ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Postural control system

Authors:
  • French National Centre for Scientific Research: emeritus

Abstract

The postural control system has two main functions: first, to build up posture against gravity and ensure that balance is maintained; and second, to fix the orientation and position of the segments that serve as a reference frame for perception and action with respect to the external world. This dual function of postural control is based on four components: reference values, such as orientation of body segments and position of the center of gravity (an internal representation of the body or postural body scheme); multisensory inputs regulating orientation and stabilization of body segments; and flexible postural reactions or anticipations for balance recovery after disturbance, or postural stabilization during voluntary movement. The recent data related to the organization of this system will be discussed in normal subjects (during ontogenesis), the elderly and in patients with relevant deficits.
Postural control system
Jean Massion
Laboratory of Neurobiology and Movements, CNRS, Marseille, France
The postural control system has two main functions: first, to build up posture
against gravity and ensure that balance is maintained; and second, to fix the
orientation and position of the segments that serve as a reference frame for
perception and action with respect to the external world. This dual function
of postural control is based on four components: reference values, such
as orientation of body segments and position of the center of gravity (an
internal representation of the body or postural body scheme); multisensory
inputs regulating orientation and stabilization of body segments; and flexible
postural reactions or anticipations for balance recovery after disturbance, or
postural stabilization during voluntary movement. The recent data related to
the organization of this system will be discussed in normal subjects (during
ontogenesis), the elderly and in patients with relevant deficits.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1994, 4:877-887
Introduction
Body posture is built up by a set of assembled segments,
each with its own mass, that are linked together by
flexible joints controlled by the neuromuscular system.
The central organization of posture involves interactions
between external forces, such as gravity, the mechanical
properties of the body and the neuromuscular forces.
In order to understand the present trends in studies on
postural organization, one should bear in mind that pos-
ture serves two main functions. First, it has a mechani-
cal antigravity function whereby the reference posture
(stance) is built up; equilibrium also depends on this
antigravity function, which requires the center of grav-
ity (CG) projection to remain inside the supporting
surface under static conditions. Second, it serves as a
reference frame for perception and action with respect
to the external world. The position and orientation of
body segments such as the head, trunk or arms serve as a
reference frame for calculating target locations in the ex-
ternal world and for organizing movements toward these
targets.
In line with the complexity of the functions mediated
by posture, central organization of its control system in-
volves many interacting elements. On the sensory side,
multisensory (visual, labyrinthine, proprioceptive and
cutaneous) inputs contribute to orienting the postural
segments with respect both to each other and the ex-
ternal world (vertical gravity vector). These classes of
sensors monitor any mismatch between the intended
and actual positions. A so-called postural body scheme
provides an internal representation of the body geom-
etry, the body dynamics (support conditions) and the
body orientation with respect to verticality. The postu-
ral reactions, like the anticipatory postural adjustments
associated with voluntary movements, are organized on
the basis of this internal representation, as is the whole set
of interactions involved in perception and action towards
the external world.
Because of the complexity of the postural control system,
many aspects have given rise to some debate. How are
posture and equilibrium centrally organized? How does
the organism adapt to changes, such as microgravity, in
the environment? How is the system built up during on-
togenesis? What impairments affect it in the elderly and
diseased?
Progress in research often depends on new methodologi-
cal approaches. A new method has recently been used
in this field to analyze postural control in the absence
of external disturbances. Classical methods of analysis,
such as recording the center of pressure oscillations from
a force platform under normal stance, have been re-ex-
amined. The stationary properties of postural sway have
been questioned [l]. New techniques of analysis using
the chaotic dynamic approach have been proposed in
order to identify putative attractors in postural sway
[2]. Collins and De Luca [3] have carried out stochas-
tic analyses on the center of pressure oscillations during
quiet stance to identify the open-loop and closed-loop
components of these oscillations. A cross-correlation
method has been used on simultaneously recorded kine-
matic parameters at various levels of the multijoint chain
involved in erect posture, with a view to identif+ng the
kinematic strategies used under various sensory condi-
tions ([4*], see also IS]). Several mathematical
analyses
have been carried out on multijoint changes to elucidate
Abbreviations
CC-center of gravity; EMG-electromyograph,
0 Current Biology Ltd ISSN 0959-4388
877
878 Neural control
Postural control
1 Orienta;;
1 1
;;““ility
1 __..-,_ - _.
fX3y
Feedback control
of
posture
Cunent Opinion in Neurobiology
Fig. 1.
Central organization of postural control. This schematic diagram summarizes the main components involved in the central organization
of postural control. There are two sets of reference values, one is related to body segment orientation and the other is related to whole body
stability (equilibrium control). These references values, and their maintenance against external or internal disturbances, are based on a body
schema or internal representation of the body, which include several components, namely, body geometry and kinetics, representation of
verticality and reference frame. In addition, postural networks contribute to the execution of the postural tasks. Multisensory inputs are used for
building up the body schema. These inputs also act as error-detecting sensors for evaluating the mismatch between the prescribed orientation
and stability, and the actual posture. The postural reactions in the presence of an error message, as well as the anticipatory postural adjustments
in association with voluntary movements, are exerted through the postural networks on one or several segments. The execution of the postural
reactions or anticipations are controlled on line by local feedbacks.
the control processes involved in the kinematic control
of posture [6*,7*]. Biomechanical modelling is another
interesting means of studying the postural strategies de-
veloped, depending on the constraints [8*,9,10-121.
This review will focus on four aspects of the postural
control system, where the main contributions have been
made recently. These advances are related to the control
of the center of gravity, the concept of postural body
schema, postural reactions and anticipation, and age-re-
lated changes.
Center of gravity control versus body geometry
When stance is disturbed in bipeds or quadru-
peds, the resulting postural reactions tend to move the
body back toward its initial position, as long as the
imposed disturbance does not overcome given limits.
During stance, there is thus a reference position that
is stabilized. Some controversy has arisen as to whether
the reference value stabilized to preserve balance is the
center of gravity position with respect to the ground, or
whether the body geometry is the main value regulated.
In the former situation, the fictive point (CG), which is
the result of the masses of the individual body segments,
would be directly regulated, whereas in the latter, this
fictive point would be regulated secondarily as a result
of body geometry control.
Lacquaniti has provided evidence (see [13]) that in the
cat under normal stance, it is the body geometry rather
than the CG that is regulated. First, the length and an-
gle of the limb axis with respect to the vertical were kept
constant when the supporting platform was tilted in the
sagittal plane. Second, the CG moved when a load was
added in front of the CG position, indicating that no
direct regulation of the CG position occurred. In a re-
cent study, Lacquanti and Maioli [14**] identified two
separate control systems, one regulating limb length and
axis with respect to the vertical, and the other the hori-
zontal contact forces (lever component) exerted by fore
and hindlimbs in order to stabilize the body. This kinetic
control system presumably plays a role in maintaining the
CG within the support area. In fact two parallel control
systems therefore exist, the one focusing on the body
geometry and the other on stability [14*-l (Fig. 1).
Is the limb axis, with respect to the vertical, the ge-
ometrical reference value which is regulated during
stance? An alternative explanation has been proposed
by Macpherson [15**] and by Fung and Macpherson
[PI. By changing the interlimb distance in the sagit-
tal and frontal plane, they concluded that the length
and the orientation of the trunk with respect to the
supporting surface, i.e. an external reference frame, is ac-
tually regulated, the legs being used as levers to maintain
the trunk length constant, while the interlimb distance
is varied. These authors concluded that the preferred in-
terlimb distance during natural stance corresponds to the
distance involving the least energy consumption [Y].
Is the CG regulated in human? In human bipedal stance,
the narrow support surface provided by the foot area and
height of the body with respect to the ground make
for a much smaller safety margin than in quadrupeds.
It has been established previously that after stance dis-
turbances, one of the strategies used, namely the hip
strategy, consists of flexing or extending the hip, in or-
der to keep the CG within the stability limits [lb]. The
same change in body geometry occurs when a voluntary
movement of the upper trunk is performed; this would
entail a movement of the CG in the same direction if no
corrective processes intervened. Opposite displacements
of the lower segments then occur however, which re-
sults in the maintenance of CG within the support area.
This has been termed synergy by Babinski [17] but a
more appropriate name might be kinematic strategy, in
line with the hip strategy known to counteract imposed
disturbances. These findings indicate that in humans, the
body geometry changes in order to regulate the CG po-
sition.
The question then arises as to how the CG is regulated,
especially when a voluntary movement is performed
which would induce imbalance. The kinematic strat-
egy observed during upper trunk bending has been ana-
lyzed using the principal components analysis method.
Surprisingly, both the movement and the CG regula-
tion carried out using the kinematic strategy were ex-
pressed by the first principal component as fully as 99%
in the case of forward upper trunk movements [7] and
96% in backward movements. This indicates that a single
central control fixes the ratios between angular changes
in the hip, knee and ankle joints and synchronizes their
changes with time. The axial kinematic changes associ-
ated with upper trunk movements thus constitute an au-
tomatic control, probably learned in childhood, whereby
the movement and the CG position are controlled simul-
taneously This would explain why angular changes asso-
ciated with upper trunk movements remain unchanged
under microgravity, in the absence of any equilibrium
constraints [ 181.
In addition to the stabilization of the CG position, the
orientation of several body segments has also been re-
ported to be regulated simultaneously during stance,
movement or locomotion. For example, the head axis
is often stabilized with respect to the vertical during lo-
comotion [19,20]. This stabilization provides a reference
value aligned with the vertical axis, used for monitor-
Postural control system
Massion 879
ing, via visual and vestibular inputs, the head and body
movements with respect to the environment. Head- and
trunk-centered reference Ii-ames are also used for target
location and movement trajectory planning (see [21]).
The trunk axis is actively stabilized during locomotion
in both the frontal and sagittal planes, due to the ac-
tion of the hip muscles [22*,23]. The trunk stabilization
in the frontal plane is increased in labyrinthine defective
patients [24]. The trunk axis also remains vertically ori-
ented during leg raising, especially in dancers [25]. As
the trunk axis serves as an egocentric reference frame
for calculating leg position [26*], vertical orientation of
the trunk may be used for direct calculation of the leg
position with respect to space.
To conclude, the results of recent investigations indicate
that in cats and humans, two control systems can be
identified: one fixes the orientation of the body seg-
ments with respect to the external world, while the
other ensures the stability of the body and contributes
to stabilizing the CG (Fig. 1).
Postural body scheme
According to Gurfinkel [27], there is an internal repre-
sentation of the body, or postural body scheme, which
is not primarily based on sensory information and deals
with the body kinematics and kinetics as well as the ori-
entation of the body with respect to the vertical. This
representation is used for the perception of body posi-
tion and its orientation in space and is also used for mo-
tor control, including reactions directed towards main-
taining stable body position”[27].
The body scheme remains quite stable during dras-
tic changes in environmental conditions, such as those
occurring under microgravity, where the vestibular and
proprioceptive inputs are greatly modified (see [28-l). As
ascertained by testing subjects perception of complex
tactile stimuli [29] or by asking them to draw ellipses
with prescribed orientations [30], the egocentric refer-
ence frame is still used in the absence of gravity to per-
form perceptual and spatial orientation tasks efficiently.
The amplitude of forearm movements in the vertical and
horizontal planes remains roughly unchanged under mi-
crogravity, except in the case of slow movements, which
are disturbed because they are controlled by propriocep-
tive feedbacks, which are depressed under microgravity
[28*]. The ability of subjects to point to remembered
target positions deteriorates in space [31].
The sensors that contribute to estimating the bodys
orientation, configuration and support conditions have
been further investigated. One of the questions currently
under debate concerns the estimation of the gravity vec-
tor. Besides the well-known contribution of labyrinthne
inputs and vision to monitoring the vertical axis, a pu-
tative role of body graviceptors has been suggested in
the past [32,33], and this idea has recently been indi-
880 Neural control
rectly supported. By adding loads under water, where
the bodys weight is cancelled by the water pressure, or
by adding a horizontal force equal to the body weight
while the subject is lying down, the previously absent
postural reaction to support disturbances can be re-
stored. These results argue in favour of the presence of
graviceptors distributed among the segments.
Which sensors are used to monitor the load? One
possible explanation might be that the muscle effort
opposing the effects of gravity or other forces may be
estimated by the Golgi tendon organs, whose discharge
pattern correlates with the number of active motor units
[34,35]. U . g dff
sm a 1 erent approach, based on a subjective
estimation of verticality, Riccio, Martin and Stoffregen
[36] reached similar conclusions by artificially dissociat-
ing the gravity vector horn the axis of the ground reac-
tion forces that have to be controlled in order to maintain
equilibrium. These authors concluded that the subjective
vertical depends on both the gravity vector and the di-
rection of the ground reaction forces needed to control
balance. Interactions between the orientation of the vi-
sual reference frame and somatosensory inputs relating
to the body orientation with respect to the vertical in
the perceived subjective vertical have also been found
to occur by Nemire and Cohen [37], indicating the
importance of the trunk axis (idiotropic vector) in the
perceived vertical [33]. Under microgravity, the percep-
tion of the subjective vertical in the absence of vision
may depend on the saccular Z bias, that is, on the
difference between the mean resting discharges of sac-
cular units polarized in the rostra1 and caudal directions.
Other cues - such as tactile, visual and proprioceptive
- also play an important role in body orientation under
microgravity [38].
One of the most clearly emerging properties of the sen-
sory inputs that contributes to the body scheme is the
marked dependence of these inputs on context, partic-
ularly on the reference frame used for their analysis (see
discussion in DiZio it
a/.
[39-l). For example, the illu-
sory or effective body sways that result horn artificial Ia
inputs produced by vibrating bilaterally the tendon of
either the gastrocnemius or tibialis anterior muscle in
a standing subject tend to disappear under microgravity
[40*]. These postural illusions or reactions are therefore
gravity-dependent.
The role played by vestibular and proprioceptive inputs
in human self-motion perception in space also depends
on the reference frame. Mergner ET al. [41*], using vari-
ous combinations of head, trunk and feet rotation, have
proposed a model in which the vestibular signal and the
proprioceptive input arising along the whole body axis
were used to reconstruct the perception of head, trunk
and feet position in space. In vestibular patients, the
perception of trunk rotation in space with the head
free is deficient contrary to normal subjects, indicat-
ing that vestibular inputs are normally responsible for
this perception [42*]. With the head fixed during trunk
rotation, patients perceived an illusory head movement
(due to the nuccal afferents). As soon as a fixed visual or
tactile reference frame was used, the same patients with
vestibular deficits perceived the real trunk rotation. Sim-
ilar results were obtained by Guxfinkel and Levik [43-l
in normal subjects, using very slow trunk rotations that
were not perceived at all with the head f?ee. With the
head fixed, illusory head rotation is perceived; whereas
when the hand is in contact with a rigid handle fixed to
the wall, the real trunk rotation is perceived [43*]. How
the proprioceptice input is interpreted by the subject is
therefore highly dependent on the reference frame used
at the same time.
Postural responses are also markedly context-depen-
dent. This is illustrated by the responses induced by
labyrinthine stimulation [44*]. A standing subjects re-
sponse to galvanic stimulation of the labyrinth consists
of a postural sway in the direction of the ear behind
which the anode is placed. As previously established,
the direction of the sway changes with the head posi-
tion with respect to the trunk, and that of the trunk
with reference to the legs; the direction of sway therefore
depends on the body geometry at the time of stimula-
tion. The electrbmyograph (EMG) response associated
with the sway (the late component of the response) de-
pends on the support conditions: it occurs in the arm
muscles when part of the body support is exerted by the
hands, and disappears in the soleus in the sitting sub-
ject. It therefore occurs in the muscles that are actively
engaged in balance. Moreover, reduced responses were
observed when other sources of afferent information are
available, for example when the subjects touched a fixed
support or when their eyes were open.
Postural reactions
Postural reactions are elicited on the basis of sensory sig-
nals that indicate a disturbance ofposture and/or equilib-
rium. Experimental stance disturbances are usually pro-
duced by moving the supporting platform. Disturbances
restricted to the hip level have also been tested and con-
pared between normal and hemiparetic patients [45].
Discrete disturbances have usually been used; pseudo-
random disturbances have also been tested [46]. The
central organization of the postural reactions will not
be specifically discussed here (see Jankowska and Ed-
gley [47] on the spinal cord organization). It is worth
mentioning however that chronic spinal cats show very
poor postural control [48].
One of the emerging ideas about the organization of
postural reactions is that the central nervous system is
unable to control individual muscles separately and that
it controls only a small number of degrees of freedom by
activating functional synergies, involving a set of muscles
regulated as a whole [49,50]. Synergies have commonly
been observed in postural reactions. They appear to be
flexible and depend on external constraints such as the
Postural control system
Massion 881
direction of the forces disturbing posture. The bifunc-
tional muscles that extend across two joints contribute
largely to this flexibility, because their activity depends
more strongly on sensory inputs than the single joint
muscles, and they might serve to orient the force vec-
tor exerted by the single joint muscles depending on the
requirements of the postural task [51*].
It has been suggested by Horak and Nashner [16] that
a higher level of organization may be involved in the
postural reactions to stance disturbance: at this level
strategies are selected that each define a given type of
action for restauring balance. Hip strategy (torques and
movement at the hip joint), ankle strategy (torques and
movement at the ankle joint), and stepping are different
ways of restoring balance, depending on the intensity of
the balance disturbance and the constraints. The mus-
cle synergy may be a lower level of organization, imple-
menting the strategy by providing the appropriate muscle
forces.
Recent data tend to show that the strategy level is also
flexible and adaptable to task constraints. For exam-
ple, the hip and ankle strategies are not all or none
reactions but rather form a continuum under progres-
sively changing external constraints. Horak and Moore
[52*] have observed that a continuum occurs in postural
changes involving gradually more hip strategy and less
ankle strategy when leaning forward is increased. The
postural response to stance disturbance is also adapted
when a disturbance to equilibrium is used as a sig-
nal for gait initiation; in this situation, goal-directed
changes in postural responses were found to occur [53].
Conclusions on the same lines were reached by Al-
lum
et al.
[54-l, who disturbed standing posture by
random combinations of rotation (triggering the hip
strategy) and translation (triggering the ankle strategy)
of a supporting surface. On the basis of kinematic and
EMG analysis, they established that one out of two
discrete muscle synergies was selected on the basis of
leg afferent input. In addition, segmental velocity re-
lated inputs continuously update the initial pattern. A
comparable updating of the basic pattern by segmental
inputs was also reported by Forssberg and Hirschfeld
[55-l during disturbance of the sitting position. A depen-
dence on the biomechanical constraints was also found
in the case of the force constraint strategy observed in
the cat, whatever the direction of the stance disturbance
[9*,15**]. This strategy does not depend on prior experi-
ence and is therefore part of the animals repertoire [56].
It is characterized by the fact that the force directions ex-
erted to restore balance under individual paws are always
oriented forward and backward whatever the direction
of the balance disturbance. This strategy disappears with
small interpaw distances. On the basis of a biomechani-
cal analysis, Macpherson and Fung [9*] have proposed
that this strategy may be aimed at preventing a lateral
bending of the trunk, thus maintaining the back length
invariant when lateral disturbing forces are present. With
shorter interlimb distances, the back incurvation seems
to be prevented by an increase in back stiffiless [9*].
There is also some flexibility in compensatory stepping
observed in response to stance disturbance. This response
depends on the velocity of the disturbance as well as on
the instructions given to the subjects. Aborted reactions
are also observed where only the initial weight transfer
toward the supporting limb occurs [57,58]. The step-
ping response is thus composed of several stages, each of
which includes a decision made on the basis of sensory
cues.
Role of sensory inputs
Some recent investigations have been carried out on the
respective roles of the various categories of sensory in-
puts involved in postural stability.
To what extent do the vestibular inputs contribute to the
postural reactions to stance disturbance? In cats, with
bilateral labyrinthectomy, no change in the force con-
straint strategy nor in the muscle synergy afier stance
disturbance was detected [59]. Hypermetric responses
are seen early after the operation. In humans, a spe-
cial device for producing phasic vestibular linear stim-
ulation by displacing the head was tested. This stimula-
tion induced postural responses in leg and trunk muscles
in standing subjects. These responses were rather weak,
however, suggesting that vestibular inputs do not con-
tribute strongly to early postural reactions to balance
stance disturbances [60*]. The vestibular system plays
a role in organizating hip strategy [61]. Imposed ankle
joint rotation induces a hip strategy in normal subjects,
which disappears in vestibular patients and is replaced by
a rather inefficient ankle strategy [54*]. It should also be
mentioned that no vestibular deficits were detected in
Parkinsonian patients with impaired balance [62], and
that, generally speaking, the usual multisensory inputs
are properly integrated in these patients [63]: this suggests
that the disorder affects only the control of the postural
reactions.
The role of leg somatosensory inputs in postural stabil-
ity has also been investigated. For example, longitudinal
platform oscillations at 8-24 Hz markedly increased the
subjects postural instability [64]. The somatosensory
inputs are not only involved in postural stability: so-
matosensory inputs Gem the lower leg also contribute
directly to the stabilization of the head as do the vi-
sual and vestibular inputs 1651. Are the somatosensory
inputs from the leg needed to trigger and scale human
automatic postural responses? In patients with lower limb
neuropathy, the postural reactions to stance disturbance
are still present, but their latency increases (2@3Oms)
[66*]. This suggests that leg somatosensory inputs are
actually used to trigger and scale the postural reactions;
however, other inputs (trunk, vestibular and visual)
may
replace the missing inputs.
How are somatosensory inputs used to scale the postural
response in ccrebellar patients [67]! Interestingly, in pa-
tients with anterior lobe pathology, feedback-controlled
scaling, depending on the velocity of the disturbance, is
882 Neural control
still possible. In contrast, scaling of the response am-
plitude, which depends on presetting before the dis-
turbance onset, disappears and is replaced by tendency
towards hypermetria.
Concerning the role of vision in postural control, it has
been shown that postural sway is equally controlled by
peripheral and central visual fields [68].The effects of a
moving visual environment on postural oscillations have
been investigated by Previc
et al.
[69] and Dijkstra
et
al.
[70]. The latter paper concludes that retinal slip
minimization does not explain the coupling between
a moving visual environment and postural sway, and
that a dynamic coupling between the two must exist.
Vision has been found to shorten the latency of postural
responses [71]. Vection has been shown to shorten or de-
lay the onset of balance recovery in unexpected forward
falls, depending on the direction of the moving scene
[72]. Lastly, the respective roles of the somatosensory
and visual inputs in stabilizating body motion depend
on the stance width. With a larger supporting base, the
role of vision in body stabilization in the frontal plane
decreases in favor of somatosensory inputs [ll]. Visual
motion compensates in the cat for loss of vestibular in-
put during the early stages of recovery after unilateral
labyrinthectomy. The vestibular nucleus response to op-
tokinetic stimuli on the deafferented side shows an in-
creased band width [73].
Anticipatory postural adjustments
Unlike the postural reactions in response to the onset of
posture or balance disturbances, the anticipatory postural
adjustments precede the disturbance onset and therefore
minimize the effects of the forthcoming disturbance in
a feedforward manner.
Anticipatory postural adjustments usually occur in asso-
ciation with voluntary movements, which are one of the
main sources of posture and balance disturbance. Antici-
patory adjustments can also be observed, however, when
imposed disturbance is recurrent, as in the case of hu-
man stance on a sinusoidally translating platform. The
anticipatory adjustments which are then observed serve
to orient the body so as to minimize the effects of grav-
itoinertial forces. When the frequency of the platform
translation is changed, the feedforward mode is replaced
by a feedback mode of control for a few oscillations
and the feedforward control then reappears [74]. This
anticipatory postural control is impaired in Parkinsonian
patients [75].
Anticipatory postural adjustments before the disturbance
of a single joint position caused by the voluntary move-
ment of another segment have been described. For ex-
ample, when a subject is tapping with a hammer on the
radial muscle of his other forearm, there is a silent period
in the muscle response pattern before the actual me-
chanical impact. This inhibition may minimize impact
effect on forearm posture by reducing muscle stiffness
in advance of the disturbance [76]. The mechanisms re-
sponsible for anticipatory postural adjustments have been
investigated. In a bimanual load lifting task, where one
arm was supporting the load and the other voluntary
lifting the load, anticipatory postural adjustments were
observed in the postural arm (see [77]). Interestingly,
the sarne adjustments were observed in a patient with
forearm-afferent deprivation, indicating that these ad-
justments result from feedforward control. The limb
afferents are necessary, however, for new anticipatory
postural adjustrnent when unloading the postural fore-
arm is triggered by new movement [78].
Anticipatory postural adjustments aimed at maintaining
balance have been observed with upper trunk move-
ments (see [77]). These adjustments are impaired in
cerebellar anterior lobe patients; the main deficit involves
a lack of feedforward activation in the thigh muscles at
the onset of the movement [79]. Respiratory oscillations
can also lead to balance disturbance. As the respiratory
oscillation estimated from the center of pressure sway
path recorded from a force platform is larger in sitting
than in standing subjects, anticipatory control seems to
be more efficient when standing than when sitting [80].
The last specific type of postural adjustment occurs with
movements involving the legs, such as gait initiation,
standing on tip toe or the heels and lifting or raising
a leg. With motor acts of this kind, the movement is
preceded by postural changes that shift the CG toward a
new position compatible with equilibrium maintenance
during the leg movement. These types ofpostural adjust-
ments are impaired in patients with hemiparesis. When
asked to lift a leg, they are not able to use the paralyzed
limb to exert the appropriate horizontal ground reaction
forces needed to move the CG toward the supporting
leg [81]. When asked to stand on tip toe, the temporal
sequence is disturbed on both sides [82]. Standing on
tip toe is also markedly impaired in anterior lobe cere-
bellar patients [83] and Parkinsonian [84]. The scaling of
the amplitude and duration of the preparatory postural
phase is abnormal in cerebellar patient (low amplitude,
prolonged duration) and the temporal relationship be-
tween the postural and movement phase is lost.
Age-related changes
Ontogenesis
Successive steps have been described during the devel-
opment of posture: postural control of the head, postural
control of the trunk, sitting position, standing and loco-
motion. The emergence of these various stages depends
on the evolutional state of several systems, such as the
musculo-skeletal system, the sensori-motor system, the
level of motivation and of behavioral development, and
the internal and environmental constraints [85].
Postural control system
Massion 883
Both the visual and somatosensory systems are crucial for
stabilization of head, trunk and whole body posture, but
vision is effective earlier than the somatosensory system.
At each stage in postural development, moving visual in-
formation contributes to maintaining posture and equi-
librium. It has been established that vision and optic
flow influence spontaneous head oscillation as early as
2 to 3 days after birth, and head posture as early as
5 months of age [86]. A response to optic flow oc-
curs with a supported standing posture as early as 5
months of age and increases considerably later on. The
somatosensory system is mainly involved in reactions to
support disturbances. Head stabilization is observed from
3 to 4 months of age and head-trunk stabilization in sit-
ting posture at 5 months [87]. Hirschfeld and Forssberg
[88*] have observed that in sitting children who undergo
a forward horizontal balance disturbance, the EMG
pattern is roughly comparable to the adult one [55*],
since the activation of the front muscles induced by
backward trunk imposed displacement is similar to what
occurs in adults, although this is not so in the case of the
back muscles. These authors suggested that two levels of
control might exist: basic one present in children, and
that which develops in adults as the result of learning.
At 9 months of age, the classical distal-proximal EMG
pattern present in adults subjects to platform disturbances
is present in standing children. It is preceded by a stage
where only the ankle joint muscles are activated and
standing without support is not possible [89,90]. The
vestibular contribution to postural reactions has turned
out to be apparently less important than the somatosen-
sory one early in life, but further investigations will be
necessary to elucidate this point.
Special attention has been paid to equilibrium control
during locomotion. As soon as locomotion starts, stabi-
lization of the hip in the frontal plane with respect to
space is observed [91*,92]. After two months of walk-
ing experience, stabilization of the shoulders improves.
This suggest that a hip-centered temporal organization of
balance control occurs while walking. Long-term mat-
uration of locomotor balance has also been investigated.
During the first stage (3-6 years), the head is stabilized
on the trunk whenever constraints makes equilibrium
difficult. By 7-8 years, the head is also stabilized in space
during locomotion when facing equilibrium constraints.
Adults use similar stabilization of the head in the frontal
plane while walking on a narrow support (see [85]).
Does the head stabilization in space depend on visual in-
puts or is it based on vestibular cues? Although peripheral
vision and movement visual cues play an important part
in balance control, especially in young children, head
stabilization in space does not depend to any great ex-
tent on visual cues and may therefore mostly depend on
vestibular cues [20]. Head stabilization in space in chil-
dren as in adults [93] is a basis for top-down postural
control on basis of visual and vestibular cues.
Ageing
The balance deficits that occur with ageing have been
extensively studied over recent years. Authors of three
recent papers [94-961 have emphasized the multiple
sources of balance deficits in the elderly and the need
to adopt a systemic approach. For example, sensory
receptors deteriorate with ageing. Sensory deprivation
or sensory conflicts have more drastic effects on balance
in the elderly than in younger subjects [97]. Mechanical
properties of the tendons and muscles are also affected
in terms of their force and elasticity. Postural responses
are delayed and weaker with more co-activation in the
elderly [98]. Ability to adapt changing external perturba-
tions such as shifting from a translation to a rotational dis-
turbance is impaired. Postural responses associated with
voluntary movements are also impaired (increased laten-
ties, excessive co-contractions, etc.) [99]. Furthermore,
the attentional demands of postural tasks are increased
in the elderly [loo]. Generally speaking, the impairment
of balance that develops in the elderly results from the
degradation of multiple systems that participate, either
directly or indirectly, in the task of balance control.
Conclusions
The overall picture of postural organization that emerges
horn recent investigations is a long way off the picture
of classical postural reflexes presented by the Sherring-
tonian School. While the old description of these re-
flexes is still valid and their analysis is still a useful means
of experimentation and neurological evaluation, the en-
phasis now is on the flexibility of postural control and its
adaptability to different contexts.
Such flexibility is reflected in the multisensory integra-
tion which is involved in postural orientation and sta-
bilization. The multisensory aspect of postural control
was first pointed out as early phylogenetically as in the
lamprey [lOl*], but its ability to adapt to context and
task is characteristic of higher vertebrates. This explains
the large range of compensatory possibilities available in
the case of selective sensory deficits, and also the fact that
a given sensory input can induce various perceptions or
postural reactions, depending on the reference frame se-
lected and the external constraints such as gravity This
flexibility is under the control of the internal representa-
tion of the body or postural body scheme, which remains
quite stable under changing conditions.
Postural reactions show a similar degree of flexibility.
Some invariant aspects of postural reactions, such as
strategies and synergies, appear to be less fixed than
was first thought to be the case, and change depend-
ing on the constraints. The external constraints, as well
as the biomechanical properties of the body segments,
impose choice of control. One of the main criteria for
the emergence of a postural pattern after training is that
884 Neural control
it should be as economical as possible in terms of en-
ergy consumption. This does not mean that the brain is
merely a passive player in the game. Its role is to utilize
passive
forces to organize the most suitable spatiotempo-
ral pattern in order to carry out a task. It plays a crucial
role in coordinating postural tasks with various aspects of
the ongoing action. The coordination between posture,
equilibrium and movement is certainly one of the main
functions of the postural control system.
Acknowledgements
The author wish to thank S Zakarian for the very efficient
help
in the preparation of this
review paper, and F Horak and
J Macpherson for their critical reading of the manuscript. The
CNES (Centre National detudes Spatiales) is acknowledged
for
its support.
References and recommended reading
Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of
review, have been highlighted as:
. of special interest
. . of outstanding interest
1. Carroll JP, Freedman W:
Nonstationary properties of postural
sway. ) Biomech 1993,
26:409-416.
2. Newell
KM, Van Emmerik REA, Lee D, Sprague RL: On
postural
stability and variability. Cair & Posture
1993, 4:225-230.
3. Collins JJ, De Luca CJ:
Open-loop and closed-loop control of
. posture: a random-walk analysis of center of pressure trajec-
tory. Exp Brain
Res
1993, 95:308-318.
The center of pressure trajectories during quiet stance (steady states
behavior) are modelled as fractional Brownian motion. This analysis
reveals two modes of postural control: open loop and feedback.
4. Amblard 8, Assaiante C, Lekhel H, Marchand A:
A statistical
. approach to somatosensorimotor strategies: conjugate cross-
correlations. /
Motor Behav
1994, 26: 103-l 12.
A method based on cross-correlation function between two time series of
kinematic measurements is proposed for the analysis of multisegmental
movements. It can be used for defining postural strategies during stance.
5. Allum JHJ, Honegger F:
Synergies and strategies underlying
normal and vestibulary deficient control of balance: implica-
tion for neuroprosthetic control. In Progress in
brain research,
vol 97. Edited by Allurn JHJ, Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris
FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.;
1993:33 l-348.
6. Lacquaniti F, Maioli C:
Coordinate transformations in the con-
. trol of cat posture. / Neuro,ohysio/
1994, 72: 1496-l 515.
The authors have tested a number of different hypotheses on the nature
of the processing stage that transforms end point coordinates of the limbs
specified by limb length and orientation into the angular coordinates of
the joints (inverse mapping).
7. Alexandrov A, Frolov A, Massion J:
Voluntary forward bending
. movement in human: a principal component analysis of axial
synergies.
In
Vestibular and neural iron&.
Edited by Taguchi K,
Igarashi M, Mozi S. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1994:345-348.
Principal component analysis was used to determine the intersegmen-
tat constraints during upper trunk movement. A single component (PClJ
fixes the ratio of angle changes, and the amplitude and time course of
the movement.
8. Kuo AD, Zajac FE:
Human standing posture: multi-joint move-
. ment strategies based on biomechanical constraints.
In
Progress
in brain research, vol 97.
Edited by Allum JHJ, Allum-Meck-
lenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science
Publishers B.V.; 1993:349-358.
The authors proposed a theoretical framework for studying coordination
strategies in standing posture. This framework consists of a musculoskele-
tal model of human lower extremity in the sag&al plane and a technique
to visualize, geometrically, how constraints internal and external to the
body affect the movement. The model predicts that the hip strategy is
most effective at controlling the center of mass with minimal muscle ac-
tivation.
9. Fung J, Macpherson JM:
Determinants of postural orientation
. in quadrupedal stance.
J Neurosci
1994, in press.
The determinants of postural orientation in the standing cat were inves-
tigated by examining stance kinematics and kinetics at various interpaw
distances. The authors show that the trunk orientation is kept constant
due to maintaining a constant intralimb geometry.
10.
Lee WA, Russo AM:
Constraints and coordination in whole-
body actions. In
lnterlimb coordination: neural, dynamical, and
cognitive constrainfs. Edited by Swinnen S, Heuer Ii, Massion
J, Casaer P. San Diego: Academic Press; 1994537-569.
11. Day BL, Steiger MJ, Thompson PD, Marsden CD:
Effect of
vision and stance width on human body motion when stand-
ing: implications for afferent control of lateral sway.
J Physiol
(Londl
1993, 469:479-499.
12. Merfeld DM, Young LR, Oman CM, Shelhamer MJ:
A mul-
tidimensional model of the effect of gravity on the spatial
orientation of the monkey.
J Vest Res
1993, 3:141-161.
13. Lacquaniti F:
Automatic control of limb movement and pos-
ture. Curr
Opin Neurobiol
1992, 2:807-814.
14. Lacquaniti F, Maioli C:
Independent control of limb posi-
. . tion and contact forces in cat posture. /
Neurophysiol
1994,
72:1476-l 495.
The main aim of this study was to assess whether or not control of limb
position in the standing cat is independent of control of contact forces
at the feet. By tilting the support surface, changing interfeet distance and
head orientation, or by adding a load to the back, the authors showed
that two independent controls do exist, one addressed to limb length and
orientation, the other to the lever component oftangential contact forces.
15. Macpherson JM:
Changes in a postural strategy with inter-paw
. . distance.
J Neurophysiol
1994, 71:931-940.
The postural strategy during stance disturbance in the cat is characterized
by a forward or backward orientation of the ground reaction forces. This
strategy is lost for short interlimb distance. The author suggests that the
strategy is aimed at controlling the back orientation.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
.
Horak FB, Nashner LM:
Central programming of postural
movements: adaptation to altered support surface configura-
tions. /
Neurophysiol
1986, 55: 1369-l 381.
Babinski J:
De Iasynergie c&+belleuse.
Rev Neural
1899,
7:806-816.
Massion J, Curfinkel V, Lipshits M, Obadia A, Popov K:
Axial
synergies under microgravity conditions.
J Vestibular Res
1993,
3~275-287.
Berthoz A, Pozzo T: Head and body coordination during lo-
comotion and complex movements. In
Interlimb coordination:
neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints.
Edited by Swin-
nen S, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P. San Diego: Academic
Press; 1994: 147-l 65.
Assaiante C, Amblard B:
Ontogenesis of head stabilization in
space during locomotion in children: influence of visual cues.
Exp Brain Res
1993, 93:499-515.
Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Li CS, Stricanne 6:
Coordinate trans-
formations in the representation of spatial information.
Curr
Opin Neurobiol
1993, 3:171-l 76.
Mackinnon CD, Winter DA:
Control of whole body balance in
the frontal plane during human walking. /
Biomechanics
1993,
26:633-644.
The authors use a whole-body inverted pendulum model to investigate
control of balance and posture in the frontal plane during human walk-
ing.
23. Winter DA, Mackinnon CD, Ruder CK, Wieman C:
An in-
tegrated EMC/biomechanical model of upper body balance
Postural control system
Massion 885
and posture during human gait. In Progress in brain research,
vol 97.
Edited by Allum JHJ, Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris
FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.;
1993:359-367.
24. Glasauer S, Amorim MA, Vitte E, Berthoz
A: Coal-directed lin-
ear locomotion in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects.
Exp Brain
Res
1994, 98:323-335.
25. Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J, Pedotti
A: Coordina-
tion between equilibrium and head-trunk orientation during
leg movement: a new strategy built up by training. / Neuro-
physiol 1992, 67:1587-l 598.
26. Mouchnino L, Aurenty R,
Massion J, Pedotti
A: Is the trunk a
. reference frame for calculating leg position? Neuroreporf 1993,
4:125-l 27.
By comparing leg angle with respect to trunk during leg raising in dancers
(keepingthe trunk axis vertical) and untrained subjects (trunk inclination).
The authors concluded that leg position is estimated with respect to the
trunk axis.
27. Curfinkel VS:
The mechanisms of postural regulation in man.
Soviet Scientific
Reviews F Phys Gen Biol 1994, 7:59-89.
28.
Fisk j, Lackner JR, Dizio P:
Gravitoinertial force level
. influences arm movement control. /
Neurophysiol
1993,
69:504-511.
The ability to move the forearm between remembered elbow joint angle
after rapid changes of the gravitoinertial forces (G) level was measured. C
changes only influenced the movement amplitude for slow movements
that rely on proprioceptive information, suggesting a decrease of spindle
activity in 0 C.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
.
Curfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik YuS, Popov KE:
Egocentric
references and human spatial orientation in microgravity. I.
Perception of complex tactile stimuli. Exp
Brain Res
1993,
95:339-342.
Curfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik YuS, Popov KE, Lefort L:
Ego-
centric references and human spatial orientation in micrograv-
ity. II. Body-centred coordinates in the task of drawing ellipses
with prescribed orientation. Fxp
Brain
Res
1993, 95:343-348.
Young LR, Oman CM, Merfeld D, Watt D, Roy S, Deluca C,
Balkwill D, Christie J, Groleau N, Jackson DK ef al.:
Spatial
orientation and posture during and following weightlessness:
human experiments on spacelab life sciences 1. /
Vestibular
Res
1993, 3:231-239.
Gurfinkel VS, Lipshits YuS, Popov KE:
Stabilization of body
position as the main task of postural regulation. Fiziologya
Cheloveka
1981, 7:40&410.
Mittelstaedt H:
A new solution to the problem of the subjective
vertical.
Nafurwissenschaften
1983, 70:272-281.
Dietz V:
Neuronal basis of stance regulation. Interlimb coor-
dination and antigravity receptor function.
In
lnferlimb coor-
dination: neural, dynamical, and cognifive constraints.
Edited
by Swinnen SP, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P. New York: Aca-
demic Press; 1994:167-l 78.
Dietz V, Gollhofer A, Kleiber M, Trippel M:
Regulation of
bipedal stance: dependency on load receptors.
Exp Brain Res
1992, 89:229-231.
Riccio GE, Martin El, Stoffregen T:
The role of balance dynam-
ics in the active perception of orientation. / Exp
Psycho/ (HPPJ
1992,
18~624-644.
Nemire K, Cohen MM:
Visual and somesthetic influences on
postural orientation in the median plane.
Perception 8. Psy-
chophysics
1993, 53:106-l 16.
Mittelstaedt H, Glasauer S:
Crucial effects of weightlessness on
human orientation. /
Vesfibular Res
1993, 3:307-314.
Dizio P, Lathan CE, Lackner JR:
The role of brachial muscle
spindle signals in assignment of visual direction. /
Neurophysiol
1993, 70:1578-l 584.
The illusion of target light displacement (oculobrachial illusion) is per-
ceived when a target light is attached to the unseen stationary hand
and an illusory forearm motion is elicited by brachial muscle vibra-
tion. When two targets are fixed, one on each hand, and both biceps
vibrated simulatneously, the perceived distance between the targets in-
creases significantly. These findings indicate that brachial muscle spindle
signals can contribute to an independent representation of felt target lo-
cation in head-centric coordinates. The authors propose a model of these
representations.
40. Roll JP, Popov K, Gurfinkel V, Lipshits M, Andre-Deshays C,
. Gilhodes JC, Quoniam C:
Sensorimotor and perceptual
func-
tion of muscle proprioception in microgravity. 1
Vesfibular Res
1993, 3:259-273.
During long-term exposure to microgravity, the postural responses or the
illusory postural displacements induced by vibratory stimulation of Tib-
ialis anterior or Gastrocnemius tendons are reduced or disappear and are
replaced by body lift illusion. They reappear by artificially increasing the
foot support by means of braces.
41. Mergner T, Hlavacka F, Schweigart G:
Interaction of vestibular
. and proprioceptive inputs. /
Vestibular
Res
1993, 3:41-57.
The study investigates the interaction of leg proprioceptive and vestibular
afferents for human self perception in space.
42. Schweigart G, Heimbrand S, Mergner T, Becker W:
Perception
. of horizontal head and trunk rotation: modification of neck
input following loss of vestibular function.
Exp Brain Res
1993,
95:533-546.
The horizontal head and trunk rotation perception is investigated in pa-
tients with chronic vestibular loss. In these patients, the trunk rotation
with respect to a stationary head is perceived as a rotation of the head
with respect to a stationary trunk. The normal perception is restored when
visual or somatosensory space reference is presented.
43. Gurfinkel VS, Levik YuS:
The suppression of the cervico-ocular
. response by the haptokinetic information about the contact
with a rigid, immobile object. Fxp Brain
Res
1993, 95:359-364.
Super slow trunk rotation with respect to a stationary head is perceived
as a head rotation with respect to a stationary trunk. Eye movements in
phase with the illusory head rotation are seen. Grasping a rigid handle
(haptic contact) restores perception of trunk rotation with respect to the
head.
44. Britton TC, Day BL, Brown P, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Mars-
. den CD:
Postural electromyographic responses in the arm and
leg following galvanic vestibular stimulation in man. Fxp
Brain
Res
1993, 94:143-151.
The effect of galvanic vestibular stimulation in man depends on support
conditions (feet, hands). Early and late EMG responses are described,
which are computed independently.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
51.
.
Wing AM, Goodrich S, Virji-Babul N, Jenner JR, Clapp S:
Balance evaluation in hemiparetic stroke patients using lat-
eral forces applied to the hip.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
1993,
74:292-299.
Maki BE, Ostrovski G:
Do postural responses to transient and
continuous perturbations show similar vision and amplitude
dependence? /
Biomech
1993,
26:
1181-l 190.
Jankowska E, Edgley S:
Interaction between pathways control-
ling posture and gait at the level of spinal interneurones.
in
Progress in brain research,
vol 97. Edited by Allum JHJ. Allum-
Mecklenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Sri-
ence Publishers B.V.; 1993:161-l 71.
Pratt CA, Fung J, Macpherson JM:
Stance control in the chronic
spinal cat.
J Neurophysiol
1994, 71:1981-1985.
Bernstein N:
The coordination and regulafion oi
movemenf.
London: Pergamon Press; 1967.
Macpherson JM:
How flexible are muscle synergies? In
Mofor
control: concepts and issues.
Edited by Humphrey DR, Freund
H-J. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 1991:33-47.
Van lngen Schenau GJ, Pratt CA, Marpherson JM: Differential
use and control of mono- and biarticular muscles. Human
MOV
Sci
1994, In press.
The respective role of mono- and biarticular muscles In multIJoIns move-
ments is analyzed in various tasks, in cat and human. As a whole, the
bialticular muscle activity is more flexible under the influence of motion-
related feedback and aimed at fine regulation of the distribution of net
moments over the limb joints.
52. Horak FB, Moore SP: The effect of prior leaning on human
. postural responses. C;ait &
Posfure
1993,
1:203-210.
886 Neural control
This study examines how human postural responses to stance disturbance
are altered by leaning about the ankles to five different initial stance
PO-
sitions.
53. Burleigh AL, Horak FB, Malouin F:
Modification of postural re-
sponses and step initiation: evidence for goal directed postural
interactions.
J Neurophysiol
1994, in press.
54. Allum JHJ, Honegger F, Schicks H:
Vestibular and propriocep-
. tive modulation of postural synergies in normal subjects.
J
Vestibular
Res
1993, 3:59-85.
The different muscle synergies underlying human balance control were
tested by combining rotation and translation of the support surface.
55. Forssberg H, Hirschfeld H:
Postural adjustments in sitting hu-
. mans following external perturbations: muscle activity and
kinematics. Exp Braain
Res 1994,
97:515-527.
Translation and rotation disturbances were tested in sitting subjects. The
results suggest that two levels of control can be identified: first, a pattern
of muscle activation is generated centrally, and second, the pattern is
shaped and timed by interaction from the entire somatosensory, vestibu-
lar and visual input.
56.
57.
58.
59.
60.
.
Macpherson JM:
The force constraint strategy for stance
is independent of prior experience. Exp
Brain
Res
1994,
101:397-405.
Mcllroy WE, Maki BE:
Task constraints on foot movements and
the incidence of compensatory stepping following perturbation
of upright stance.
Brain Res 1993,
616:30-38.
Maki BE, Whitelaw RS, Mcllroy WE:
Does frontal-plane asym-
metry in compensatory postural responses represent prepara-
tion for stepping?
Neurosci Lett 1993, 149:87-90.
Macpherson JM, lnglis JT:
Stance and balance following bi-
lateral labyrinthectomy.
In Progress in brain research, vol 97.
Edited by Allum JHJ, Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst
R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1993:219-228.
Horak FB, Shupert CL, Dietz V, Horstmann C:
Vestibular and
somatosensory contributions to responses to head and body
displacements in stance. Exp
Brain Res
1994, 100:93-l 06.
The relative contribution of vestibular and somatosensory information to
triggering postural responses to external body displacements was inves-
tigated by comparing responses to displacements of the head alone with
responses to displacement of head and body, in healthy subjects and pa-
tients with vestibular deficits.
61.
62.
63.
64.
65.
66.
.
Horak FB, Nashner LM, Diener HC:
Postural synergies asso-
ciated with somatosensory and vestibular loss. Exp
Brain Res
1990, 82:167-l 77.
Pastor MA, Day BL, Marsden CD:
Vestibular induced postural
responses in Parkinsons disease.
Brain
1993, 116:1177-l 190.
Wasterton )A, Hawken MB, Tanyeri S, Jantti P, Kennard
C:
Influence of sensory manipulation on postural control
in Parkinsons disease.
J Neural Neurosurg Psychiatry
1993,
56:1276-1281.
Pyykko I, Aalto H, Starck J, lshizaki H:
Postural stabilization on
a moving platform oscillating at high frequencies.
Aviat Space
Environ Med
1993,64:300-305.
Di Fabio RP, Anderson JH:
Effect of sway-referenced visual and
somatosensory inputs on human head movement and postural
patterns during stance.
J Vestibular Res 1993, 3:409-417.
lnglis IT, Horak FB, Shupert CL, Jones-Rycewicz C: The
impor-
tance of somatosensory information in triggering and scaling
automatic postural responses in humans.
fxp
Brain Res
1994,
100:159-164.
The role of lower limb somatosensory information in triggering and scal-
ing the postural responses in patients with sensory peripheral neuropathy
was tested. The postural response was delayed by 20-30ms and the scal-
ing of the response to both velocity and amplitude of surface translation
impaired. The lower limb sensory afferents play a role in both triggering
and scaling the postural responses.
67. Horak FB, Diener HC:
Cerebellar control of postural scaling
. and central set in stance.
J Neurophysiol
1994, in press.
The scaling of the automatic postural response was studied in patients
with anterior cerebellar deficits. Although the responses were hyperme-
tric, the earliest postural responses were scaled to platform velocity as
normal subjects. The main deficit was the lack of anticipatory scaling of
the amplitude of the responses.
68.
69.
70.
71.
72.
73.
74.
.
Straube A, Krafczyk S, Paulus W, Brandt T:
Dependence of
visual stabilization of postural sway on the cortical magnifi-
cation factor of restricted visual fields.
Exp Brain Res
1994,
99:501-506.
Previc FH, Kenyon RV, Boer ER, Johnson BH:
The effects of
background visual roll stimulation on postural and manual con-
trol and self-motion perception.
Perception &
Psychophysics
1993, 54:93-l 07.
Dijkstra TMH, Schoner C, Cielen CUM:
Temporal stability of
the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving
visual environment.
Exp Brain Res
1994, 97:477-486.
Timmann D, Belting C, Schwarz M, Diener HC:
Influence of
visual and somatosensory input on leg EMG responses in dy-
namic posturography in normals.
Electroencephalogr C/in Neu-
rophysiol
1994, 93:7-l 4.
Hoshiyama M, Watanabe S, Kaneoke Y, Koike Y, Takahashi A:
Effecl of optokinetic stimulation on human balance recovery
in unexpected forward fall.
Neurosci Res
1993, l&121-127.
Zennou-Azogui Y, Xerri C, Harlay F: Visual sensory substitution
in vestibular compensation: neuronal substrates in the alert
cat.
Exp Brain Res
1994, 98:457-473.
Dietz V, Trippel M, lbrahim IK, Berger W:
Human stance on
a sinusoidally translating platform: balance control by feed-
forward and feedback mechanisms.
Exp Brain Res 1923,
93:352-362.
Body inclination and EMC activity was tested in subjects standing on
a treadmill moving sinusoidally backward and forward. Both feedback
reactions to changes in sinusoidal frequency and feedforward control
during repeated cycles at the same frequency were analyzed. The au-
thors conclude that feedforward control acts on CC position relative to
the feet in order to minimize forces acting on the body during treadmill
movement.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
81.
82.
83.
84.
85.
Dietz V, Zijlstra W, Assaiante C, Tripple M, Berger W:
Balance
control in Parkinsons disease.
Gait & Posture
1993,
1:77-84.
Struppler A, Gerilovsky L, Jakob C:
Self-generated rapid taps
directed to opposite forearm in man: anticipatory reduction
in the muscle activity of the target arm.
Neurosci Lett
1993,
159:115-118.
Massion J: Movement, posture and equilibrium: interaction and
coordination.
Progress Neurobiol
1992, 38:35-56.
Forget R, Lamarre Y:
Postural adjustments associated with dif-
ferent unloading of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and
cutaneous afferent deprivation.
Can
J Pbysiol Pharmacol
1994,
in press.
Viallet F, Massion J, Bonnefoi-Kyriacou B, Aurenty R, Obadia
A, Khalil R:
Approche quantitative de Iasynergie posturale en
pathologie cMbelleuse.
Rev Neural
1994, 150:55-60.
Bouisset S, Duchene JL:
Is body balance more perturbated by
respiration in seating than in standing posture!
Neuroreport
1994, 5:957-960.
Rogers MW, Hedman LD, Pai YC:
Kinetic analysis of dy-
namic transitions in stance support accompanying voluntary
leg flexion movements in hemiparetic adults.
Arch Phys Med
Rehabil
1993, 74:19-25.
Diener HC, Bather M, Guschlbauer B, Thomas C, Dichgans
J:
The coordination of posture and voluntary movement in
patients with hemiparesis.
J Neural
1993, 240:161-167.
Diener HC, Dichgans J, Guschlbauer 8, Bather M, Rapp H,
Klockgether T:
The coordination of posture and voluntary
movement in patients with cerebellar dysfunction.
Mov Dis
1992, 7:14-22.
Horak F, Frank J:
Three separate postural systems affected
in Parkinsonism.
In
Motor control VII.
Edited by Stuart DC,
Cantcher GN, Curfinkal VS, Wiesendangez M. Tucson, Ari-
zona: Motor Control Press; in press.
Woollacott M, Assaiante C, Amblard B:
Development of bal-
ance and gait control.
In
Clinical aspects oi balance and gait
Postural control system
Massion 887
disorders. Edited by Bronstein A, Brandt T, Wooliacott M. City?:
Arnold Publ; 1994, in press.
86. Jouen F:
Head position and posture in newborn infants.
In The
head-neck sensory-motor system. Vol 7: Evolution, biomechan-
its and basic aspects. Edited by Berthoz A, Graf W, Vidal PP.
Chichester: John Wiley; 1994, in press.
87. Woollacott MH, Sveistrup H:
The development of sensorimo-
tor integration underlying posture control in infants during
the transition to independent stance.
In hterlimb coordina-
tion: neural, dynamical, and cognitive constrain&. Edited by
Swinnen S, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P. San Diego: Aca-
demic Press; 1994:371-389.
aa. Hirschfeld H, Forssberg H:
Epigenetic development of postu-
. ral responses for sitting during infancy.
Exp Brain Res 1994,
97:52a-540.
The ontogenesis of the postural reactions to platform translation is ana-
lyzed in sitting toddlers.
89. Sveistrup H, Woollacott MH:
Systems contributing to emer-
gence and maturation of stability in postural development. In
The
development of coordination in infancy. Edited by Savels-
bergh GJP. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1993:319-336.
90. Woollacott MH, Sveistrup H:
Changes in the sequencing and
timing of muscle response coordination associated with devel-
. .
opmental transItIons in balance abilities.
Human Movement
Science 1992,
11:23-36.
91. Assaiante C, Thomachot B, Aurenty R:
Hip stabilization and
. lateral balance control in toddlers during the first four months
of autonomous walking. Neuroreport
1993, 4:875-878.
The stabilization on hip, shoulder and head is analyzed during the first
four months of autonomous walking in toddlers.
92. Breniere Y, Bril B, Fontaine R:
Analysis of the transition from
upright stance to steady state locomotion in children with
under
200
days of autonomous walking. /
Motor Behav 1989,
21:20-37.
93.
94.
95.
96.
97.
98.
99.
100.
101.
.
Pozzo T, Berthoz A, Lefort L, Vitte E:
Head stabilization during
various locomotor tasks in humans. II. Patients with bilateral
peripheral vestibular deficits.
Exp Brain Res 1991, 85:208-2 17.
Woollacott MH:
Age-related changes in
ment. /
Gerontology 1993, 48:5&60.
Alexander NB:
Postural control in older
50~ i
994, 42:93-i 08.
posture and move-
adults. /
Am Geriat
Horak FB, Shupert CL, Mirka A:
Components of postural
dyscontrol in the elderly: a review.
Neurobiol Aging 1989,
io:727-738.
Whipple R, Wolfson L, Derby C, Singh D, Tobin I:
Altered
sensory function and balance in older persons. /
Geronrology
1993, 48:71-76.
Keshner EA, Allum JHJ, Honegger F:
Predictors of less sta-
ble postural responses to support surface rotation in healthy
human elderly. /
Vestibular Res 1993, 3:419-429.
Maki BE:
Biomechanical approach to quantifying anticipatory
postural adjustments in the elderly.
Med Go/ Eng Comput
1993,
31:355-362.
Teasdale N, Bard C, Larue J, Fleury M:
On the cognitive pen-
etrability of posture control.
Exp Aging Res 1993, 19:1-13.
Deliagina TG, Grillner S, Orlovsky CN, Ullen F: visual input
affects the response to roll in reticulospinal neurons of the
lamprey.
Exp Brain Res 1993, 95:421-428.
The role of visual input in body orientation and on reticulospinal neurons
activity is investigated in the lamprey.
J Massion, Laboratory
of Neurobiolob? and Movements, CNRS
31, Chemin Joseph Aiguier, 13402 Marseille Cedrx 20, France.
... Collectively, these observations and our current findings lead us to hypothesize that there exist largely distinct cerebellar pathways/modules for computing head-versus body-centered motion representations, each reflecting at least a partial solution to the tilt/translation ambiguity and subserving distinct functional roles. In particular, head-centered NU translation and tilt estimates may play a primary role in stabilizing the head and gaze in space, forming a stable earth-horizontal platform for integrating visual and vestibular cues that serves as a reference for perception of motion and orientation relative to the external world-essential for both motor (e.g., postural control; Massion, 1994) and more cognitive functions (e.g., navigation; Yoder and Taube, 2014;Laurens et al., 2016). Such a role is consistent with NU projections to brainstem nuclei, which mediate vestibulocollic reflexes and eye movement control (Barmack, 2003;Meng et al., 2014) and with deficits after NU lesions (Dow, 1938;Angelaki and Hess, 1995;Wearne et al., 1998;Tarnutzer et al., 2015). ...
... In contrast, pathways through the AV and rFN may play a more direct role in maintaining body postural equilibrium (Massion, 1994;Horak and Macpherson, 1996) and in estimating body motion for heading perception (Gu, 2018), path integration (Rochefort et al., 2013), and goal-directed action (Medendorp and Selen, 2017;Martin et al., 2021). This is consistent with rFN projections to brainstem regions involved in postural control and locomotion Homma et al., 1995;Fujita et al., 2020) and to thalamic nuclei projecting to parietal areas that encode body-centered motion (Akbarian et al., 1992;Meng et al., 2007;Chen et al., 2013). ...
Article
Many daily behaviors rely on estimates of our body's motion and orientation in space. Vestibular signals are essential for such estimates, but to contribute appropriately, two key computations are required. First, ambiguous motion information from otolith organs must be combined with spatially transformed rotational signals (e.g., from the canals) to distinguish head translation from tilt. Second, tilt and translation estimates must be transformed from a head- to a body-centered reference frame to correctly interpret the body's motion. Studies have shown that cells in the caudal cerebellar vermis (nodulus and ventral uvula, NU) reflect the output of the first set of computations to estimate translation and tilt. However, it remains unknown whether these estimates are encoded exclusively in head-centered coordinates or whether they reflect further transformation toward body-centered coordinates. Here, we addressed this question by examining how the 3D spatial tuning of otolith and canal signals on translation- and tilt-selective NU Purkinje cells in male rhesus monkeys varies with changes in head-re-body and body-re-gravity orientation. We show that NU cell tuning properties are consistent with head-centered otolith signal coding during translation. Furthermore, while canal signals in the NU have been transformed into a specific world-referenced rotation signal indicating reorientation relative to gravity (tilt), as needed to resolve the tilt/translation ambiguity, the resulting tilt estimates are encoded in head-centered coordinates. Our results thus suggest that body-centered motion and orientation estimates required for postural control, navigation, and reaching are computed elsewhere, either by further transforming NU outputs or via computations in other parallel pathways.
... Several models on the organization of teleokinetic and ereismatic components of the motor program have been proposed (Cordo & Nashner, 1982;DufossC, Hugon, Massion, & Padgnan, 1988;Massion, 1992Massion, , 1994. Many researchers have tried to show the feedforward and nonreflexive nature of postural adjustments that precede the voluntary movement (Friedh, Hallett, & Simon, 1984;Zattara & Bouisset, 1986, 1988Clement, 1987;Stelmach, P o p u h , & Miiller, 1990;Rogers, Kukulka, & Soderberg, 1992). ...
Article
Full-text available
A segmental teleokinetic movement performed from an upright standing position is usually preceded by a sequence of postural movements having the goal of counteracting the imbalance which will be caused by the forthcoming movement. This study had two aims, (1) to verify whether the postural component was programmed in the same way as that of the teleokinetic component and (2) to verify whether the postural component was specific to the direction and speed of the movement. 12 participants performed a pistol-shooting task involving a rapid arm-raising movement. Three directions, two time deadlines, and three types of cue were manipulated. This study showed that the postural component was sensitive to the cue effects, the characteristics of anticipatory postural adjustments were specific to the direction of the movement, and the characteristics of the postural muscle EMG burst were the same whether the participants programmed or deprogrammed and reprogrammed their movements for a given direction. The speed parameter did not seem to have any significant influence on the characteristics of the anticipatory postural adjustments.
... A primary purpose of posture control is to preserve body stability during voluntary movements and environmental interactions. [1][2][3][4][5] Gait, as full-body movement, has been described as a 'controlled falling' (Perry, 1992) and this is especially true during gait initiation when the motor system must cope with the challenge of safely translating the body from a stable stationary state to a new one. 6,7 Before the first step, to ensure stability, the nervous system executes a set of anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs) to avoid perturbations dangerous for stability. ...
Article
Background/Objectives: Low back pain (LBP) is the leading cause of disability worldwide, with most cases classified as non-specific low back pain (NSLBP). Various treatments exist, among which are physical exercises that promote flexibility, mobility and core stabilization, improving muscle function and body posture. The Canali Postural Method (CPM) is a kinesiological method that offers a personalized approach to postural reprogramming. This study compares the effects of the CPM and generic exercises in individuals with NSLBP. Methods: Subjects with NSLBP were engaged in a four-week intervention either based on a CPM reprogramming phase (CPM group) or generic exercises for the control group (CG). The CPM group underwent an assessment phase to identify the possible musculoskeletal causes of compensatory postural arrangements. The functional disability and pain level were assessed before, immediately after and 3 months post-intervention in both groups. Data were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA. Results: Thirty-five subjects per group participated, with the CPM group averaging 38.6 ± 10.1 years and the CG 40.2 ± 12.1 years. The CPM group experienced significantly greater pain relief both immediately post-intervention and at the 3-month follow-up (p < 0.001). While the disability perception decreased in both groups, the CPM group showed superior improvement at the 3-month follow-up (p < 0.001). Conclusions: The CPM represents promise for enhancing motor control and quality of life, suggesting potential benefits for other musculoskeletal issues. Future research should explore its broader applications and underlying physiological mechanisms.
Article
From a vestibular perspective, it is remarkable that dancers are often performing challenging tasks such as pirouettes, and yet manage to do so without falling. Some have suggested that dancers’ resistance to vertigo may be explained by a generalized suppression of vestibular signaling. Here, we aimed to test this hypothesis by examining the impact of galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) on postural control in dancers. A total of 38 participants were recruited for this study and were divided into two groups: 19 dancers and 19 healthy controls. Postural control was assessed at baseline and during GVS. As expected, dancers exhibited better postural control, as assessed by a decrease in sway area, when compared to the control group in the baseline condition. However, contrary to expectations, dancers did not differ from controls during GVS. This confirms that dancers’ resistance to vertigo cannot be explained by a generalized suppression of vestibular signaling. Rather, dancers may have developed a more accurate body representation due to top-down modulation of subcortical neuronal networks and may only be able to modify vestibular input during active movement.
Article
Background: Our study investigated the impact of multicomponent balance exercises (MBE) and sensorimotor mobilization with foot muscle strengthening (SMFE) on postural stability in patients with balance disorders, assessed using the Balance Error Scoring System (BESS) while standing still. Methods: Twenty postoperative patients were included in a randomized clinical study and divided into an MBE group (six women and four men with an average age of 30.2 years) and the SMFE group (six women and four men aged 34.5 years). Balance was assessed with the BESS on the third postoperative day and before discharge. The hospitalization lasted 10 days. Results: All patients in both groups showed significant clinical and statistical improvements (p < 0.05) in maintaining an upright posture after the BESS test. In the MBE group, 80% of patients achieved a minimum clinically significant change of 10 points in postural stability, whereas 100% of patients in the SMFE group did the same. The SMFE group exhibited a statistically significant improvement (p < 0.05) in specific balance tasks conducted on hard and soft surfaces. Conclusions: Our patient sample results suggest that SMFE is more effective than MBE. We recommend its use in early rehabilitation, although further research is necessary.
Article
This study investigated cortical reorganization in older adults following short-term interactive balance training. Twenty participants aged 65–74 received training in stabilometer stance, visually aligning plate movement with a horizontal line on a monitor. Pre-test and post-test measured posture fluctuations and scalp EEG during stabilometer stance. Results showed a training-related decrease in root mean square (RMS) (p = 0.001) and an increase in mean frequency (p = 0.006) of posture fluctuations. Despite a decline in theta relative power in Fp1 (p = 0.027), stabilometer training led to a post-test increase in alpha relative power around electrodes of the ventral visual pathway (p = 0.002). Additionally, augmentations were noted in theta relative power in Tp8 (p = 0.033) and beta relative power in F7 (p = 0.039). Analysis of the minimum spanning tree (MST) of alpha inter-regional connectivity indicated a training-related decrease in leaf fraction (p = 0.011) and increase in average eccentricity (p = 0.041), respectively. Training-related changes in the RMS of posture fluctuation were positively correlated with changes in pooled alpha relative power in electrodes of the ventral visual pathway (r = 0.459, p = 0.042) and negatively correlated with changes in average eccentricity of the alpha MST network (r = − 0.487, p = 0.029). In conclusion, short-term interactive training enhances balance by reorganizing regional and alpha-band network activities, which supports improved visual attention and prevents early visual processing idling during initial postural learning.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter discusses theory in which it is argued that during complex movements, the head is stabilized intermittently under the control of gaze, and that this stabilization allows the head to serve as an inertial guidance platform for the control of multilimb movement. The otolithic organs, sacculus and utriculus, are inertial detectors of linear acceleration of the head in the plane of their macula. It is well established that in normal gravity conditions, although the perceived gravitational vertical deviates from the objective vertical by a few degrees, gravitational reference is used not only for posture but also for perceptual tasks that involve orientation in space, including mental rotation. The movement is organized in successive steps, during which the head is stabilized at different angles, allowing the brain to utilize a series of different configurations of sensory inputs.
Chapter
Upper trunk bending in humans is always accompanied by lower segment movements in the opposite direction, as first described qualitatively by Babinski (1899). He called this phenomenon “axial synergy” and suggested that it may serve to maintain equilibrium.
Chapter
This chapter presents several analytical models that explore the way different constraints, or sets of constraints, can account for coordination and goal achievement in whole-body actions performed by standing subjects. These models are important because they enable multidimensional coordinative patterns to be quantitatively analyzed. The engineering-based approaches have adopted impedance-based models for controlling joint motion and equilibrium during standing. While scientists generally agree that some kinds of constraints mediate coordinated action in systems with many degrees of freedom, less agreement exists about the precise nature, level, and mechanisms by which constraints emerge or are imposed. It seems likely that future investigations into the mechanisms of coordination during whole-body actions will increasingly combine analytical and empirical methodologies.
Chapter
The theory of perception-action proposed by Gibson (1977), and further expanded upon in the area of development by von Hofsten (1983) among others, is based on the perspective that the human is a perceiving and active participant in movement. There are multiple sensory systems that contribute to our perceptions of our movements, postures and interactions with the environment including the visual, vestibular and somatosensory systems. Over time, these perceptions have been hypothesized to create an internal representation or schema of our body and its relationship to the environment (Gurfinkel & Levik, 1979). This internal representation is not necessarily an anatomical mapping of body parts but may consist of a set of functional input-output relationships or internal reference conditions. Other neural subsystems that cannot be classified as perceptual systems but that also contribute to our ability to regulate and control posture and movement and influence the development of independent postural control and the development of the internal representation include the motor system, higher level integrative systems and attentional systems. Postural control and movement are also constrained by the physical structure of our body and non-neural systems such as the muscular and skeletal systems as well as by the physical environment within which we live and move (i.e., gravity). The environment within which we live (i.e., gravity or the external conditions we encounter) and the physical structure of our body (i.e., number of segments, type ofjoints between segments, etc.) influence our immediate perceptions and, over time, these perceptions result in modifications to the internal representations or schema of our body and its relationship with the environment. Interest in the development of each of these individual neural and musculoskeletal subsystems as well as their changing contributions to the control and regulation of posture and movement is driving much of the present research in motor control. The focus of this chapter is to investigate the varying contributions of the individual subsystems to the development of independent posture control. We will also suggest that the study of developing systems wiU allow us to ldentify the form of the internal representation as well as its developmental sequence.
Chapter
The early development of head-neck and postural control systems has garnered interest not only from developmental psychologists but from researchers from other domains as well. Until recently, however, this field had been ruled by an essentially maturationistic approach and has not extended much beyond more or less accurate descriptions of observed postural behavior. Major aspects of postural development are elaborated in terms of the central nervous system. Recent research on infants' sensory-motor systems has revealed an intricacy overlooked by a maturationistic perspective. Therefore, reconsidering early postural development in a wider framework is a must, combining all the different inputs involved from proprio to ex-proprioceptive signals.
Article
The Stoffregen and Riccio (1988) hypothesis that perceived orientation is determined primarily by balance dynamics was tested. Perception of orientation was evaluated in the context of a task that required Ss to control the roll orientation of a device in which they were seated. The device's direction of balance was manipulated across trials and thus was independent of gravity. Eighteen Ss participated in the investigation. After each trial, Ss estimated their mean tilt with respect to upright. Correlations of perceived tilt with tilt from balance were consistently higher than the correlations with gravity tilt. The dominance of balance over gravity depended on the magnitude of tilt from balance.
Article
Most purposive movements are multi-joint actions in which specific patterns of joint moments are necessary to achieve the required segmental accelerations and to exert the appropriate direction and magnitude of force on the environment. Both human and animal studies suggest that mono- and biarticular muscles have different roles in these complex movements. Monoarticular muscles appear to show simple flexor or extensor activation patterns closely related to the required joint displacements, whereas biarticular muscles often exhibit complex multiple bursts of activity aimed at a fine-regulation of the distribution of net moments over the joints of the limb. Different patterns of activation of mono- and biarticular muscles have been observed during natural movements and in response to sudden perturbations or peripheral afferent inputs. Evidence is provided that the control of mono- and biarticular muscles is based on different processes and different sources of information. Monoarticular muscles may be more rigidly grouped into simple flexor or extensor synergies, whereas biarticular muscle activity may be more flexibly sculpted by motion-related feedback from peripheral afferents. Little is known about where and how the integration between perceptual information and the control of net joint moment is realized.
Article
It has been hypothesized that animals go through phases in behavioral development where movements initially show excessive degrees of freedom, then are simplified with skill mastery, and are re-elaborated with learning to reach the same goal through variable means. To determine if this is true for stages seen in children's balance development we explored changes in balance abilities in infants (7–15 months) during the transition to independent stance. With the emergence of ‘pull to stand’ behavior infants showed excessive movement at the ankle, knee and hip joint and postural muscle responses were disorganized, with delayed and variable onset latencies. Response organization improved with experience in dependent stance. At the onset of independent stance, response patterns again showed increased variability. However, with increased experience and the onset of independent walking the response organization became consistent and latencies were shortened to more mature levels. With continued experience children began to experiment with alternate strategies of balance adjustments.