ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract

Research in the area of motivation in autistic and other children suggests that autistic children may be capable of performing at a higher level than they typically function. This article describes research in the area of 'learned helplessness' which suggests that autistic children's handicaps may expose them to frequent failure and to an unusual level of non-contingent reinforcement which may produce a 'learned helplessness' state of extremely low motivation, with a consequent abnormally low overall functioning level. The present article also discusses research on strategies which might be employed to improve autistic children's exposure to favourable response-reinforcer contingencies. This would be expected to improve the general level of motivation in such children, with related gains in the acquisition, generalization maintenance of a broad array of target behaviours.
/
Child
Psychol.
Psychiat.
Vol. 26, No. 2, pp.
185-191,
1985. 0021-9630/85 $3.00
+
0.00
Printed in Great Britain. Pergamon Press Ltd
© 1985 Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
ANNOTATION
MOTIVATION IN CHILDHOOD AUTISM: CAN THEY OR WON'T THEY?
ROBERT L. KOEGEL and MICHELLE MENTIS
University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, U.S.A.
PARADOXICALLY, autistic children have been described at times as extremely
intelligent and at other times as severely retarded (cf. Rimland, 1978). For example,
teachers are often puzzled by autistic children appearing very intelligent in isolated
areas yet also giving the appearance of being unable to learn relatively simple tasks
such as self-help and basic language skills. As late as 1978, Rutter reported that
approximately
75 %
of autistic children could be expected to function at a retarded
or severely retarded level throughout life. This 'retardation', however, appears to be
highly variable across tasks and situations. For example, such children have been
described as extremely unmotivated during testing and in other circumstances where
demands are miade of them (Koegel & Egel, 1979; Mittler, 1966) and seem to p)erform
especially poorly under such conditions. These and other observations have led
researchers to question whether autistic children's typically very low level of per-
formance reflects an inability to learn, or instead a possible lack of motivation to learn.
EFFECTS OF FAILURE ON MOTIVATION
A number of researchers have suggested that motivation may be depressed,
performance impaired and task avoidance increased by repeated experiences of
failure (Clark & Rutter, 1979; MacMillan, 1971; Rodda, 1977). This is particularly
pertinent to autistic children as the severity of their handicap may result in them
being repeatedly exposed to failure. In addition, pathological failure in autistic and
schizophrenic children has been found to increase dramatically during exposure to
failure (Churchill, 1971). It has also been noted that as autistic children frequently
respond incorrectly when attempting a task, noncontingent reinforcement or unusual
accidental contingencies of reinforcement may be created and the attempts the child
does make may either go unrewarded or be minimally rewarded (Koegel & Egel,
1979).
These conditions may thus serve to. decrease further their motivation.
In contrast, the positive influence of success on motivation was clearly demon-
strated in a study conducted by O'Dell, Dunlap & Koegel (1983). They compared
the effectiveness of two reinforcement contingencies on the verbal responding of
four autistic children with severe communication delay. They found that all the
children achieved higher percentages of correct verbal responding and progressed
more rapidly when a 'motivation' contingency was used in which the children's
Requests for reprints to: Autism Research Center, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, U.S.A.
185
186 R. L. KOEGEL
AND
MICHELLE MENTIS
observable attempts to verbalize were reinforced. The other, less successful con-
tingency used was a more narrowly defined shaping contingency under which only
very strictly defined successive approximations to a target verbalization were
reinforced regardless of how hard the child was 'trying'. The former (attempts)
contingency was superior in all areas. In addition to improvements in their learning,
when the chOdren's affect was rated by naive observers all the children were judged
to be happier, more enthusiastic, more interested and better behaved when their
verbal attempts were reinforced. The results suggest that by reinforcing communica-
tion attempts, the children's motivation to maintain interest and attention in a task
may be increased zind that, at least for this severely handicapped population, it may
be more important to increase motivation before further refining the types of
behaviour that are being taught through shaping.
LEARNED HELPLESSNESS
A further significant implication of this study relates to the notions of success and
failure and of learned helplessness. Seligman and his colleagues have argued that
learned helplessness results from learning that reinforcement and responding are
independent (Miller & Seligman, 1975; Overmier & Seligman, 1967; Seligman &
Maier, 1967). They state that such learning serves to: "lower performance by
reducing the incentive for instrumental responding, which results in lowered response
initiation. In addition, learning that reinforcement and responding are independent
interferes with learning that responses later control reinforcements" (Miller &
Seligman, 1975, p. 228). Learned helplessness thus results in individuals being slow
to initiate responses and in their having greater difficulty in learning the response-
reinforcement contingency. It has been suggested that the types of behaviours shown
in learned helplessness studies are very similar to those exhibited by autistic children
(Koegel & Egel, 1979). Seligman, Maier & Geer (1968) have suggested that the
maladaptive behaviours associated with learned helplessness may be corrected by
enforced exposure to the response-reinforcement contingency. In the above study,
as the children experienced more success during the attempt condition they experienced
the response-reinforcer contingency more frequently. Dunlap (1984) also found a
similar positive effect by interspersing well-learned maintenance tasks among new
tasks being taught, suggesting that there may be a generalized positive influence
obtained when autistic children obtain more experience with response-reinforcer
contingencies.
The notion of learned helplessness and the suggestion that success may heighten
motivation was also discussed in a study conducted by Koegel & Egel (1979). They
investigated the effect on motivation of correct vs incorrect task completion. Their
results indicated that the children's motivation (which was defined in terms ofthe
children's attempts to complete a task and the level of enthusiasm the child displayed)
decreased to extremely low levels when they worked on tasks that they did not
complete correctly. However, when the children were prompted to keep responding
until they had completed the tasks correctly, their motivation to respond to those
tasks was markedly increased. The results are thus consistent with those reported by
ANNOTATION 187
O'Dell, Dunlap & Koegel (1983) and suggest that the experience of failure may
result in decreased motivation, cind that by increasing successful task completion,
motivation may be heightened. This may be done through prompting, as in the above
study, by interspersing maintenance tasks (Dunlap, 1984) or by altering the reinforce-
ment contingency so that attempts are reinforced regardless of whether they are
followed by success (O'Dell
et
al, 1983).
In a related study investigating the variables controlling social avoidance behaviour
(Dyer, Bell & Koegel, 1983) it was found that when autistic children engaged in
child-preferred activities, decreased levels of social avoidance behaviour were
exhibited. On the basis of these results the authors suggested that by being able to
direct the sociad situation, increased success in this situation was experienced and
social approach behaviour was therefore reinforced. Thus by maximizing the child's
chances of success, motivation to engage in social approach behaviour was
heightened and, by being reinforced for this behaviour, the generalization and
maintenance of the behaviour was increased. These results are also significant in
that the child's motivation was maintained by reinforcement contingencies occurring
in the child's everyday life, important for the continued maintenance ofthe behaviour.
SHARED CONTROL AND INCREASED SUCCESS
Recently, several studies have investigated the effects of shared control amd the
use of child-preferred activities on the motivation of autistic children. Although the
results of these studies are preliminary, they strongly suggest that the motivation of
autistic children can be significantly increased and that this may result in increased
generalization and maintenance of learned behaviour.
It has been suggested that by giving autistic and other children some control over
the choice of topic of conversation, the materials to be used or the activities to be
engaged in, an increased level of interest and motivation in the activity may be
achieved (Dyer et al, 1983; O'Dell & Koegel, 1981; Turner, 1978). This is an
important consideration as it has been reported that the major type of interaction
between adults and autistic children seems to be adult-directed demands and requests
(Bernard-Opitz, 1982; Duchan, 1983). In a study conducted by Wetherby (1982)
autistic children were found to initiate communication with adults in a free-play
setting frequently. As this is inconsistent with anecdotal reports in the literature that
autistic children lack spontaneous use of communication, Wetherby suggested that
the results she obtained may have been due to the fact that the adult in her study
was non-directive. She felt that this gave the child opportunities to engage in child-
preferred activities rather than those selected by the adult. Similar results were
obtained by Bernard-Opitz (1982), who found that an autistic child initiated com-
munication more frequently when the parent was instructed not to initiate communica-
tion with her child, but only to respond to her child's initiations. It may be suggested
that the increase in the child's initiations was due to her increased control in the
interaction once she was able to initiate topics with a high probability of success.
In a related study described by O'Dell & Koegel (1981) a natural language
paradigm of treatment delivery was more successful than a repetitive practice
paradigm. Two of the variables differentiating the 'natural language paradigm'
188 R. L. KOEGEL AND MICHELLE MENTIS
from the 'repetitive practice paradigm' were turn-taking and shared control. In the
natural language paradigm, turn-taking (the exchange between the speaker and
listener that takes place in normal conversation) and shared control (the sharing by
both the child and the clinician of the topic and the control over the communicative
act) were major factors in delivering the stimulus materials. If the child indicated
with an appropriate vocal request that the activity should be discontinued, or that a
new activity should be started, the clinician would comply. Thus in this treatment
approach both the child and the clinician were active participants in the activity. In
the 'repetitive practice paradigm', however, a driU format was used in which repeated
trials were presented by the clinician who then waited or prompted the child to
respond (i.e. used clinician control). A marked difference was found between the
two methods of language intervention regarding the children's utterances in the
imitative, deferred imitative and spontaneous utterance categories. In the 'natural
language paradigm' the results indicated an increased use of appropriate, spon-
taneous and generalized speech and language, whereas in the 'repetitive practice
paradigm' the results showed a low level of imitative and spontaneous production of
target words, and negligible evidence of generalization. These results and those of
related research (cf. Hart & Risley, 1980; McGee, Krantz & McClannahan, in
press;
Neef,
Walters & Egel, in press) suggest that a 'natural language paradigm'
may be a useful form of treatment as it increases the child's interest and motivation
to learn and facilitates generalization of acquired skills. Generalization is further
facilitated by the fact that the programme utilizes stimulus events and contingencies
which more closely approximate the child's natur2il environment.
That choice may be a significant motivational variable for autistic children was
further demonstrated in the previously mentioned study by Dyer
et
al (1983). They
found that decreased levels of social avoidance behaviour were exhibited when the
children engaged in child-preferred activities £ind that higher ratings of interest and
involvement in the social interaction were obtained when the toys and topics for
conversation were selected by the child. They also found that a child who participated
in a long-term clinical intervention exhibited a generalized reduction of social avoid-
ance responses after clinical intervention. Thus it may be suggested that generalization
and maintenance of appropriate behaviour was facilitated by the child's increased
motivation in interacting with strangers, resulting from the child tEiking some control
ofthe interaction and introducing preferred topics.
These results are consistent with what has been reported in associated areas.
Studies in the normal population have suggested that when a subject is given a choice
in the selection of the antecedent stimuli, motivation is increased. In addition,
opportunity for choice has been found to affect learning ability and the generalization
of the learned behaviour significantly (Blackwell, 1975; Kail, 1975; Monty,
Rosenberger & Perlmuter, 1973). In a study conducted by Turner (1978) with two
preschool language-impaired children, interest in the remediation setting was
increased when they were given a choice of stimulus materials. In addition, she
found that language structures learned during this condition were more likely to
generalize to extra-clinic settings than when materials were selected by the
experimenter.
The above studies suggest that the low motivation demonstrated by autistic
ANNOTATION 189
children can be increased in a setting where the control is shared by the child and
clinician and where the child is given a choice regarding the selection of materials,
activities and topics. In addition, they suggest that increased motivation may
facilitate generalization and maintenance of treatment gains as the child is moving
towards functioning in the environment in a manner more similar to that of normal
children. This may be facilitated by eliciting teacher, parental, sibling and peer
support in giving the child opportunities to initiate communication and select preferred
topics, activities and toys.
CONCLUDING COMMENTS
The results of the above research suggest that the motivation of autistic children
can be increased and that this may be a crucial variable in the acquisition, generaliza-
tion and maintenance of their treatment gains. This is because the emphasis is
being placed on helping the autistic child respond in a manner more similar to that
of normal children. The low level of motivation evident in autistic children may
inhibit the acquisition of new skills and restrict the generalization and maintenance
of acquired skills beyond treatment settings. However, it seems that if their motivation
can be increased, this will result in the acquisition and maintenance of a much wider
range of behaviour. Thus motivation may be a key target behaviour which could
result in more widespread behaviour change than has typically been seen in the past.
Although the research is still in its infancy, it has led to the identification of some
of the variables that influence autistic children's motivation. Sensory reinforcers
have been found to be powerful for autistic children, and have the advantages of
being more resistant to satiation than primary reinforcers such as food (cf. Rincover,
Newsom, Lovaas & Koegel, 1977). The variation of antecedent and consequent
stimuli has also been shown to influence autistic children's motivation and thereby
maintain a high level of responding (Dunlap & Koegel, 1980; Egel, 1980, 1981). A
most significant finding in this area is the relationship that has been found between
success and motivation. By encouraging autistic children to respond until they
successfully complete tasks, their motivation to perform such tasks in the future may
be increased. A further significant finding is that by increasing the child's control
of the environment, success and motivation may be increased. This can affect a
number of other behaviours and may result in widespread generalization and
maintenance of
a
number of types of behaviour. In doing so, steps are being taken to
enable the autistic child to function in a way that will be more effective in learning
and maintaining new skills. However, these are essentisilly the first steps; while
several ways to increase the autistic child's motivation to work on specific tasks
within treatment have been identified, less research has been directed towards
identifying how the child's motivation in the natural environment can be enhanced.
The further development of more powerful motivational techniques for use with the
autistic child may represent one of the major challenges to researchers in the field
today.
190 R.L. KOEGEL AND MICHELLE MENTIS
SUMMARY
Research in the area of motivation in autistic and other children suggests that
autistic children may be capable of performing at a higher level than they typically
function. This article describes research in the area of 'learned helplessness' which
suggests that autistic children's handicaps may expose them to frequent failure and
to an unusual level of non-contingent reinforcement which may produce a 'learned
helplessness' state of extremely low motivation, with a consequent abnormally low
oversdl functioning level. The present article also discusses research on strategies
which might be employed to improve autistic children's exposure to favourable
response-reinforcer contingencies. This would be expected to improve the general
level of motivation in such children, with related gains in the acquisition, generalization
maintenance of
a
broad array of target behaviours.
Acknowledgements—Preparation
of this annotation was supported in part by the U.S. Department of
Education, Special Education Program Contract No. 300-82-0362 and by U.S. Public Health Service
Research Grants MH28210 and MH28231 from the National Institute of Mental Health.
REFERENCES
Bernard-Opitz, V. (1982). Pragmatic analysis of the communicative behavior of an autistic child.
Journal of
Speech
and
Hearing
Disorders,
4i7, 99-109.
Blackwell, L. R. (1975). Student
choice
in
curriculum,
feelings of
control
and
causality,
and
academic
motivation
and
performance.
Unpublished dissertation.
Churchill, D. W. (1971). Effects of success and failure in psychotic children.
Archives
of
General
Psychiatry,
25,208-214.
Clark, P. & Rutter, M. (1979). Task difficulty and task performance in autistic children.
Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry,
20, 271-285.
Duchan, J. F. (1983). Autistic children are noninteractive: or so we say.
Seminars
in
Speech and
Language,
4,53-61.
Dunlap, G. (1984). The influence of task variation and maintenance tasks on the learning and affect
of autistic children. Journal of
Experimental
Child
Psychology,
37, 41-64.
Dunlap, G. & Koegel, R. L. (1980). Motivating autistic children through stimulus variation, youma/
of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 619-627.
Dyer, K., Bell, L. K. & Koegel, R. L. (1983).
Generalized reduction
of
social avoidance behaviors
in autistic
children. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Annual Convention,
Anaheim, CA.
Egel, A. L. (1980). The effects of constant versus varied reinforcer presentation on responding by
autistic chUdren. Journal of
Experimental
Child
Psychology,
30, 455-463.
Egel, A. L. (1981). Reinforcer variation: implications for motivating developmentally delayed children.
Journal of Applied
Behavior
Analysis,
14, 345-350.
Hart, B. M. & Risley, T. R. (1980). In vivo language intervention: unanticipated general effects.
Journal of Applied
Behavior
Analysis,
13, 40 7-43
2.
Kail, R. V. (1975). Freedom of choice, task performance and task persistence.
Journal
of
Experimental
Education,
44, 32-34.
Koegel, R. L. & Egel, A. L. (1979). Motivating autistic children.
Journal
of
Abnormal
Psychology,
88,
418-426.
MacMillan, D. L. (1971). The problem of motivation in the education of the mentally retarded.
Exceptional
Children,
37, 579-586.
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J. & McClannahan, L. E. (In press). The facilitative effects of incidental
teaching on prepositional usage by autistic children.
Journal
of
Applied
Behavior Analysis.
ANNOTATION 191
Miller, W. R. & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Depression and learned helplessness in man. Joumat of
Experimental
Psychology,
84,228-238.
Mittler, P. (1966). The psychological assessment of autistic children. In J. K. Wing (Ed.), Early
childhood
autism:
clinical,
educational and social aspects
{pp. 145-158). London: Pergamon Press.
Monty, R. A., Rosenberger, M. A. & Perlmuter, L. C. (1973). Amount of locus of choice as sources
of motivation in paired-associate \ca.rn\n^. Journal of
Experimented
Psychology,
97,
16-21.
Neef,
N. A., Walters, J. & Egel, A. L. (In press). Establishing generative yes/no responses in develop-
mentally delayed chiXdren. Journal of Applied
Behavior
Analysis.
O'Dell, M. C, Dunlap, G. & Koegel, R. L. (1983). The
importance
of
reinforcing verbal
attempts during
speech
training with
nonverbal
children.
Paper presented at the American Psychological Association
Annual Convention, Los Angeles, CA.
O'Dell, M. & Koegel, R. L. (1981). The
differential effects
of two
methods
of promoting
speech
in
non-verbal
autistic
children.
Paper presented at the 1981 Annual Convention ofthe American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, Los Angeles, CA.
Overmier, O. B. & Seligman, M. E. P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape
and avoidance learning, youmo/ of
Comparative Physiological
Psychology,
63,
28-33.
Rimland, B. (1978). Inside the mind of an autistic savant.
Psychology
Today, 12, 68-80.
Rincover, A., Newsom, CD., Lovaas, O. I. & Koegel, R. L. (1977). Some motivational properties
of sensory reinforcement in psychotic children. Journal of Experimentat Child
Psychology,
24, 312-323.
Rodda, M. (1977). Language and language-disordered children. Bulletin of the British
Psychological
Society,
30, 139-142.
Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds), Autism: a
reappraisal
of
concepts
and
treatment
(pp.
1-26).
New York: Plenum Press.
Seligman, M. E. P. & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock, youma/ of
Experimental
Psychology,
74, 1-9.
Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F. & Geer, J. (1968). The alleviation of learned helplessness in the dog.
Journal of
Abnormal
and
Social
Psychology,
73, 256-262.
Turner, B. L. (1978). The
effects
of
choice
of stimulus
materials
on
interest
in the
remediation process
and the
generalized use
of
language
training.
Unpublished thesis. University of California, S8mta Barbara, CA.
Wetherby, A. (1982). Communicative, cognitive, and social
development
in autistic children. Unpublished
dissertation. University of California, San Francisco, CA.
... Just as the blind men describe the elephant as like a snake, tree or fan depending on whether they are touching the trunk, a leg, or an ear, so our present-day health care providers, educators and researchers describe impairments in initiation in terms of their perspective. In this way, a neurologist sees initiation impairments in Parkinson's disease, attributes them to a neurological deficit, and calls them akinesia or apathy (Radakovic and Abrahams, 2018); a psychiatrist sees a lack of goal directed activity in schizophrenia, considers it a lack of motivation, and calls it avolition (e.g., Mucci et al., 2015); a behavior therapist sees an autistic child's failure to respond to an instruction, considers it behavioral, and does not acknowledge it as a separate issue at all (e.g., Koegel and Mentis, 1985;Chevallier et al., 2012). Understanding the commonalities of volition impairments across diagnoses is impeded by a lack of consistent terminology, a tendency to pigeonhole conditions into categories such as 'neurological' and 'psychiatric' , defining conditions solely based on overt characteristics, and the habit of looking only to the prevailing definitions and theories to explain observed traits. ...
... So, when an autistic person does not respond to a request, it is seen as willful refusal to cooperate. Similarly, a lack of social initiation is seen as the outcome of a lack of social motivation (Koegel and Mentis, 1985;Chevallier et al., 2012). These assumptions can lead to inappropriate behavioral interventions, as in the case report by Ming et al. (2004) where an autistic woman who had been subjected to behavior modification interventions for 'non-compliance' was shown to be exerting measurable mental effort that was not manifest in physical action. ...
Article
Full-text available
Difficulty initiating voluntary action is an under-recognized and often invisible impairment in various psychiatric, neurodevelopmental, and neurological conditions. Understanding the commonalities of volition impairments across diagnoses is limited by a lack of consistent terminology, arbitrary distinctions between conditions, the habit of looking only to the prevailing definitions and theories to explain observed traits, and the covert nature of initiation. The siloed approach to research in this area evokes the parable of the blind men and the elephant, where understanding the whole picture is impeded by a limited view. There has been little effort to consider how differing terms overlap or to use objective methods to differentiate phenomena along meaningful lines. We propose a triad of interacting elements, all of which are needed for successful initiation of voluntary action: (i) executive function, (ii) volition, and (iii) movement. Failure to initiate a response may be due to impairments in any of these, which often co-occur. This paper calls for the following considerations to improve research in this area: (i) put aside preconceptions about conditions and their mechanisms to adopt a flexible transdiagnostic approach; (ii) consider executive function, movement, and volition as possible dimensional variations with related underlying mechanisms; (iii) carefully differentiate components of complex functions; (iv) look to first-hand reports for covert and previously unrecognized traits. These approaches have the potential to elucidate the cognitive and biological mechanisms underpinning voluntary action and create a foundation to develop more appropriate and informed interventions.
... Questo forse è dovuto, come per il quinto principio, alla consapevolezza delle ben note difficoltà e alla scarsa motivazione all'apprendimento sociale, benché esplicito, da parte delle persone nello spettro dell'autismo (Bottini, 2018;Clements et al., 2018) anche se, va detto, continua a non essere chiaro se tale mancanza di motivazione all'apprendimento in contesti sociali sia dovuta all'assenza di motivazione sociale (Koegel & Mentis, 1985), alle difficoltà neuropsicologiche esperite durante le interazioni sociali dai bambini e dai ragazzi nello spettro o alla combinazione delle due in senso evolutivo (Vivanti & Rogers, 2011). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
L’adeguatezza e la capacità delle scuole montessoriane di accogliere bambini e ragazzi nello spettro dell’autismo e più in generale con disturbi e condizioni del neurosviluppo appare alquanto controversa ed è un tema che, attualmente, risulta poco esplorato dalla ricerca educativa nonostante la sua innegabile importanza in termini di possibilità di scelta educativa degli stakeholder. Partendo dall’eloquente lavoro di analisi scientifica della pedagogia montessoriana realizzato da Angeline Stoll Lillard nel suo libro Montessori: the science behind the genius (2005; 2007; 2016) e attraverso il framework della Pedagogia della Neurodiversità (Cadavero & Salerni, 2022), il presente elaborato, va ad analizzare e approfondire i nove principi cardine della pedagogia montessoriana, enunciati dall’autrice, mettendoli in relazione al paradigma della neurodiversità e ai più attuali orientamenti scientifici rispetto al trattamento e agli interventi educativi portati avanti con bambini e ragazzi nello spettro dell’autismo e con condizioni e disturbi del neurosviluppo valutandone, al contempo, similarità e divergenze con i contesti educativi tradizionali in modo da poter chiarire se e quanto le scuole montessoriane possano essere considerate effettivamente una buona scelta per lo sviluppo neuroculturale di bambini e ragazzi a sviluppo atipico e, più in generale, una buona scelta educativa per tutti e per ciascuno in termini di inclusività e convivenza delle differenze. Completano questo elaborato a carattere esplorativo, osservazionale e trasversale, alcune considerazioni basate sull’analisi statistica, descrittiva e inferenziale, delle opinioni dirette di un campione non probabilistico composto da 54 stakeholder, raccolte attraverso un questionario online redatto con Google Moduli. Alla fine di ogni capitolo e all’interno delle conclusioni, oltre all’analisi della letteratura e delle opinioni degli stakeholder rispetto all’adeguatezza dei principi montessoriani per lo sviluppo neurodivergente vengono inoltre proposti alcuni suggerimenti utili per l’implementazione di tali principi all’interno delle scuole tradizionali seguendo l’approccio dell’evidence based education e dell’Universal Design for Learning. L’analisi dei principi della pedagogia montessoriana enunciati da Angeline Stoll Lillard conferma la possibilità che le scuole montessoriane, con i dovuti adattamenti e un puntuale lavoro di rete con gli altri servizi, possano garantire un’istruzione e un’educazione di qualità ai bambini e ai ragazzi nello spettro dell’autismo e più in generale a sviluppo neurodivergente o atipico. Per quel che concerne i risultati, la pedagogia montessoriana appare dunque raccomandabile come scelta educativa, tanto all’analisi della letteratura che a giudizio degli stakeholder che hanno risposto al questionario, per queste specifiche popolazioni. Nonostante ciò, i principi montessoriani appaiono ancora piuttosto assenti all’interno dei contesti educativi e scolastici tradizionali e necessitano di essere implementati. Ulteriore ricerca, tuttavia, sarà necessaria in questa direzione per ampliare e confermare i risultati di questo piccolo studio osservazionale a carattere esplorativo.
... Technology can be used to leverage interactions already associated with acoustic instruments whilst providing the ability to design interactions from the ground up to suit users. The breaking down and reconstructing of instruments in this way has a danger of confusing the user if cause and effect are not made meaningful to them and can trigger "learned helplessness" (Koegel and Mentis, 1985), when the user loses motivation and no longer believes that they can achieve the outcome or have any control if an interaction does not match their expectation or is not fully made accessible to them. The problems of this decoupling can be mitigated by clear signposting of how a system works including demonstrating the features and functionality to allow users to become comfortable; obeying commonly used interaction mechanisms (pressing something harder makes it louder); and creating robust systems with few technical issues-or at least mechanisms in place to rapidly find solutions or workarounds to problems to help users get things working again. ...
Article
Full-text available
Within the field of digital musical instruments, there have been a growing number of technological developments aimed at addressing the issue of accessibility to music-making for disabled people. This study summarizes the development of one such technological system—The Modular Accessible Musical Instrument Technology Toolkit (MAMI Tech Toolkit). The four tools in the toolkit and accompanying software were developed over 5 years using an action research methodology. A range of stakeholders across four research sites were involved in the development. This study outlines the methodological process, the stakeholder involvement, and how the data were used to inform the design of the toolkit. The accessibility of the toolkit is also discussed alongside findings that have emerged from the process. This study adds to the established canon of research around accessible digital musical instruments by documenting the creation of an accessible toolkit grounded in both theory and practical application of third-wave human–computer interaction methods. This study contributes to the discourse around the use of participatory and iterative methods to explore issues with, and barriers to, active music-making with music technology. Outlined is the development of each of the novel tools in the toolkit, the functionality they offer, as well as the accessibility issues they address. The study advances knowledge around active music-making using music technology, as well as in working with diverse users to create these new types of systems.
... As described earlier, PBSI value play as a way to utilize the narrow and perseverative interests of individuals with ASD and motivate active engagement in the intervention tasks. For PBSI, the inclusion of play offers an answer to the question that has plagued the field for decades (Koegel & Mentis, 1985): how can we motivate people with ASD to engage in the activities that help them? Proponents of childcentered play therapy also value play as a mode of development, albeit for different reasons. ...
Article
Full-text available
The field of child-centered play therapy has long recognized the value of play for children’s growth and development, however other fields of inquiry have come to realize the value of play as well, such as play-based social interventions for youth with autism spectrum disorder. Despite the differences between the theoretical and pragmatic applications of child-centered play therapy and play-based social interventions, the purpose of this work is to orient a play-based social intervention study with child-centered play therapy approach. In particular, this work will discuss the theoretical and pragmatic approaches of the two fields with emphasis on 2 factors of play: (a) play materials and (b) permissiveness. Additionally, the play experiences of 1 boy, Enoch, are reviewed through the typical stages of child-centered play therapy, to demonstrate how permissive play with technology-based play materials functions in play-based social interventions.
... Zbog toga je neophodno da defektolozi budu upoznati sa procedurama koje se primenjuju prilikom tretmana, a koje se odnose na povećanje saradljivosti deteta tokom tretmana, jer na taj način deca nesmetano mogu da usvajaju neophodne veštine i znanja (Bordieri, 2021;Cuvo et al., 2010;Foxx, 2008;Hagopian & Jennett, 2014;Mohammadzaheri et al., 2015). Kada su u pitanju procedure za povećanje saradljivosti dece tokom tretmana, ističe se kako je najefikasnije primenjivati strategije uz pomoć kojih bi se unapred sprečila pojava nepoželjnih oblika ponašanja (Kern et al., 2002;Odom & Strain, 1986;Wood et al., 2018), kao što su davanje češćih pauza prilikom rada (Butler & Luiselli, 2007;Vollmer et al., 1995), davanje nagrada koje su značajne detetu Osborne & Reed, 2008), kao i postepeno povećanje težine prezentovanih zadataka (Koegel & Mentis, 1985;Tillmann & Swettenham, 2019). ...
Article
The aim of this research was to examine the frequency of different barriers faced by special education teachers in working with children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The sample included 53 participants of both genders and of different ages who were diagnosed with ASD. The instrument used to assess the presence of barriers in teaching refers to the Assessment of Barriers in Teaching (VB-Mapp Barriers Assessment -Sundberg, 2008). In our sample, it was found that all respondents have a pronounced presence of teaching barriers, while the most frequent barriers are prompt dependence, generalization difficulties, socialization problems, escape or avoidance of given tasks and the absence of the tact ability. Having in mind the obtained results, it is highlighted as a necessity to develop an approach that would lead to elimination of the mentioned barriers, as well as to develop a treatment plan to address those teaching barriers in working with children with PSA and consequently enable a higher degree of adoption of new knowledge and skills by children with ASD.
... It has been used to improve language skills, play and social behaviour in children with ASD through the use of natural reinforcement by offering as reinforcement that which is related to the behaviour being taught, and by offering a reward when it is attempted even though it is not performed perfectly, except in the case of a lack of effort or a chance hit [11]. The development of PRT focuses on specific areas that in turn produce generalised positive improvements and the production of a consequence after a given behaviour, since in children with ASD the term learned helplessness (they do not understand the connection between response and consequence) occurs [10]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The main aim of this study is to show an intervention protocol for emotional deficits through the use of robotics as a therapeutic tool in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Pepper, the humanoid robot which is capable of detecting emotions and is designed to interact with people, was used together with the Empatica E4 bracelet, a portable device during interactive and invasive states, capable of collecting physiological data such as electrodermal activity that reflects changes in the skin’s electrical resistance (conductance) due to different types of emotions. This intervention takes place through the relationship between the psychological state originated by therapy sessions and the galvanic skin response throughout the sessions based on pivotal area training, complemented with a set of psychological tests that verify the diagnosis of ASD and the evolution of the disorder during therapy.KeywordsAutism spectrum disorderRobotic therapyPRTGSR
... Nearly all research on initiation in autism is interventionbased research with autistic children using the frequency of social initiation as an outcome measure. Researchers consider various mechanisms for diminished social interaction in autistic children, such as lack of social motivation Kohls et al., 2012) or learned helplessness (Koegel and Mentis, 1985); however, none consider the possibility of an underlying deficit in the ability to initiate actions. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study, called for by autistic people and led by an autistic researcher, is the first to explore ‘autistic inertia,’ a widespread and often debilitating difficulty acting on their intentions. Previous research has considered initiation only in the context of social interaction or experimental conditions. This study is unique in considering difficulty initiating tasks of any type in real life settings, and by gathering qualitative data directly from autistic people. Four face-to-face and 2 online (text) focus groups were conducted with 32 autistic adults (19 female, 8 male, and 5 other), aged 23–64 who were able to express their internal experiences in words. They articulate in detail the actions they have difficulty with, what makes it easier or harder to act, and the impact on their lives. Thematic analysis of the transcripts found four overarching themes: descriptions of inertia, scaffolding to support action, the influence of wellbeing, and the impact on day-to-day activities. Participants described difficulty starting, stopping and changing activities that was not within their conscious control. While difficulty with planning was common, a subset of participants described a profound impairment in initiating even simple actions more suggestive of a movement disorder. Prompting and compatible activity in the environment promoted action, while mental health difficulties and stress exacerbated difficulties. Inertia had pervasive effects on participants’ day-to-day activities and wellbeing. This overdue research opens the door to many areas of further investigation to better understand autistic inertia and effective support strategies.
Chapter
Pivotal response treatment (PRT) is an evidence-based intervention (Koegel and Koegel, Pivotal response treatment for autism spectrum disorders. Brookes Publishing Company, 2019; National Research Council, Educating children with autism. National Academies Press, 2001; Wong et al., J Autism Dev Disord 45(7):1951–1966, 2014) that broadly targets social communication and other areas. Children with autism often lack the pivotal area of social motivation or exhibit apparent learned-helplessness, which can be a barrier to many areas of development. Social motivation drives people to orient to the social world, seek and receive pleasure from interactions, and work to create and maintain social relationships (Chevallier et al., Trends Cogn Sci 16(4):231–239, 2012), and thus, it is an important consideration for intervention. As motivation is crucial in the process of learning, PRT, which focuses on implementing specific variables to improve motivation, creates opportunities for developing skills more rapidly while decreasing interfering behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
In our study, we wanted to determine how parents of children on the autistic spectrum, assessed their child's therapy during and before the pandemic. We tried to determine the children's participation in therapy, the change in frequency, the form and type of therapy, parental evaluation of competence and satisfaction with the therapy. For this purpose, we used a questionnaire. The results showed that almost all children participated in therapy during this period despite the limitations caused by the pandemic. However, the number of hours of therapy provided to children in institutions decreased, while that provided remotely increased. The effectiveness of the conducted therapies decreased compared to the time before the pandemic. The most negative changes in children's functioning occurred in the areas of social functioning, sensory and sensory integration. The results showed that the competence of the parents either increased or held at the same level, whilst their satisfaction in their child's therapy significantly decreased during the pandemic.
Article
Full-text available
Otizm Spektrum Bozukluğu (OSB) olan çocuklarda sosyal iletişim problemleri ve tekrarlanan, basmakalıp davranışlar gözlenmektedir. OSB olan çocukların hayal gücü ve yaratıcılık gerektiren aktivitelerdeki farklı gelişimine ilişkin araştırmalar da söz konusudur. Özellikle hayal gücüne dayalı sembolik oyunlarda OSB olan çocukların tipik gelişim gösteren çocuklarla veya diğer gelişimsel yetersizliği olan çocuklarla kıyaslandığı çalışmalar hem sembolik oyunun OSB’deki gelişimini hem de zihinsel gelişimi anlamak adına önemlidir. Bu derlemede OSB olan çocukların sembolik oyun üretimini ve bu tür oyunu anlamlandırmalarını inceleyen ampirik çalışmalara yer verilmiş, OSB’de görülen sınırlılıkların nedenlerini açıklamaya çalışan farklı görüşler tartışılmıştır. Taranan çalışmalar, OSB olan çocukların özellikle spontane/kendiliğinden sembolik oyun üretme konusunda kontrol gruplarından farklılaştığını (örn., bu oyuna daha az zaman ayırmak, daha az karmaşık oyunlar yaratmak), gözlemlenen farklılıkların kaynağının ise hem yeterlilik hem de performans ile ilişkili olabileceğini göstermektedir.
Article
Full-text available
Investigated the effect of giving 120 undergraduates the opportunity to choose the response materials they wish to learn in a paired-associate task. A paradigm was used which manipulated the amount of choice allowed during the selection procedure and the locus or point at which choice took place. Results show that locus of choice was an extremely powerful variable and that if properly manipulated, choosing only 3 responses in a 12-item list elevated performance to a level near that of a 12-item list in which all responses were chosen. Motivational mechanisms are postulated to account for these findings. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
A major problem encountered in the field of autism is the children's characteristic lack of motivation. This problem is especially apparent when autistic children attempt to complete learning tasks. The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of correct vs incorrect task completion on children's motivation to respond to such tasks. Ss were 3 autistic children aged 6 yrs 1 mo, 11 yrs 11 mo, and 12 yrs 3 mo. Results demonstrate that when Ss worked on tasks at which they were typically incorrect, their motivation for those tasks decreased to extremely low levels. However, designing treatment procedures to prompt Ss to keep responding until they completed the tasks correctly served to increase Ss' motivation to respond to those tasks. The implications of these findings are that (a) autistic children's learning handicaps (which typically lead to low levels of correct responding) may result in few or inconsistent rewards for attempting to respond at all, thus decreasing the children's motivation; and (b) treatment procedures designed to keep the children responding until they complete a task correctly may result in coincidental reinforcement for perseverance, increasing the children's motivation to respond to those tasks. Results are discussed in relation to the literature on learned helplessness. (22 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
This volume aims to provide the reader with an up-to-date account of knowledge, research, education, and clinical practice in the field of au­ tism, from an international perspective. The emphasis throughout is on the growing points of knowledge and on the new developments in prac­ tice. We have tried to keep a balance between the need for rigorous research and systematic evaluation and the importance of expressing new ideas and concepts so that they may influence thinking at a stage when questions are being formulated and fresh approaches to treatment are being developed. The book had its origins in the 1976 International Symposium on Autism held in St. Gallen, Switzerland but it is not in any sense a proceed­ ings of that meeting. Most papers have been extensively rewritten to provide a fuller coverage of the topic and also to take account of the issues raised at the meeting. Discussion dialogues have been revised and re­ structured to stand as self-contained chapters. Many significant contribu­ tions to the conference have not been induded in order to maintain the balance of a definitive review; however a few extra chapters have been added to fill crucial gaps 0 We hope the result is a vivid picture ofthe current state of the art. As editors we have been most impressed by the advances since the 1970 international conference in London.
Article
Most of the chapters in this book take for granted the definition of infantile autism and the criteria to be used in its diagnosis. That is right and proper, but the questions of definition and diagnosis have given rise to such controversy over the years that it is necessary to set the scene for what follows by some discussion of the issues involved.
Article
Much current educational literature argues that providing the learner more freedom in the learning situation enhances the learning process. This experiment tested two relevant hypotheses: (1) Ss who freely choose a task will perform better at that task than Ss who are forced to do it; and (2) Ss who freely choose a task will persist longer at that task than those who are forced to do it. The experiment employed a yoked-8 design in which the first S chose to perform any of five tasks, while the second S was forced to perform that same task. The results supported only the second hypothesis. A suggested explanation of the effects of freedom of choice as a psychological variable was presented.
Article
Twelve autistic and schizophrenic children were observed under controlled conditions of high success and high failure on the same task. Two measures of "normal" behavior and three measures of "pathological" behavior were made. "Looking" behavior was not significantly different under the two conditions but tended to be higher during failure. All measures of "pathological" behavior were significantly increased during failure. "Avoidance" behavior showed a significant conditionby-interval interaction with the behavior increasing across time during failure only. A similar trend was found for "self-stimulatory" behavior. Many children dignosed as autistic or schizophrenic may have their level of performance severely limited by brain dysfunction and the "pathological" behaviors considered characteristic of their condition may not be unique to their diagnostic category but may be a function of an unrelieved low success: failure ratio.
Article
One possible reason for the failure to demonstrate the efficacy of self contained classes for the EMR lies in the failure of such classes to balance the emphasis on motivational and cognitive variables. Several motivational variables have been isolated experimentally and the research findings have been interpreted to suggest that children who have experienced excessive amounts of failure dramatically differ from children with little history of failure on these variables. Three specific motivational variables are discussed and the related research evidence presented. The variables are (a) expectancy for failure, (b) outer-directedness, and (c) positive and negative reaction tendencies. Implications are drawn and suggestions made regarding ways of dealing with these behaviors.
Article
The study was designed to examine the effects on the performance of autistic children of varying the cognitive demands being made upon the children. The Board Form of the Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices was administered to 30 children and then, if necessary, either a range of easier, “matrices-type” problems, or the more difficult Standard Progressive Matrices. Distribution and type of errors suggested that for most of the children tested, success or failure on any item was best predicted by the intrinsic difficulty of that item rather than by the child's lack of co-operation. However, it did appear that for some of the lower functioning children, early experience of failure did interfere with subsequent performance. The implications of the findings are discussed in the context of the clinical assessment of autistic children.