ArticlePDF Available

Increased Cortical Representation of the Fingers of the Left Hand in String Players

Authors:

Abstract

Magnetic source imaging revealed that the cortical representation of the digits of the left hand of string players was larger than that in controls. The effect was smallest for the left thumb, and no such differences were observed for the representations of the right hand digits. The amount of cortical reorganization in the representation of the fingering digits was correlated with the age at which the person had begun to play. These results suggest that the representation of different parts of the body in the primary somatosensory cortex of humans depends on use and changes to conform to the current needs and experiences of the individual.
Increased Cortical Representation
of
the
Fingers of the Left Hand
in
String Players
Thomas Elbert, Christo Pantev, Christian Wienbruch,
Brigitte Rockstroh, Edward Taub
Magnetic source imaging revealed that the cortical representation of the digits of the left
hand of string players was larger than that
in
controls. The effect was smallest for the left
thumb, and no such differences were observed for the representations of the right hand
digits. The amount of cortical reorganization
in
the representation of the fingering digits
was correlated with the age at which the person had begun to play. These results suggest
that the representation of different parts of the
body
in
the primary somatosensory cortex
of humans
depends
on use and changes
to
conform
to
the current needs and experiences
of the individual.
Evidence
has
accumulated
over
the
past
rwo
decades
that
indicares
that
alterations
in
afferent
input
can
induce
plastic
reorga-
nizational
ch<mges
within
the
adulr
mam-
malian
cenrral
nervous
system
(/).
Ch<lnges
in
the
relation
between
peripheral
sensory
fields
and
their
centra
I
representations
ha
ve
been
observed
for
the
somatosensory
(2),
visual
(I,
3,
4),
and
auditory
systems
(5),
and
comparable
changes
also
have
been
found for
motor
systems
(6).
In
many
of
these
experimeonts,
the
removal
of
afferent
input
from
,1
cortical
region
resulted
in
an
"invasion"
by a
neighburing
area
whose
innervation.
remained
intact.
For
example,
the
cortical
region
representing
a
digit
be-
fore
amputation
in owl
monkeys
could
he
activated
after
amputation
by
tactile
stim-
ulation
of
an
intact
adjacent
finger
(7).
The
changes
noted
were of
the
order
of
afew
millimeters.
More
extensive
plastic
changes
have
recently
been
observed
after
the
abo-
lition
of
input
from larger
portions
of
the
body-for
example,
with
somCltosensory
deafferentation
of
an
entire
forelimb
in
ma-
caque
monkeys
(8)
and
upper
extremity
amputation
in
humans
(9-11).
In
addition,
it
has
heen
shown
in
studies
with
owl
monkeys
that
3
prolonged
increase
of
tactile
stimulation
to
the
distal
pad
of
one
or
two
phalanges
results
in
a
greatly
increased
cortical
representation
speciflc
to
that
portion
of
the
fingers
(12,
13) Evi-
dence
has
also
been
reported
that
suggests
an
increased
cortical
representation
of
the
index
flnger
used in
reading
by
blind
Braille
readers
(14).
Violinists
and
other
string
players pro-
vide
a
good
model
for
the
study
of
the
effects
of
differential
afferent
input
to
the
two
sides
of
the
brain
in
humans.
During
their
pnlCtice
or
performance,
the
second
to
the
fifth digits
(D2
to
05)
of
the
left
hand
are
continuously
engaged
in
fingering
the
strings, atask
that
involves
considerable
manual
dexterity
and
eonhanced
sensory
stimulation.
At
theo
same
time,
theo
thumb
grasps
the
neck
of
the
instrument
and,
al-
though
not
as
active
8S
the
fingers, engages
in
relatively
frequent
small
shifts
of
position
and
pressure.
The
right
h.and,
which
manip-
ulates
th.eo
bow,
participates
in atask
involv-
ing
much
less
individual
finger
movement
and
fluctuation
in
tactile
and
pressure in-
put.
Here,
we
present
data
from
magnetic
source
imaging
that
indicates
that
the
cereo-
bral
cortices
of
string
players
3reo
different
from
the
cortices
of
controls
in
that
the
representation
of
the
digits
of
the
left
hand
is
substantially
enlarged
in
the
cortices
of
string
players.
Nine
musicians
(six violinists,
two
cel-
lists,
and
one
guitarist)
who
had
played
their
instruments
for a
mean
pcoriod
of
11.7
years
(range,
7
to
17 years) served as sub-
jects
for
our
study.
Six
nonmusicians
served
as
controls
(/5).
The
mean
age for
bm
l,
groups was 24
:!:
3
yecltS.
Before
our
im',
tig,ltion,
the
musicians
kept
adiary for 1
week,
recording
the
amount
of
time
prac-
ticed
per
day
(mean
9.8
:!:
8.4
hours
per
week),
and
had
estimated
the
amount
of
ti
me
spent
practicing
during
the
preovious
month
and
year
(l0.8
:!:
8.8
hours
per
week)
During
theo
experimental
session, so-
matosensory
stimulation
was
delivered
to
the
first
digit
and,
in
separate
runs, to
the
fifrh digit
of
either
hand.
Stimulation
con-
slsted
of
light
superficial
pressure
applied
by
means
of
a
pneumatic
stimulator
with
the
use
of
standard,
nonpainful
stimulation
in-
tensity
(9,
16, 17)
The
daw
(Fig 1)
indi-
cate
that
the
center
of
cortical
rcsponsivity
for
tactile
stimulation
of
the
digits
of
the
left
hand
was
shifted
in
musicians
as
com-
pared
to
that
in
controls,
while
at
the
same
time
the
strength
of
response
increased.
The
topographic
shift
was
toward
the
mi,J
sagittal
plane,
which,
along
the
surface
th~
postcentral
gyrus,
is
toward
the
region
30
B
25
••
..
String players
20
15
..
10 @
I
5
O+-~'--'--",-"'-",-"'-r-..---,-
o0.5 11.5 22.5
Distance,
01
to
05,
left
hand (cm)
Controls
.~
.
~@
@
,.
String players
E3C
~
-g
2.5
'"
.J::
:g,
2
.;:
~~
1.5
.8
Ei
2f
0.5
c:
~
'"
is
E
~
.s
.J::
c,
c:
e
U;
III
(5
C.
1J
'"
Cl Controls
O-j-.,...~..,--~..........,~~...".-~~
o510 15
20
Age at
inception
of
musical
practice
Fig.
1.
(A)
Equivalent current dipoles elicited
by
stimulation
of
the thumb (01)
and
fifth
finger (05)
of
the
left
hand are superimposed onto an
MRI
(magnetic
resonance imaging) reconstruction
of
the cerebral cortex
of
acontrol, who
was selected
to
provide anatomical landmarks
for
the interpretation
of
the
MEG-based localization. The arrows represent the location and orientation
of
the
ECO
vector
for
each
of
the two digits averaged
across
musicians (black)
and controls
(yellow).
The length
of
the arrows represents the mean magni-
tude
of
the dipole moment
for
the two digits
in
each
group. The average
locations
of
05
and
01
are shifted medially
for
the string players compared
to
controls; the
shift
is
larger
for
05
than
for
01.
The dipole moment
is
also larger
for
the musicians'
05,
as
indicated by the greater magnitude of the black
arrow. (8) The magnitude
of
the dipole moment
as
afunction of the
age
of
inception
of
musical practice; string players are indicated
by
filled
circles,
control subjects by hatched circles. Note the larger dipole moment
for
indi-
viduals beginning musical practice before the
age
of
12.
(C)
Scatterplot
of
the
Euclidean distances
(in
centimeters) between the cortical representations
of
01 and 05. This distance
for
the musicians'
left
hands was greater than that
in
controls, but this difference
is
not statistically significant.
SCIENCE VOL.
270
13
OCTOBER J
995
305
Konstanzer Online-Publikations-System (KOPS)
URL: http://www.ub.uni-konstanz.de/kops/volltexte/2008/6428/
URN: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-64286
First publ. in: Science 270 (1995), pp. 305-307
11
of
the
cortex
that
represents
the
palm
of
the
hand
(18).
For
OS
(little
finger)
of
the
left
hand,
the
shift
was 0.7
cm
(t =3.6, P<
0.01, degrees
of
freedom =13); for
01
(thumb),
the
shift was
0.5
cm
(t =3.3, P<
0.0l).
This
shift
of
OS
was significantly
greater
than
the
shift
of
01.
Correspondingly,
the
analysis
of
vari-
ance
(ANOVA),
which
included
data
from
both
hemispheres
(18),
showed
an
interac-
tion
of
group (a factor
in
ANOVA)
and
digit
[F(l,13)
=4.78, P<0.05].
This
effect
was
dependent
on
the
side
on
which
stim-
ulation
was given:
There
were
no
signifi-
cant
shifts for
the
digits
of
the
right
hand
of
the
musicians
compared
to
those
of
the
contr,)ls.
The
dipole
moment,
which
is
pre-
sumed
to
be
an
index
of
total
neuronal
~ctivity,
also increased for
the
stimulation
o'-..J
the
digits
of
the
left
hand
of
musicians
compared
to
the
left
hand
of
controls
[ANOV
A
interaction
of
group
and
side
of
stimulation,
F(l,
13) =5.54, P<0.05j.
The
increase was larger for
OS
(t =5.4, P<
0.0l)
than
for
Dl
[t
=2.0, P<0.1;
ANOV
A
interaction
of
group
and
digit,
F(l,13)
=
4.81,
P<
0.05]
(19).
There
was a
correlation
between
the
age
at
which
the
string
players began studying
their
instruments
and
the
magnitude
of
the
change
in
the
dipole
moment
of
05
com-
pared
to
that
in
controls
(r =0.79, r=
0.0l)
(Fig.
lB).
The
relation
between
the
amount
of
practice
and
the
cortical
mea-
sures was
not
significant.
The
increase
of
the
dipole
moment
of
both
01
and
05
in
the
string players indicates
that
an
extend-
ed
cortical
network
responds
to
tactile stim-
ulation.
If
the
active
area expands,
the
point
location
determined
by
an
equivalent
"Dole
model
will shift inward.
Given
the
~erical
geometry
of
the
head,
measure-
ments
of
the
01
and
05
representations
would
approach
each
other
if
the
center
of
activity
remained
unchanged.
Conversely,
an
increase in area of
representation
would
produce
opposing
effects
on
the
measured
cortical
distance
between
01
and
D5,
which
could
explain
the
absence
of
asig-
nificant
group difference
in
the
measured
distance
between
cortical
representations
of
01
and
05
(Fig. 1
C).
Further
experiments
should
attempt
tl)
model
the
size
of
the
activated
area
to
resolve this
question.
The
significant shift in medial
direction
of
the
cortical
representation
of
the
fingers
of
the
left
hand
in
string
players
and
the
increase in
the
corresponding
dipole mo-
ments
suggest
that
the
cortical
territory oc-
T.
Elbert and
B.
Rockstroh, Department of Psychology,
University of Konstanz, 0-78434 Konstanz, Germany.
E-mail: Thomas.Elbert@Unl-Konstanz.de
C.
Pantev and
C.
Wienbruch, Biomagnetism Center, Uni-
versity of Munster,
0-48129
Munster, Germany.
E.
Taub, Department of Psychology, University of Ala-
bama
at
Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294, USA.
306
cupied
by
the
representation
of
the
digits
increased
in
string players
as
compared
with
that
in controls.
Two
alternate
interpreta-
tions
of
our
data
sh'luld
be
taken
into
ac-
count.
First, it
could
be
argued
that
individ-
uals
with
agenetically
determined
large
representatil11l
of
the
left
hand
digits make
superior
string
players
and
therefore
are
more
likely to
continue
with
musical
train-
ing
once
they
have
begun. However,
in
research
with
animals,
use-dependent
en-
largements
of
portions
of
the
somatosensory
map
in
cortical
area
3b
have
been
clearly
demonstrated
under
conditions
of
increased
use generally similar
to
those
in
this
study
(12, 13).
In
either
case,
the
relatively larger
representations
of
individually
important
digits could
have
the
role
of
enhancing
the
particular
needs
of
astring player
in
an
adaptive
manner.
Asecond
alternative
explanation
of
our
results
is
that
they are aconsequence
of
a
shift
in
cortical responsivity
combined
with
an
intensification
of
the
response. However,
we
think
amore plausible
explanation
is
that
the
c,)[tical territory
of
the
left-hand digits
has expanded.
This
is
more plausible because
(i)
there
is
a
correlation
between
amount
of
cortical reorganization
and
age [or stage
of
central
nervous system
(CNS)
maturation]
at
which
musical practice
began
and
(ii)
the
equivalent
current
dipole
(ECD)
shift fol-
lows
the
one
direction
that
is
consistent
with
the
expansion
interpretation.
Related work (10) has shown
that
there
is
astrong correlation
in
humans with upper
extremities amputated between
extent
of
cor-
tical rellrganization
and
amount
of
phantom
limb
pain
experienced.
Although
phantom
limb
pain
is
amaladaptive result of nervous
system injury, our results demonstrate the
functional relevance
of
cortical reorganiza-
tion, similar
to
results reported
in
the
context
of
the visual sysrem (3)
and
auditory system
(12,
20,
21
).
One
may speculate
that
one
role
of cortical reorganization might be
to
contrib-
ute to
the
functional recovery
of
organisms
after
CNS
damage, possibly in terms
of
recov-
ery from
CNS
shock.
The
evolutionary ad-
vantage
of
this mechanism
is
brought into
question, however, by
the
fact
that
the
process
of recovery
is
usually slow
and
thus would
not
permit
an
organism
that
was seriously im-
paired to survive long enough
to
engage in
sllccessful reproduction
and
transmission
of
this capacity (22).
However,
in accord
with
the
results
of
Merzenich
and
co-workers (7),
continuous
plastic reorganization
of
cortical
space
that
permits
rapid
reallocation
of
available
CNS
circuitry would
confer
an
obvious
practical
advantage.
The
possible
contribution
of
cortical
reorganization
to
recovery
of
func-
tkm
after
CNS
injury
might
thus
be
an
adventitious
result
that
"piggy
backed"
onto
a
mechanism,
which
permitted
the
SCIENCE
VOL.
270
13
OcrOBER
1995
much
more critically
important
plastic pro-
cesses associated
with
learning, physical
growth
during
maturation,
and
adjustment
to
current
environmental
demalllk
The
role
of
this
mechanism
in
the
recovery
of
function
would
become
impl)rtant
only
when
protecting
an
individual
with
CNS
damage would artificially
prolong
survival
long
enough
for
extensive
cortical
reorga-
nization
to
work.
REFERENCES AND NOTES
1.
J.
H.
Kaas, Annu.
Rev.
Neurosci.
14,137
(1991).
2.
A.
Mogilner
et
al., Proc. Nat!. Acad. Sci.
USA
90,
3593 (1993); M.
M.
Merzenich
et
al
.. Neuroscience
8,33
(1983);
S.
A.
Clark.
T.
Allard,
W.
M.
Jenkins,
M. M.
Merzenich, Nature 332,
444
(1988); J.
Kalaska and
B.
Pomeranz, J. Neurophysiol. 42,
618
(1979);
D. D.
Rasmusson, J. Comp. Neural.
205,313
(1982);
A.
M.
Kellohan and G.
S.
Doetsch,
Somatosens
Res.
2,49
(1984);
J.
T.
Wall and
C.
G.
Cusick,
J.
Neurosci.
4,
1499(1984); J.
T.
Wall etal.,
Ibid.
6,218
(1986).
3.
C.
Darian-Smith and
C.
O.
Gilbert, Nature 368, 737
(1994).
4.
C.
D.
Gilbert and
T.
N.
Wiesel, ibid. 356. 150 (1992);
F.
Rosier,
B.
Rbder,
M.
Heil,
E.
Henninghausen, Cog-
nit. Brain
Res.
1, 145 (1993).
5.
G.
H.
Recanzone,
C.
E.
Schreiner,
M. M.
Merzenich,
J. Neurosci
13,87
(1993);
D.
Robertson and
O.
R.
F.
Irvine, J. Comp. Neurol. 282, 456 (1989);
D.
R.
F.
Irvine,
R.
Rajan,
L.
Z.
Wize,
P.
Heil, Soc. Neurosci.
Abstr. 17, 1485 (1991).
6.
L.
G.
Cohen,
S.
Bandinelli,
T.
W. Frindley,
M.
Hallet,
Brain 114, 615 (1991);
J.
N.
Sanes,
S.
Sun
er,
J.
F.
Lando,
J.
PDonaghue, Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci.
USA
85, 2003 (1988).
7.
M. M.
Merzenich et al., J. Comp. Neurol.
224,591
(1984).
8
T.
P.
Pons et al., Science 252, 1857 (1991).
9.
T.
Elbert
et
al., Neurarepol1
5;
2593 (1994).
10.
H.
Flor
et
al., Nature 375, 482 (1995)
11.
T.
T.
Yang
et
aI., Neurorepol15,
701
(1994).
12. G.
H.
Recanzone,
M.
M.
Merzenich,
W.
M.
Jenkins,
A.
Grajski,
H.
R.
Dinse, J. Neurophysiol. 67,
1031
(1992)
13. W.
M.
Jenkins,
M.
M.
Merzenich,
M.
T.
Ochs,
T.
Allard,
E.
Guic-Robles, ibid. 63, 82 (1990).
14.
A.
Pascual-Leone and
F.
Torres, Brain 116, 39
(1993).
15. Controls were either students or university employ-
ees who worked
in
an
academic environment and
had no experience playing string instruments. They
were selected to be comparable
to
the experimental
groupwith respect
to
age, social status, and gender.
After giving informed consent, experimental partici-
pants were briefly interviewed concerning their mu-
sical activities and their history of practicing. Experi-
mental participants were also interviewed
to
elicit
information regarding their usual manual activities:
None ofthe controls engaged
in
manual tasks to
an
unusual extent oUler than typing on computer key-
boards. Each of them used both hands for word
processing orothercomputeroperations. Two
of
the
rnusicians were found to have atendency toward
ambidexterity, but
all
other test participants were
right-handed (Edinburgh handedness questionnaire)
[R.
C.
Oldfield. Neuropsychologia
9,
97
(1971)J.
16.
T.
T.
Yang,
C. C.
Gallen,
B.
J. Schwartz,
F.
E.
Bloorn,
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA.
90,3089
(1993).
17. ABTI-Magnes system was used for MEG (magneto-
encephalographic) recordings. The sensor array was
positioned over one of the two hemispheres (C3 or
C4)
in
afixed irregular order. At each site, 1000
stimuli were delivered at
an
average rate of 0.5 Hz
(the interval between stimulus onsets was 500
:'::
50
rns).
Stimulation site sequence vaned according to a
fixed irregular ordm across test participants. Within
the range of 30
to
75 ms, afirst major peak was
identified
in
each of the evoked wave forms. The
mean latencies were 46.2:':: 7.9 ms for left
01,
and
48.1
:'::
7.2 ms for right
01;
for left 05, the mean
latency was 52.2
:+:
8.3 ms, and for right
05
52.8
:+:
7.5 ms. The difference between musicians and con-
trols for latency was not significant. For each evoked
magnetic field, asingle
EGO
model (best fitting local
sphere) was fitted and the medians of the dipole
moment and
the
dipole location were computed
from aselection of points within a20-ms time seg-
ment
(11
sampling points) around the maximal rms
(root mean square across the 37 channels) within the
range of 30
to
75 ms.
POints
were selected if they
met the
follOWing
requirements:
(i)
rms indicating a
signal-to-noise ratio
>3;
(ii)
agoodness of fit of the
EGO
model to the measured field
>0.95;
and
(Iii)
a
minimal confidence volume of the
EGO
location
<300
mm'J,
18. For
all
cortical measures,
an
ANOVA with the be-
tween-subJect factor group (musicians versus con-
trols) and the within-subject factors digit
(01
versus
05) and side of stimulation (left versus right) was
computed first. ANOVAs for subsets ofthe data
or
t
tests were used to resolve interactions.
19.
Given aconstant direction of the equivalent current
dipole, the dipole moment indicates the total
strength of cortical
polarization-that
is,
tile number
of neurons involved during acortical response.
If
this
number increases,
the
dipole moment also increas-
es.
Any active focal area can be modeled by
an
equivalent current dipole. Each dendritic currentflow
contributes
to
this dipole moment according to the
formula
dipole moment =(conductivity)
x(cross section
of
the
dendrite)
x(potential difference along
the
dendrite)
S. J. Williamson and
L.
Kaufman
[in
Auditory
Evoked
Magnetic
Fields
and
Electric Potentials,
F.
Grandori,
M. Hoke,
G.
L.
Romani, Eds. (Karger, Basel, 1990),
pp.
1-39]
assume the diameter of
an
apical dendrite
to
be 4
fCm,
the intracellular conductivity to be about
0.25
S/m,
and the potential difference
to
be about 10
mV.
With the use of these assumptions, about
30,000 dendrites would be necessary to produce a
dipole moment of 10 nA-m
If
conductivity and po-
tential difference are not different
in
musicians and
controls, the magnification of the dipole moment
in
response
to
finger stimulation ofthe left hand
in
mu-
sicians can be explained
if
approximately twice as
many cells were activated
in
musicians than were
activated in the controls.
20. G.
H.
Recanzone, M. M. Merzenich, J. Schreiner,
J.
Neurophysiol. 67, 1071 (1992)
21.
N.
M.
Welnberger
et
al.,
Concepts
Neurosci. 1,
91
(1990)
22. We are indebted
to
T.
Pons for this observation.
23. We appreciate the assistance of
S.
Hampson,
B.
Lutkenh6ner, and
O.
Steinstrater. Supported by the
Oeutsche Forschungsgemeinscllaft.
25 May 1995; accepted 13 September 1995
r
..
",
SCIENCE
VOL.
270
13
OCTOBER 1995
,/
307
... Specifically, the intense training of extremely refined somatosensory integration that is professional musical practice has been shown to induce structural and functional neuroplastic alterations. For instance, musicians have been shown to have an enhanced representation of their playing fingers in the somatosensory cortices (9) as well as having an increased functional connectivity of insula-based networks (10). Both, the insula and the somatosensory cortices are part of the brain network responsible for pain processing (11). ...
... We suspect that the N 2 latency alteration in musicians indicates a neuroplastic adaptation of the nociceptive system, which can indeed be induced by musical training (9,10,25,26). Likely due to their higher sensitivity to pain and to mechanical stimuli (9,27), musicians experience pain more often during their career, as is evidenced by their high prevalence of pain syndromes (2)(3)(4). At the same time, we argue that professional musicians often endogenously inhibit their pain, for example to keep practicing despite having pain to be able to compete in their stressful and highly competitive environment (28). ...
... We suspect that the N 2 latency alteration in musicians indicates a neuroplastic adaptation of the nociceptive system, which can indeed be induced by musical training (9,10,25,26). Likely due to their higher sensitivity to pain and to mechanical stimuli (9,27), musicians experience pain more often during their career, as is evidenced by their high prevalence of pain syndromes (2)(3)(4). At the same time, we argue that professional musicians often endogenously inhibit their pain, for example to keep practicing despite having pain to be able to compete in their stressful and highly competitive environment (28). ...
Article
Full-text available
Classical musical training requires extreme levels of fine motor control, resulting in adaptive neuroplastic alterations in professional musicians. Additionally, musicians have a high prevalence of pain syndromes, which makes them an interesting group to research the influence of neuroplasticity on nociception. This report consists of two parts. Firstly, we present the results of a preliminary study comparing musicians and non-musicians with respect to their cortical responses to noxious heat stimuli at their hands and feet, using contact heat evoked potentials (CHEPs). Secondly, we quantitatively discuss the influence of the heating rates of two different stimulation devices on CHEPs when applying the exact same settings. For this, we measured the temperature curves of the devices’ stimuli and connected their respective heating rates to the resulting CHEPs. Musicians showed a significantly larger N 2 latency difference between hands and feet (20.86 ms, p = 0.0045 ), compared to non-musicians. Additionally, we found that, despite the exact same settings, different stimulation devices produced considerably different temperature curves. The resulting time difference between the stimulation devices of 104.78 ms explains the latency difference of the CHEPs produced by the respective device of 104.09 ms extremely well. This study underlines that musicians are an interesting model for neuroplasticity regarding nociception, as they respond differently to nociceptive stimuli. Moreover, it contributes to the understanding of the connection between a stimulation device’s heating rate and the resulting CHEPs, an important finding that has never been quantified before but has considerable consequences on the comparability of results.
... Four healthy control male children participated in the study, aged 7-17 years, ambidextrous, from a well socioeconomic background belonging to upper middle class, scored between 16 and 25 according to the modified Kuppuswamy socioeconomic scale, 12 with at least one person, either father or mother, as main source of income in the family and a stable occupation, having their children enrolled at a school. ...
... 15 The brain reorganisation process is generally adaptive, with the extent of changes determined by multiple factors like age, usage, needs, experience of an individual and during trauma, the extent and severity of damage. 16,17 Understanding of these plastic changes following injury may lead to better ways of facilitating reorganisations that are beneficial and preventing those with undesirable outcomes. In healthy adults, both manual and robotic neural navigated methods have been used for mapping the motor cortex. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Single-pulse paradigms of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) have been used to understand cortical excitability and reorganisation. Purpose This study aims to develop a protocol for mapping of the primary motor cortex (M1) using single-pulse TMS in children. Methods A 11×11 manual grid was created around the hotspot of abductor pollicis brevis (APB). Motor-evoked potential (MEP) amplitude was recorded from 41 sites around the hotspot of APB and analysed. Results Maximum MEP amplitude was recorded from the hotspot that reduced as distance increased. Heat maps generated were consistent among the participants. Conclusion In children, 41 single-pulse TMS stimulations with interstimulus interval of two seconds provide reliable information about motor maps. This procedure will aid in understanding cortical reorganisation in children with neurodevelopmental disorders.
... Motor practice aimed at executing problematic daily tasks more productively, combined with challenging components (increasing task difficulty) and repetition of desired movement, can drive plasticity. The progression of this practice is key, starting with lower difficulty and progressing to higher as the movement is practiced productively [52] through repetition and task difficulty in terms of kinematics and kinetics [53,54]. It's well known that personalized task difficulty training yields superior learning outcomes compared to fixed difficulty increases [55]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative condition that impacts motor planning and control of the upper limbs (UL) and leads to cognitive impairments. Rehabilitation approaches, including motor imagery (MI) and action observation (AO), along with the use of brain-machine interfaces (BMI), are essential in the PD population to enhance neuroplasticity and mitigate symptoms. Objective To provide a description of a rehabilitation protocol for evaluating the effects of isolated and combined applications of MI and action observation (AO), along with BMI, on upper limb (UL) motor changes and cognitive function in PD. Methods This study provides a detailed protocol for a single-blinded, randomized clinical trial. After selection, participants will be randomly assigned to one of five experimental groups. Each participant will be assessed at three points: pre-intervention, post-intervention, and at a follow-up four weeks after the intervention ends. The intervention consists of 10 sessions, each lasting approximately 60 minutes. Expected results The primary outcome expected is an improvement in the Test d’Évaluation des Membres Supérieurs de Personnes Âgées score, accompanied by a reduction in task execution time. Secondary outcomes include motor symptoms in the upper limbs, assessed via the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale - Part III and the 9-Hole Peg Test; cognitive function, assessed with the PD Cognitive Rating Scale; and occupational performance, assessed with the Canadian Occupational Performance Measure. Discussion This study protocol is notable for its intensive daily sessions. Both MI and AO are low-cost, enabling personalized interventions that physiotherapists and occupational therapists can readily replicate in practice. While BMI use does require professionals to acquire an exoskeleton, the protocol ensures the distinctiveness of the interventions and, to our knowledge, is the first to involve individuals with PD. Trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05696925.
... A set of pressure and heat pain thresholds were performed on the pad of the middle finger on the right hand to avoid the calluses on the musicians' fingertips, and to minimise variability arising from handedness and instrument-specific somatosensory adaptations, as prior studies have shown that handedness and instrument training influence tactile and pain sensitivity Elbert et al. 1995). Additionally, to control for potential confounding factors related to enhanced tactile perception, we included assessments of mechanical and electrical detection thresholds. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The integration of concurrent endogenous and exogenous multisensory information throughout years of dedicated sensorimotor training is associated with enhanced interoceptive accuracy and altered pain perception in healthy individuals. However, this relationship remains inconclusive, with outcomes varying by training modality and pain stimulus. This study examines associations between distinct forms of sensorimotor training, interoception and pain perception. Methods Two groups of individuals performing extensive sensorimotor training, 17 musicians and 15 athletes, and 14 non‐musicians/athletes were recruited. Participants completed a cardiac interoceptive accuracy (IAcc) task and quantitative sensory tests, including mechanical and electrical detection thresholds (MDTs and EDTs), pressure and heat pain thresholds (PPTs and HPTs), as well as music‐related perceptual discrimination and self‐reported physical activity assessments. Results Results revealed superior IAcc and PPTs in athletes compared to controls. Musicians exhibited increased heat pain sensitivity. While IAcc in musicians did not reach significance, training duration significantly predicted IAcc across both groups. PPTs correlated positively with both IAcc and accumulated training, but mediation analyses revealed that training effects on PPTs occurred independently of IAcc, suggesting distinct pathways for interoception and pain modulation. Additionally, physical activity levels correlated positively with both IAcc and PPTs across participants. Conclusions These findings support the emerging view that individuals engaging in sensorimotor training routines, which require embodied multisensory integration for optimal performance, enhance interoceptive accuracy. They also confirm that pain processing varies by training modality. Furthermore, they suggest that the type of acute pain stimulus may explain inconsistencies in the interoception–pain relationship in healthy populations. Significance Statement This study advances our understanding of the interoception–pain‐training nexus by revealing two distinct pathways: one linking sensorimotor training, interoceptive accuracy and pressure pain perception and another showing that accumulated sensorimotor training independently elevates pain thresholds. By differentiating between pain modalities, the findings contribute to resolving previous contradictory results, refine our insights into interoception in healthy populations and inform about potential clinical interventions.
Chapter
V monografiji Širjenje obzorij o poučevanju glasbe so študenti magistrskega študijskega programa Razredni pouk na Pedagoški fakulteti Univerze v Mariboru proučevali sodobna spoznanja o poučevanju glasbe, zlasti na osnovnošolski stopnji. Monografija, namenjena učiteljem in študentom, ponuja teoretične vpoglede v številne ključne glasbene koncepte in njihovo praktično aplikacijo v pedagoškem delu. Obravnavani so različni vidiki glasbenega poučevanja, razvoj glasbenega mišljenja, vključevanje petja in igranja ter številne teme, ki spodbujajo razvoj pedagoških spretnosti. Izzivi so vključevali pravilno uporabo digitalnih orodij, ki temeljijo na umetni inteligenci, kot sta Elicit in ChatGPT, ki so spremenila tradicionalne raziskovalne pristope. Kljub prednostim teh tehnologij so študenti morali zagotoviti avtentičnost in izvirnost svojih raziskav, obvladovanje etične uporabe tehnologij in ohranjanje akademske integritete pri citiranju virov. Monografija na koncu ponuja nabor vprašanj za refleksijo, ki bodo študentom in učiteljem razrednega pouka pomagali ozavestiti, izboljšati in načrtovati izboljšave njihove pedagoške prakse pri poučevanju glasbene umetnosti v osnovnih šolah.
Article
We analyzed the movement durations of 14 expert and 18 nonexpert string players as they prepared to play their instruments and in three other physical tasks unrelated to music making. We hypothesized that expert musicians would take more time to prepare their playing than nonexperts, but we found this not to be the case. There were no significant differences in movement duration means between experts and nonexperts in any of the four tasks including the Instrument Task. Surprisingly and somewhat inexplicably, we found that both expert and nonexpert musicians who participated in sports activities ( n = 17) took significantly more time to prepare their playing than did the other participants. Further inspection of the video recordings revealed important differences in how experts’ and nonexperts’ movements unfolded in the Instrument Task. Nonexperts’ movements tended to be uneven and disjunct, whereas experts’ movements were fluid and even from the start, suggesting that experts’ conceptions of “starting a note” begin prior to the onset of movement and not when the bow is in close proximity to the string.
Article
Full-text available
It was hypothesized that a specialized gait recognition skill enables humans to distinguish the gait of familiar from unfamiliar individuals, and that this may have relevance in team sports. Runners seen for less than half a second can be classified as teammates or not by adult players, so it may be asked whether this skill would also be demonstrated by young team players. In the current study, junior football players (M age=10.0 yr., SD = 0.8, N =13) viewed 400-msec. video clips of runners sprinting past a fixed forward facing digital video camera and similarly showed teammate recognition scores significantly above chance. Given the variation among the junior players in this skill, it seems possible for researchers to assess whether improvement can be obtained with structured training for young team players, where running teammates are seen in peripheral vision during training drills.
Article
Full-text available
The “Mozart effect” is the tendency to score higher on spatiotemporal IQ subscales following exposure to complex music such as Mozart's Sonata K.448. This phenomenon was investigated in 20 musicians and 20 nonmusicians. The trion model predicts increased synchrony between musical and spatiotemporal centres in the right cerebral hemisphere. Since increased left-hemispheric involvement in music processing occurs as a result of music training, predictions deriving from the possibility of increased synchrony with left-hemispheric areas in musicians were tested. These included improved performance on language as well as spatiotemporal tasks. Spatiotemporal, synonym generation, and rhyming word generation tasks were employed as was the Mozart Sonata K.448. A Mozart effect was demonstrated on the spatiotemporal task, and the facilitatory effect of exposure to Mozart was greater for the non-musician group. This finding adds to the robustness of the Mozart effect since novel tasks were used. No Mozart effect was found for either group on the verbal tasks, although the musicians scored higher on rhyming word generation. This new finding adds to the number of nonmusical tasks apparently showing long-term benefits from music training. However, no systematic link was found between performance on any task and number of years spent in music training. The failure to induce a Mozart effect in the musician group on verbal tasks, as well as that group's limited facilitation on spatiotemporal tasks, may be associated with either a ceiling effect due to the long-term effects of music training or from methodological factors. Both possibilities are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
While professional musicians are generally considered to possess better control of finger movements than nonmusicians, relatively few reports have experimentally addressed the nature of this discrepancy in fine motor skills. For example, it is unknown whether musicians perform with greater skill than control subjects in all aspects of different types of fine motor activities. More specifically, it is not known whether musicians perform better than control subjects on a fine motor task that is similar, but not identical, to the playing of their primary instrument. The purpose of this study was to examine the accuracy of finger placement and accuracy of timing in professional musicians and nonmusicians using a simple, rhythmical, bilateral fingering pattern and the technology that allowed separate assessment of these two parameters. Professional musicians (other than pianists) and nonmusicians were given identical, detailed and explicit instructions but not allowed physically to practice the finger pattern. After verbally repeating the correct pattern for the investigator, subjects performed the task on an electric keyboard with both hands simultaneously. Each subject's performance was then converted to a numerical score. While musicians clearly demonstrated better accuracy in timing, no significant difference was found between the groups in their finger placement scores. These findings were not correlated with subjects' age, sex, limb dominance, or primary instrument (for the professional musicians). This study indicates that professional musicians perform better in timing accuracy but not spatial accuracy while executing a simple, novel, bimanual motor sequence.
Article
Full-text available
1. Adult owl monkeys were trained to detect differences in the frequency of a tactile flutter-vibration stimulus above a 20-Hz standard. All stimuli were delivered to a constant skin site restricted to a small part of a segment of one finger. The frequency-difference discrimination performance of all but one of these monkeys improved progressively with training. 2. The distributed responses of cortical neurons ("maps") of the hand surfaces were defined in detail in somatosensory cortical area 3b. Representations of trained hands were compared with those of the opposite, untrained hand, and to the area 3b representations of hands in a second set of monkeys that were stimulated tactually in the same manner while these monkeys were attending to auditory stimuli (passive stimulation controls). 3. The cortical representations of the trained hands were substantially more complex in topographic detail than the representations of unstimulated hands or of passively stimulated control hands. 4. In all well-trained monkeys the representations of the restricted skin location trained in the behavioral task were significantly (1.5 to greater than 3 times) greater in area than were the representations of equivalent skin locations on control digits. However, the overall extents of the representations of behaviorally stimulated fingers were not larger than those of control fingers in the same hemisphere, or in opposite hemisphere controls. 5. The receptive fields representing the trained skin were significantly larger than receptive fields representing control digits in all but one trained monkey. The largest receptive fields were centered in the zone of representation of the behaviorally engaged skin, but they were not limited to it. Large receptive fields were recorded in a 1- to 2-mm-wide zone in the area 3b maps of trained hands. 6. Receptive-field sizes were also statistically significantly larger on at least one adjacent, untrained digit when compared with the receptive fields recorded on the homologous digit of the opposite hand. 7. There was an increase in the percent overlaps of receptive fields in the cortical zone of representation of the trained skin. A significant number of receptive fields were centered on the behaviorally trained skin site. 8. The effects of increased topographic complexity, increased representation of the trained skin location, increased receptive-field size, and increased receptive-field overlap were not observed in the representations of the untrained hands in these same monkeys. Only modest increases in topographic complexity were recorded in the representations of passively stimulated hands, and no effects on receptive-field size or overlap were noted.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Article
Full-text available
After limited sensory deafferentations in adult primates, somatosensory cortical maps reorganize over a distance of 1 to 2 millimeters mediolaterally, that is, in the dimension along which different body parts are represented. This amount of reorganization was considered to be an upper limit imposed by the size of the projection zones of individual thalamocortical axons, which typically also extend a mediolateral distance of 1 to 2 millimeters. However, after extensive long-term deafferentations in adult primates, changes in cortical maps were found to be an order of magnitude greater than those previously described. These results show the need for a reevaluation of both the upper limit of cortical reorganization in adult primates and the mechanisms responsible for it.
Article
Full-text available
The potential for peripheral nerve injury to reorganize motor cortical representations was investigated in adult rats. Maps reflecting functional connections between the motor cortex and somatic musculature were generated with intracortical electrical stimulation techniques. Comparison of cortical somatotopic maps obtained in normal rats with maps generated from rats with a facial nerve lesion indicated that the forelimb and eye/eyelid representations expanded into the normal vibrissa area. Repeated testing from an electrode placed chronically in the motor cortex showed a shift from vibrissa to forelimb within hours after facial nerve transection. These comparatively quick changes in motor cortex representation pattern suggest that synaptic relations between motor cortex and somatic musculature are continually reshaped in adult mammals.
Article
We studied the organization of the somatosensory cortex in proficient Braille readers, recording somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in 10 subjects and using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in five subjects, and compared the results with those of 15 control subjects. Somatosensory evoked potentials were elicited by a focal electrical stimulus to the tip of the index finger and recorded from a contralateral 4×4 grid of scalp electrodes centred around C3' or C4'. Transcranial magnetic stimulation, with an 8-shaped coil centred over the same scalp positions, was delivered simultaneously with, and at different intervals after, the finger stimulus. The results of the right index (reading) finger in Braille readers were compared with those of their left index (non-reading) finger and of the right and left index fingers of the control subjects. The scalp areas from which we recorded N20 and P22 components of the SEP with an amplitude of at least 70% of the maximal amplitude recorded in each trial were significantly larger in SEPs evoked from the reading fingers. Detection of the stimulus applied to the reading finger was blocked by TMS delivered over a larger contralateral scalp area and during a longer time window after the stimulus. These experiments suggest that reading Braille is associated with expansion of the sensorimotor cortical representation of the reading finger.
Article
1. In normal kittens and cats, cells in a region of primary somatosensory cortex (SI) responded exclusively to input from the contralateral front paw; we called this area paw cortex (PC). A neighboring region of SI responded to input from the contralateral forearm above the wrist; we called this area forearm cortex (FC). The centers of PC and FC were about 4 mm apart. 2. In kittens several weeks after transection of the nerves to the front paw, the following changes were observed in PC: a) 52% of PC cells had receptive fields on the forearm; normally, PC cells responded to natural stimulation only of the front paw; b) many cells in PC (58%) responded to electrical stimulation of the medial cutaneous nerve from the forearm; normally, very few PC cells (9%) responded to this nerve; c) there was a 370% increase in the median amplitude of medial cutaneous-evoked potentials in PC; d) in contrast to these enhanced inputs, PC responses to ulnar nerve stimulation decreased significantly. 3. In adult cats, paw denervation initiated a similar process as in kittens, but with less marked somatotopic changes. 4. In both kittens and adults, FC was unaffected by the nerve injuries. 5. We conclude that a chronic peripheral nerve injury can produce extensive changes in SI cortex somatotopic organization; the nature of the effect is age dependent.
Article
1. Temporal response characteristics of neurons were sampled in fine spatial grain throughout the hand representations in cortical areas 3a and 3b in adult owl monkeys. These monkeys had been trained to detect small differences in tactile stimulus frequencies in the range of 20-30 Hz. Stimuli were presented to an invariant, restricted spot on a single digit. 2. The absolute numbers of cortical locations and the cortical area over which neurons showed entrained frequency-following responses to behaviorally important stimuli were significantly greater when stimulation was applied to the trained skin, as compared with stimulation on an adjacent control digit, or at corresponding skin sites in passively stimulated control animals. 3. Representational maps defined with sinusoidal stimuli were not identical to maps defined with just-visible tapping stimuli. Receptive-field/frequency-following response site mismatches were recorded in every trained monkey. Mismatches were less frequently recorded in the representations of control skin surfaces. 4. At cortical locations with entrained responses, neither the absolute firing rates of neurons nor the degree of the entrainment of the response were correlated with behavioral discrimination performance. 5. All area 3b cortical locations with entrained responses evoked by stimulation at trained or untrained skin sites were combined to create population peristimulus time and cycle histograms. In all cases, stimulation of the trained skin resulted in 1) larger-amplitude responses, 2) peak responses earlier in the stimulus cycle, and 3) temporally sharper responses, than did stimulation applied to control skin sites. 6. The sharpening of the response of cortical area 3b neurons relative to the period of the stimulus could be accounted for by a large subpopulation of neurons that had highly coherent responses. 7. Analysis of cycle histograms for area 3b neuron responses revealed that the decreased variance in the representation of each stimulus cycle could account for behaviorally measured frequency discrimination performance. A strong correlation between these temporal response distributions and the discriminative performances for stimuli applied at all studied skin surfaces was even stronger (r = 0.98) if only the rising phases of cycle histogram were considered in the analysis. 8. The responses of neurons in area 3a could not account for measured differences in frequency discrimination performance. 9. These representational changes did not occur in monkeys that were stimulated on the same schedule but were performing an auditory discrimination task during skin stimulation. 10. It is concluded that by behaviorally training adult owl monkeys to discriminate the temporal features of a tactile stimulus, distributed spatial and temporal response properties of cortical neurons are altered.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Article
To evaluate reorganization in motor pathways following amputation, we studied motor evoked potentials(MEPs) to transcranial magnetic stimulation in 7 patients with unilateral upper limb amputations, a patient with congenital absence of a hand, and 10 normal subjects. Electromyographic recordings were made from biceps and deltoid muscles immediately proximal to the stump and the same contralateral muscles. Magnetic stimulation was delivered by a Cadwell MES-10 magnetic stimulator through a ‘figure eight’ magnetic coil over scalp positions separated by 1–2.5 cm. Maximal M responses were elicited by peripheral nerve stimulation at Erb's point. The amplitude of MEPs was expressed both as absolute values and as a percentage of maximal responses to peripheral nerve stimulation. Threshold for activation of muscles ipsilateral and contralateral to the stump and the region of excitable scalp positions were also determined in 7 patients. Magnetic scalp stimulation induced a sensation of movement in the missing hand or fingers in the patients with acquired amputation, but failed to do so in the patient with congenital absence of a limb. It evoked larger MEPs, recruited a larger percentage of the motoneuron pool, and elicited MEPs at lower intensities of stimulation in muscles ipsilateral to the stump than in contralateral muscles. Muscles ipsilateral to the stump could be activated from a larger area than those contralateral to the stump. These results are compatible with cortical or spinal reorganization in adult human motor pathways targeting muscles proximal to the stump after amputations.
Article
We have examined the effect of restricted unilateral cochlear lesions on the orderly topographic mapping of sound frequency in the auditory cortex of adult guinea pigs. These lesions, although restricted in spatial extent, resulted in a variety of patterns of histological damage to receptor cells and nerve fibres within the cochlea. Nevertheless, all lesions resulted in permanent losses of sensitivity of the cochlear neural output across a limited frequency range. Thirty-five to 81 days after such damage to the organ of Corti, the area of contralateral auditory cortex in which the lesioned frequency range would normally have been represented was partly occupied by an expanded representation of sound frequencies adjacent to the frequency range damaged by the lesion. The thresholds at their new characteristic frequencies (CFs) of clusters of cortical neurones in these regions were close to normal thresholds at those frequencies (mean difference across all animals was 3.8 dB). In a second series of experiments, the responses of neurone clusters were examined within hours of making similar cochlear lesions. It was found that shifts in CF toward frequencies spared by the lesions could occur, but thresholds were greatly elevated compared to normal (mean difference was 31.7 dB in five animals). The emergence of sensitive drive in such regions after prolonged recovery periods in lesioned animals thus suggests that the auditory cortical frequency map undergoes reorganization in cases of partial deafness. Some features of this reorganization are similar to changes reported in somatosensory cortex after peripheral nerve injury, and this form of plasticity may therefore be a feature of all adult sensory systems.
Article
Receptive fields (RFs) obtained at specific cortical sites can be used to define a topographic map of the body surface in adult mammalian somatosensory cortex. This map is not static, and RFs at particular cortical sites can change in size and location throughout adult life. Conversely, the cortical loci at which a given skin surface is represented can shift hundreds of micrometres across the cortex in the koniocortical field, area 3b (refs 1-12). This plasticity suggests that RFs derive not from rigid anatomical connections, but by the selection of a subset of a large number of inputs. We have proposed that inputs are selected on the basis of temporal correlation 11-15. Here we test this idea by altering the correlation of inputs from two adjacent digits on the adult owl monkey hand by surgically connecting the skin surfaces of the two fingers (the formation of syndactyly). This manipulation increases the correlation of inputs from skin surfaces of adjacent fingers. The striking discontinuity between the zones of representation of adjacent digits on the somatosensory cortex disappeared. These results support the hypothesis that the topography of the body-surface map in the adult cortex is influenced by the temporal correlations of afferent inputs.