been studied in the cat, and the present investigation examines this relationship. Electroencephalographic, electromyographic (posterior cervical or orbicularis oris muscles), and lateral geniculate activity were recorded from four adult unrestrained, unanesthetized cats. Electrical stimulation of the tibial nerve via implanted cuff electrodes elicited reflex responses which were recorded from the flexor digitorum brevis. Recordings began one week after implantation. Electrographic data were recorded on paper writeout and magnetic tape, and reflex responses were monitored on an oscilloscope as well. Phasic EMG suppressions were observed in all animals, but reflex responses coincided with these inhibitions in only three cats. Unlike those in man, reflex responses occurring with EMG suppressions during slow wave sleep in the cat were not totally inhibited. In the samples recorded, no significant reflex amplitude modulation during the EMG suppression could be demonstrated, either with respect to immediately adjacent responses or relative to another randomly selected sample of responses occurring during sustained EMG activity. These findings are related to previous observations in the human being, and the relationship between lateral geniculate spike activity and EMG suppressions in the cat is discussed.
Article
The relationship between monosynaptic spinal reflex (MSR) activity and monophasic spike activity of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) was studied in unanesthetized, unrestrained cats during spontaneous sleep. Relative to MSR modulation during slow-wave sleep (SWS), modulation during paradoxical sleep (PS) was toward decreased reflex amplitude within 500 ms subsequent to the onset of isolated LGN spikes (t0). These modulations, although statistically significant, were not particularly pronounced and were not always consistent for all animals. Reflex amplitude decreased in the 1 s following onset of either small (three to four spikes) or larger (≥ 5 spikes) spike bursts, but this reduction was significant only for the latter group. Consistent changes in reflex amplitude did not outlast brief spike bursts even when additional isolated spikes occurred in the extended sample interval (2.2 e post-t0). Extended modulation (5 s post-t0) of reflex activity was detected for bursts of ≥ 14 spikes. Whereas previous reports indicated variations in MSR activity only in association with bursts of LGN spikes, the present data suggest state-dependent modulation of such activity for isolated and low-frequency bursts as well as high-frequency spike bursts. A possible role for the caudal raphe nuclei in the generation of low frequency pontogeniculate (PGO) activity and related reflex variations is postulated, and bases for discrepancies between phasic MSR modulation during synchronized sleep in man and cat are explored.
Article
The activity of the mental and hyoid muscles, and the H-reflex were examined during nocturnal sleep and daytime naps of narcoleptic and normal subjects.The continuous, tonic EMG discharges, which were observed in all subjects in the awake state, decreased in parallel with deepening of sleep but disappeared only during the rapid eye movements (REM) period, which occurred at the sleep onset in narcoleptics and late in nocturnal sleep in normal and narcoleptic subjects. During the REM period, only transient, phasic EMG discharges of low voltage were occasionally observed.The H-reflex also decreased in amplitude when the subjects fell asleep. The degree of its decrement was slight in the drowsy stage and was greater in light and deep sleep. During the REM period which occurred at the sleep onset in narcoleptics and late in nocturnal sleep in normal and narcoleptic subjects, the decrement was most prominent and consistent and the H-reflex reflex would completely disappear.The sleep onset REM period was examined in four narcoleptics, two of which experienced sleep paralysis in this period. It is concluded that sleep paralysis is not a part of the hypnagogic hallucinations often experienced by narcoleptics but a real experience of paralysis resulting from a dissociation between the level of consciousness and the somatic muscle and reflex activities in the sleep onset REM period.
Lucid dreaming verified by volitional communication during REM sleep
  • Sp Laberge
  • Le Nagel
  • Wc Dement
  • Vp Zarcone
LaBerge SP, Nagel LE, Dement WC, Zarcone VP. Lucid dreaming verified by volitional communication during REM sleep. Percept Mot Skills 1981 ;52:727-32.