ArticlePDF Available

Attachment Behavior in Dogs (Canis familiaris): A New Application of Ainsworth's (1969) Strange Situation Test

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Fifty-one owner-dog pairs were observed in a modified version of M. D. S. Ainsworth's (1969) Strange Situation Test. The results demonstrate that adult dogs (Canis familiaris) show patterns of attachment behavior toward the owner. Although there was considerable variability in dogs' attachment behavior to humans, the authors did not find any effect of gender, age, living conditions, or breed on most of the behavioral variables. The human-dog relationship was described by means of a factor analysis in a 3-dimensional factor space: Anxiety, Acceptance, and Attachment. A cluster analysis revealed 5 substantially different classes of dogs, and dogs could be categorized along the secure-insecure attached dimensions of Ainsworth's original test. A dog's relationship to humans is analogous to child-parent and chimpanzee-human attachment behavior because the observed behavioral phenomena and the classification are similar to those described in mother-infant interactions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Comparative Psychology
1998, Vol. 112, No. 3,219-229
Attachment Behavior in Dogs
(Canis familiaris):
A New Application of Ainsworth's (1969) Strange Situation Test
József Topál, Á
dám Miklósi, Vilmos Csányi,
and
Antal Dóka
Loránd Eötvös
University
Fifty-one owner-dog pairs were observed in a modified version of M. D. S. Ainsworth's
(1969) Strange Situation Test. The results demonstrate that adult dogs
(Canis familiaris)
show
patterns of attachment behavior toward the owner. Although there was considerable variability
in dogs' attachment behavior to humans, the authors did not find any effect of gender, age,
living conditions, or breed on most of the behavioral variables. The human-dog relationship
was described by means of a factor analysis in a 3-dimensional factor space:
Anxiety,
Acceptance,
and
Attachment.
A cluster analysis revealed 5 substantially different classes of
dogs, and dogs could be categorized along the secure-insecure attached dimensions of
Ainsworth's original test. A dog's relationship to humans is analogous to child-parent and
chimpanzee-human attachment behavior because the observed behavioral phenomena and the
classification are similar to those described in mother-infant interactions.
Although the construct of attachment was first used to
explain the affectional bond that develops between a human
infant and its caregiver (Bowlby, 1958), this concept has
been elaborated for behavioral phenomena that are fundamen-
tal in social species and has been approached in a number of
ways over the years. It was considered a hypothetical factor
that ties individuals together (Lorenz, 1966) or a behavior
system that results in one individual seeking and maintain-
ing proximity to another individual (Bowlby, 1972). Cohen
(1974) defined
attachment
as a special affectional relation-
ship between two individuals that is specific in its focus and
endures over time. This relationship is based on dependency
between individuals that becomes evident through behav-
ioral preferences (Wickler, 1976).
The theories of attachment behavior range from the
psychoanalytic approach (Freud, 1946) through the different
learning theories (Cairns, 1966; Gewirtz, 1972; Hoffman &
Ratner, 1973; Solomon & Corbit, 1973) to the ethological
model of attachment (Ainsworth, 1969, 1972; Bowlby,
1958, 1969; for a review, see Rajecki, Lamb, & Obmascher,
1978). The ethological approach uses the term
attachment
in
evolutionary and developmental contexts and emphasizes
that attachment systems are neurobiological structures that
have been shaped by the normal environment and that
function best in that environment (Bowlby, 1958; Kraemer,
1992). The ultimate function of attachment may be to defend
against predation (Bowlby, 1969) or to obtain necessary
József
Topál, Á
dám
Miklósi, Vilmos Csányi,
and
Antal Dóka,
Department of Ethology,
Loránd Eötvös
University,
Göd,
Hungary.
We would like to thank
Krisztina Soproni, Márta
Gácsi, and
Szima Naderi for their assistance during the observations and
analysis. This study was supported by Grant T-016-013/1995 from
the Hungarian Foundation for Basic Research and by a grant from
Masters Food, Hungary.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
József Topál or
Ádám Miklósi,
Department of Ethology,
Loránd
Eötvös
University, Jávorka S. U. 14, H-2131
Göd,
Hungary. Electronic
mail may be sent to
jtopal@ludens.elte.h
u
or miklosa@ludens.elte.hu.
219
Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association,
Inc.
0735-7036/98/$3.00
resources that are provided by the caregiver. Nevertheless,
perhaps attachment has no distinct function but simply
evolved as a consequence of a close relationship between
two individuals (Gubernick, 1981).
Attachment is an organizational construct (J. P Connel &
Goldsmith, 1982), a product of maturation that always
denotes a one-to-one relationship with a particular other and
manifests itself in different behaviors (Sears,
Whiting,
Novlis, & Sears, 1953). Although the operational criteria of
attachment have been developed from research on humans
and other primates, they can be applied to other species.
Attachment presumes (a) the ability to discriminate and
respond differentially to the object of attachment (i.e., the
secure-base effect), (b) a preference for the attachment
figure (e.g., proximity and contact seeking and maintenance
of proximity), and (c) a response to separation from and
reunion with the attachment figure that is distinct from
responses to others (Crnic, Reite, & Shucard, 1982; Guber-
nick, 1981; Rajecki et al., 1978).
One of the most important methodological approaches
with respect to the assessment of attachment is the Strange
Situation Test elaborated by Ainsworth and Wittig (1969).
This laboratory procedure was originally designed (Ains-
worth, 1969) to examine the balance of attachment and
exploratory behaviors under conditions of low and high
stress. Researchers hypothesized that during the experimen-
tal
conditions, the attachment behavior is activated by
separation from and reunion with the attachment figure (see
Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978, for an assessment
of the human infant-parent attachment). Infant responses to
this situation are customarily classified as fitting into one of
three overall patterns of behavioral organization:
secure
(the
infant shows signs of missing the parent upon separation,
greets the parent actively upon reunion, and then settles and
returns to play; identified as Category B);
insecure-avoidant
(the infant shows little or no distress at separation from the
parent and actively avoids and ignores the parent upon
reunion; Category A); or
insecure-resistant
(the infant is
220
TOPÁL,
MIKLÓSI,
CSÁNYI,
AND
DÓKA
highly distressed by separation and seeks for contact on
reunion but cannot be settled by the parent and may show
strong resistance; Category C). Recently, Main and Solomon
(1990) described an additional insecure attachment pattern
and called it the
disorganized pattern
(Category D). Because
the Strange Situation paradigm is based on the evolutionary
approach of attachment behavior, some authors have success-
fully adapted it to ethological studies. For example, research-
ers have found that during separation from and reunion with
cagemates, chimpanzees reacted similarly to children (Bard,
1983, 1991; Miller, Bard, Juno, & Nadler, 1986) and that not
only conspecifics but also humans could serve as attachment
figures for young chimpanzees (Miller, Bard, Juno, &
Nadler, 1990).
With the exception of primate studies, only a limited
number of studies have explored the animal to human
attachment. One of the most promising subjects for investi-
gating this topic is the dog
(Canis familiaris).
The origin of
the dog-human relationship dates back through 10,000 years
of domestication. The dog's ancestor was originally a social
species (see, e.g., Serpell, 1995). Dogs were selectively bred
not only for "sociocognitive abilities" and for "attachment
to humans" (Millot, 1994), but also for "infantile" features
(Coppinger et al., 1987). The
human-dog
relationship is in
some sense similar to the parent-child relationship (i.e.,
asymmetrical and dependency based), and people are apt to
consider their dog as a child substitute (Collis, 1995). As
attachment theory presumes, "Dogs seem to miss their
owners during an absence and will appear excited upon the
owner's return" (New, 1995, p. 25). Dogs seem to be
innately responsive to humans so that not even strict
punishment conditions can extinguish the proximity seeking
of pups to a handler (Fisher, 1955). The dog's orientation to
humans is also supported by Pettijohn, Wont, Ebert, and
Scott (1977), who found that separation distress in puppies
was greatly reduced when a human being was nearby.
On the other hand, because the evidence for using a
mother as a secure base is poor for dogs (Elliot & Scott,
1961; Frederickson, 1952; Ross, Scott, Cherner, & Denen-
berg, 1960) and the puppies' separation distress can be
reduced also by nonconspecifics (Cairns & Werboff, 1967;
Pettijohn et al., 1977), attachment behavior in dogs has been
questioned by some researchers (Rajecki et al., 1978).
However, it is important to distinguish the "attachment"
behavior of puppies that still have not developed individual
relationships with their owners from the attachment of adult
dogs that have more individualized bonds to their human
masters. The calming effect that the presence of a human has
on puppies might simply be the result of general distress
reduction by a supposed conspecific and thus has no direct
relation to a dog's later attachment to a person.
Nevertheless,
most researchers assume that the accep-
tance of human beings as conspecifics and the motherlike
and effective security-providing role of humans for dis-
tressed puppies are results of domestication. For 10,000
years, artificial selection in dogs favored socialization with
humans as if they were conspecifics (Kretchmer & Fox,
1975). The genetic changes regarding a dog's capacity for
conspecific recognition might have played a key role in this
process that resulted in a preference for humans. A dog's
preparedness for forming a bond with humans, a bond that is
rooted in the evolutionary past, would be a prerequisite for
the development of attachment between a particular person
(the owner) and the dog itself.
Apart from some questionnaire studies that addressed the
psychological features of the dog-human bond (e.g., Barker
& Barker, 1988; Cox, 1993; Voith, Wright, & Danneman,
1992), to date there have been no experimental studies that
aimed to give a behavioral description of dog-human attach-
ment. We may suppose that for dogs in strange situations, as for
children, it is not only the separation from the attachment figure
(owner) but the reunion with her or him that activates the dogs'
attachment behavior. So the application of Ainsworth's
(1969) Strange Situation Test could provide useful informa-
tion regarding the owner-dog relationship. Additionally, by
using analogous methods, researchers can see how the
similarity of observed behavioral phenomena and the organi-
zational system of owner-dog interactions could provide a
useful model for human infant attachment.
The purposes of this article are to demonstrate adult dogs'
attachment behavior toward humans, to describe the human-
dog relationship by an ethological method used for evalua-
tion of
mother-child
attachment, and to study the similarity
of owner-dog relationships in the form of mother-child
interactions.
Method
Participants
Fifty-one owner-dog pairs volunteered for our experiment from
kennel clubs in the vicinity. In the group of owners, there were 31
women and 20 men whose ages ranged from 13 to 60 years
(
M
= 30.1
±
8.5 years). In the group of dogs
(
Canis familiaris),
there were 28 males and 23 females whose ages ranged from 1 to
10 years
(
M
= 3.12 ± 0.40 years). The dogs were from 20 different
pure breeds: Belgian Shepherd
(n
= 17), English Setter
(n
= 3),
Laika
(n
= 3), Staffordshire Terrier (n = 2), Hungarian
Vizsla
(n = 2), Irish
Wolfhound
(n = 2), German Shepherd (n = 2),
Briard (n = 1), Bobtail (n = 1), Caucasian Shepherd (n = 1),
Czech Wolf (n = 1), Doberman Pinscher (n = 1), Great Dane
(n = 1), Husky (n = 1), Irish Setter (n = 1), Giant Schnauzer
(n = 1), Golden
Retriever
(n = 1),
Komondor
(n = 1), Newfound-
land (n = 1), and Spaniel (n = 1). There were also mixed-breed
dogs in our sample
(n
= 7). According to the American Kennel
Club's (A.K.C.'s) classification, the dogs fit into five groups:
sporting dogs
(n
= 11), nonsporting dogs (n = 3), working dogs
(n = 27),
terriers
(n
= 3), and mixed breeds
(n
= 7). (The A.K.C.
has divided recognized breeds into six main groups on the basis of
behavioral characteristics rather than phylogeny.)
Experimental Arrangement
The basic experimental setup and the protocol were as similar as
possible to that of Ainsworth et al. (1978). The novel environment
was a relatively empty rectangular room (6 m longX
3
m
wide
X
2.5 m high) containing two chairs. At one end of the room
(opposite the door), there were toys for dogs on the floor. The
14.5-min procedure consisted of an introductory episode and seven
experimental episodes. The behavior of the dogs was videotaped
and analyzed later.
Experimental Episodes of the Strange Situation
Procedure
Introductory episode (30 s).
The observer introduces the owner
and dog to the experimental room and leaves.
Episode 1 (2 min): owner and dog.
The owner is a nonpartici-
pant while the dog explores. After 1.5 min, a signal (a knock on the
wall) is given to the owner who stimulates play.
Episode 2 (2 min): stranger, owner, and dog.
A stranger enters
and sits down. After 30 s, she initiates conversation with the owner.
At the 2nd-min mark, the stranger approaches the dog and tries to
stimulate playing. At the end of this episode, the owner leaves as
unobtrusively as possible, but the dog's leash remains on the chair.
Episode 3 (2 min): stranger and dog.
This is the first separa-
tion episode. The stranger's behavior is geared to that of the dog.
During the 1st min, the stranger tries to engage the dog and keep
him or her out of the door by playing. If the dog is not ready to play,
the stranger tries to engage the dog by petting. At the 2nd
min-mark, the stranger stops playing. If the dog initiates petting, it
is permitted.
Episode 4 (2 min): owner and dog.
This is the first reunion
episode. The owner approaches the closed door and calls the dog.
The owner opens the door and pauses a moment to allow the dog to
respond. The owner greets and comforts the dog. Meanwhile, the
stranger leaves. After 2 min, the owner leaves and says to the dog
"stay here." The leash is left on the chair.
Episode 5 (2 min): dog alone.
This is the second separation
episode.
Episode 6 (2 min): stranger and dog.
This is a continuation of
the second separation. The stranger enters and gears her behavior to
that of the dog. During the 1 st min, the stranger tries to engage the
dog and keep him or her out of the door by playing. If the dog is not
ready to play, the stranger tries to engage the dog by petting. At the
2nd min-mark, the stranger stops playing. Petting is permitted if it
is initiated by the dog.
Episode 7 (2 min): owner and dog.
This is the second reunion
episode. The owner opens the door and pauses a moment before
greeting the dog, giving him or her an opportunity to respond
spontaneously.
Then the owner greets and comforts the dog.
Meanwhile, the stranger leaves.
To conduct the Strange Situation Test in a standard manner, we
gave several instructions (see Appendix) to the stranger (who was
the same woman in all cases). The owners did not know anything
about the real goals and the hypotheses of the study in advance;
they were informed that this study was to examine the exploratory
behavior of the dogs in a strange situation.
Observations and Behavior Categories
Two trained observers analyzed the 51 videotaped sessions using
eight behavior categories. Each behavior listed was scored for both
owner and stranger. Recorded variables were as follows: explora-
tion in the presence of the owner
(EXPO)
and in the presence of the
stranger (EXPS), playing in the presence of the owner (PLYO) and
in the presence of the stranger (PLYS), passive behaviors in the
presence of the owner (PASO) and in the presence of the stranger
(PASS), physical contact with the owner (CONTO) and with the
stranger (CONTS), and standing by the door in the presence of the
owner (SBYO) and in the presence of the stranger (SBYS). The
relative percentage of the time spent with these behaviors was
established, and the relative duration of each behavioral variable
was summed across Episodes 1-7 for the statistical analysis.
We also analyzed the greeting behavior of the dogs toward the
owners during the reunion episodes (Episodes 4 and 7) and toward
ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS
the entering stranger (Episodes 2 and 6). Greeting was character-
ized by proximity of, contact seeking by, and contact maintenance
of the dogs toward the entering owner (COSO, DCONTO, and
DELO) and toward the stranger (COSS, DCONTS, and DELS).
Interobserver agreement was assessed by means of parallel
coding of 20% of the total sample (10 strange situation sessions).
Behavior was point sampled every 10 s (for assessing confidence
for Elements 1-5), and the greeting episodes were evaluated
separately for assessing confidence. We assessed agreement in two
ways: percentage agreement and Cohen's kappa, a statistic that
corrects for chance agreement (Martin & Bateson, 1986). The
descriptions for behavior categories and the kappa and percentage
scores are given in Table 1.
Analysis of Data
Results
221
We recorded behavioral data continuously during observations,
and we calculated the relative percentage of the time spent in each
behavior. Although some of the variables had a normal distribution,
in eight cases the transformation of raw data was necessary for
parametric statistical
methods. To achieve normality, we had to
perform a square-root transformation for PLYS, PASO, PASS,
CONTS, SBYO, DELO, and DCONTS and a log transformation
for DELS.
The behavior that the dogs exhibited in the presence of owner
and the stranger was compared using two-tailed
t
tests. We studied
the correlation pattern of the dog's behavior in the strange situation
using a factor analysis, which was also used to get theoretical
dimensions (superordinate variables) thought to account for indi-
vidual differences in a set of behaviors observed in the Strange
Situation Test (J. P Connel & Goldsmith, 1982).
We then reanalyzed all the behavioral variables by cluster
analysis to classify the individuals according to their strange
situation behavior and to establish categories for the dog-human
relationship. In previous human studies, researchers also explored
the quantitative consistency of the Ainsworth (1969) system using
cluster analysis, and they found that the traditional A, B, and C
classifications (see above) were more or less relevant (D. B.
Connel, 1976; Gardner & Thompson, 1983) to the distinct clusters.
Nevertheless, the categorization along the secure versus insecure
dimension has seemingly more predictive power than the A, B, and
C groups (Arend, Gove, & Sroufe, 1979; Matas, Arend, & Sroufe,
1978; Waters, Wippman, & Sroufe, 1979).
Finally,
we analyzed the effects of independent variables (i.e.,
the owner's sex and the dog's gender and breed) on the strange
situation behavior using three-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
and correlation analysis (the dog's age and the number of family
members). We also analysed the effect of breed differences on the
strange situation behavior using an ANOVA and an F test for
equality of variances on a matched-pair sample (a homogeneous
and a heterogeneous subgroup of dogs).
Dogs' Behavior in the Presence of the Owners Versus
the Stranger
The dogs tended to play more (PLYO vs. PLYS:
t[50] = 5.4, p < 0.0001)
and spent more time exploring
(EXPO
vs. EXPS:
t[50] = 2.5,p =0.013) in the presence of
their owners (Figure 1). Passive behaviors and physical
contact did not show significant differences (PASO vs.
PASS: t[50] = 1.5, p = ns;
CONTO vs. CONTS:
t[50] = 1.5,
22
2
TOPÁL,
MIKLÓSI,
CSANYI, AND
DÓKA
Table 1
Behavioral Variables Observed in the Strange Situation Test
Variable
%
Cohen's
agreement
a
Note.
EXPO
= exploration in the presence of owner; EXPS = exploration in the presence of
stranger; PLYO = playing with owner present; PLYS = playing with stranger present; PASO =
passive behavior in presence of owner; PASS = passive behavior in presence of stranger; CONTO =
physical contact with owner; CONTS = physical contact with stranger; SBYO = standing by door
with owner present; SBYS = standing by door with stranger present; COSO = contact seeking with
entering owner; COSS = contact seeking with entering stranger; DELO = delay of contact seeking
with owner; DELS = delay of contact seeking with stranger; DCONTO = duration of physical
contact while greeting entering owner; DCONTS = duration of physical contact while greeting
entering stranger.
p = ns,
respectively). During the separation episodes (i.e.,
owner absent), the dogs stood by the door more than when
the owner was present (SBYS vs. SBYO:
t[50] = 10.7,
p < 0.0001). Furthermore, dogs showed higher levels of
contact seeking toward the entering owner compared with
the stranger
(COSO vs. COSS: t[50] = 7.2, p < 0.0001).
In
the case of the former, we also noticed a shorter delay of
contact seeking (DELO vs. DELS:
t[50] = 7.0, p < 0.0001)
Relative duration of playing (left panel) and exploration (right panel) behaviors in the
Figure 1.
presence of the owner versus the stranger. *p < .02.
1.
Exploration: activity directed toward nonmovable aspects of the envi-
ronment, including sniffing, distal visual inspection
(staring
or scan-
ning), close visual inspection, or oral examination;
EXPO
and EXPS.
96
0.90
2.
Playing: any vigorous, toy- or social partner-related behavior,
including running, jumping, or any physical contact with toys (chew-
ing, biting); PLYO and PLYS.
98
0.92
3.
Passive behaviors: sitting, standing, or lying down without any orienta-
tion toward the environment; PASO and PASS.
96
0.92
4.
Physical contact; CONTO and CONTS.
100
1.00
5.
Stand by the door: the time spent close to the door (<1 m) with the
face oriented to the exit; SBYO and SBYS.
99
0.97
6.
The score of contact seeking; that is, the sum of the following scores:
approach initiation (+1); full approach, characterized by physical con-
tact (+2); any sign of avoidance behavior (-1); COSO and COSS.
88
0.73
7.
Delay of contact seeking: the amount of time (in s) from the moment
of the opening of the door to the first sign of approaching behavior;
DELO and DELS. (If approach was not recorded, DELO or DELS was
considered to be the duration of full episode, or 120 s.)
98
0.96
8.
Duration of physical contact while greeting; DCONTO and DCONTS.
100
1.00
ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS
223
and a longer duration of first physical contact
(DCONTO vs.
DCONTS: t(50) = 5.9, p < 0.0001; see Figure 2).
Factor Analysis
Factor analysis performed on 14 behavioral variables
(PASO, PASS, PLYO, PLYS, CONTO, CONTS,
SBYO,
SBYS, COSO, COSS, DELO, DELS, DCONTO,
and
DCONTS) resulted in three rotated factors (varimax rota-
tion, eigenvalue > 1.5) that accounted for 57% of the total
variability (29%, 15%, and 13% respectively). For the first
factor, five behavioral variables were represented by high
loadings (> 0.55). Individuals that scored high on this factor
behaved passively (i.e., they did not play and spent long
amounts of time exhibiting passive behaviors in the presence
of the stranger compared with the owner) and strove for
physical contact with the owner. So, this factor related to the
stressfulness of the strange situation and can be referred to as
the
Degree ofAnxiety.
Because the second factor is character-
ized by long-lasting physical contact with the stranger
(CONTS
and
DCONTS)
and by a high level of contact
seeking toward the entering stranger (COSS), it can be
Figure 2.
Scores of contact seeking toward the entering owner versus the stranger (upper right
panel) and relative duration (in seconds) of different behaviors: stand by the door in the presence of
the owner versus the stranger (upper left panel), delay of contact seeking toward the entering owner
versus the stranger (lower left panel), and physical contact with the owner versus the stranger while
greeting (lower right panel). *p < .001.
22
4
TOPÁL,
MIKLÓSI,
CSÁNYI,
AND
DÓKA
referred to as the
Acceptance of the Presence of the Stranger.
The third one is characterized by the high level of contact
seeking toward the owner with low (if any) delay of
approach and by permanent greeting contact with the
entering owner (COSO, DELO, and DCONTO). So this
factor is related to the
owner-dog
relationship and can be
referred to as the factor
of Attachment.
The results
of
this
analysis are given in Table 2.
Cluster Analysis
Using the same variables as the factor analysis, we
calculated a hierarchical cluster analysis. Visual examina-
tion
of
the
dendrogram
(Figure 3) revealed that the dogs
could be divided into three separate groups, and Group 1 and
Group 3 each consisted
of
2 subgroups (la, lb and 3a, 3b).
The number
of
dogs in each group was as follows: la
(n =
12),
lb
(n =
18), 2
(n = 5),
3a
(n = 9),
and 3b
(n = 7).
The categorization
of
dogs into these groups was further
supported by the results
of
post hoc ANOVA tests on the
behavioral variables using the groups as independent vari-
ables.
We found significant differences among the groups in
Table 2
Factor Loadings of Behavioral Variables
Note.
PLYO =
playing with owner present;
PLYS =
playing
with stranger present;
PASO =
passive behavior in presence of
owner;
PASS =
passive behavior in presence of stranger; CONTO =
physical contact with owner; CONTS = physical contact with
stranger; SBYO = standing by door with owner present; SBYS =
standing by door with owner absent; COSO = contact seeking with
entering owner; COSS = contact seeking with entering stranger;
DELO = delay of contact seeking with owner; DELS = delay of
contact seeking with stranger; DCONTO = duration of physical
contact while greeting entering owner; DCONTS = duration of
physical contact while greeting entering stranger. Boldface indi-
cates behavioral variables with high loadings
(>.55)
on the three
significant factors.
Figure 3.
The result of the cluster analysis for Groups la, lb,
2,
3a, and 3b. * = dogs of homogeneous group (Belgian Shepherds);
x = dogs of heterogeneous group.
10 of
the 16 observed behavioral variables. Furthermore, the
post hoc Duncan multiple-range test showed significant
ranges for a given dependent variable (this result revealed a
characteristic behavior pattern for the groups). According to
this latter analysis, the level
of
any behavioral variable could be
low (L), medium (M), or high (H) in a given group (Table 3).
Because individual factor scores were also calculated, we
used them for comparison
of
cluster analysis groups. This
comparison resulted in significant differences for all three
factorial variables (see, Table 4 and Figure 4), providing
further evidence for the homogeneity
of
the dogs within a
cluster group.
Effects of
Independent Variables on Strange Situation
Behavior
We analyzed the observed behavioral variables and the
three factor scores
of
the dogs by a three-way ANOVA using
the independent variables recorded in this sample (i.e., the
owner's sex, and the dog's gender and breed), but they had
no significant effect on these variables.
Behavior Anxiety
Factor
Acceptance Attachment
Playing
PLYO
-.91 -.07
-.06
PLYS
-.89
.
01
-.20
Passive behavior
PASO
.91
.
02
-.18
PASS
.80
.
25
-.19
Physical contact
CONTO
.59
.
22
.
22
CONTS
.
27
.77
-.05
Stand by the door
SBYO
.
20
-.16
.
48
SBYS
.
07
-.46
.
47
Score of contact seeking
COSO
-.10
.
11
.67
COSS
-.O1
.58
-.07
Delay of contact seeking
DELO
.
09
.
21
-.71
DELS
-.21
.
06
.
37
Duration of physical contact
DCONTO
.
15
.
46
.61
DCONTS
.
18
.78
.
12
The effects of the dog's age and the number of family
members on the strange situation behavior were analyzed by
correlation analysis. Only the number of family members
correlated significantly with some of the variables (SBYS:
-0.38, PASS: 0.43, p < 0.01
in each). That is, dogs living in
large families tended to spend less time close to the door and
showed more passive behaviors in the presence of the
stranger.
The Effect of Breed Differences on Strange
Situation Behavior
To study the effect of breed on the dog's behavior, we
selected two subgroups from the
51
subjects. Subgroup 1
was a homogeneous group; it included the 17 Belgian
Shepherds. Subgroup
2
was a heterogeneous group; it
included 17 dogs of different breeds (a German Shepherd, an
Irish Setter, a Giant Schnauzer, a Briard, a Staffordshire
Terrier, a
Golden Retriever, a Newfoundland, an Irish
Wolfhound, a Caucasian Shepherd, a Laika, a Hungarian
Vizsla,
a Bobtail, and
5
dogs of mixed breed). The two
groups were matched regarding the owners' sex and the sex
of the dogs. Subgroups 1 and
2
were also balanced for all of
the other independent variables except for breed. When we
Table
3
Behavior Patterns of the Five Cluster Groups
ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS
Note.
Dashes indicate that these variables cannot be divided into
statistically different levels.
EXPO
= exploration in the presence of
owner; EXPS = exploration in the presence of stranger; PLYO =
playing with owner present; PLYS = playing with stranger present;
PASO = passive behavior in presence of owner; PASS = passive
behavior in presence of stranger; CONTO = physical contact with
owner; CONTS = physical contact with stranger; SBYS =
standing by door with owner absent; COSO = contact seeking with
entering owner; SBYO = standing by door with owner present;
COSS = contact seeking with entering stranger; DELO = delay of
contact seeking with owner; DELS = delay of contact seeking with
stranger; DCONTO = duration of physical contact while greeting
entering owner; DCONTS = duration of physical contact while
greeting entering stranger. L = low;
M
= medium; and H
= high
value of a given variable, which are significant ranges established
by post hoc Duncan range test. ANOVA = analysis of variance.
Table 4
Factorial Patterns of the Five Cluster Groups
22
5
Note.
L = low; M
= medium; and
H
= high value of a given
variable,
which are significant ranges established by post hoc
Duncan range test. ANOVA = analysis of variance.
compared Subgroups 1 and
2,
neither the mean ages,
2.7
and
4.0,
years
F(1, 33) = 1.18, ns,
nor the average number of
family members,
5.5 and
5.3, F(1, 33) = 0.22, ns,
differed
significantly.
We analyzed the effect of breed differences on the strange
situation
behavior of the dogs using an ANOVA that
compared the homogeneous and the heterogeneous groups.
Only
2
out of
16
variables differed significantly; that is, the
Belgian Shepherds had a lower level of contact seeking
toward the entering stranger, COSS:
F(1, 50) = 7.40, p =
.
01, and they spent more time in close proximity to their
owners, CONTO:
F(1, 50) = 10.40, p < .01.
Regarding the
means of the dogs' individual factor scores, there were no
significant differences between the Belgian Shepherds and
the mixed group.
Nevertheless, we decided that comparing the variance
scores of the observed variables might demonstrate more
clearly the effect of breed differences on the strange situation
behavior. To determine whether Belgian shepherds are a
more homogeneous group than the mixed group, we used the
F
test of homogeneity of variance. With the exception of
DELO,
F(16,16) = 3.78, p < .001,
there were no significant
differences in variances of the behavioral variables and the
factor scores.
F(16, 16)
values are as follows (p >
.
05 in
each):
EXPO
=
1.69,
EXPS =
1.12,
PLYO =
1.05,
PLYS =
1.08,
PASO = 1.42, PASS = 1.42, CONTO = 0.16,
CONTS =
0.50,
SBYO =
0.70,
SBYS =
1.77, COSO =
1.12,
COSS = 1.33,
DELS =
0.44,
DCONTO =
0.65,
DCONTS =
0.47,
Anxiety =
0.99,
Acceptance =
0.81,
and
Attachment =
0.57.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the human-dog
relationship by means of Ainsworth's
(1969) Strange Situa-
tion Test. As the results show, the experimental conditions of
the test proved to be effective in activating the attachment
behavior of owner-dog dyads, despite the fact that our
participants were all physiologically adults, and attachment
behavior is usually regarded as a feature of childhood, as a
part of parent-offspring interactions. The observed attach-
ment behavior of adult dogs toward owners is presumably
the result of 10,000 years of domestication. During this time,
dogs' dependency was increased by artificial selection, and
thus long-lasting, caregiver-receiver relations between hu-
mans and dogs could be formed by way of socialization
Variable
la
Cluster group
lb
2
3a
3b
ANOVA
F(4, 50)
P
EXPO
L L L
M
H
5.30
.
0014
EXPS L L
M M
M
2.80
.
035
PLYO
H
M
L
L
M 50.30
>.0001
PLYS
H
L
L
M
L
13.90
>.0001
PASO
L L
M
M
H
13.70
>.0001
PASS
L L
H H
L
8.90
>.0001
CONTO
L L
H
L L
18.00
>.0001
CONTS
M
L
M
L L
2.90
.
03
SBYS
L
H
M
L
H
40.20
>.0001
COSO
H
H H
L
H
2.40
.
05
SBYO
-----
1.27
ns
COSS
-----
1.91
ns
DELO
-----
1.69
ns
DELS
-----
1.52
ns
DCONTO
-----
1.26
ns
DCONTS
-----
1.87
ns
Cluster group
ANOVA
Factor
la
lb
2
3a
3b
F(4,50) p
Anxiety
L
L H
M
M
25.0
>.0001
Acceptance
M
L H
M
M 5.0
>.01
Attachment
L
M
H L
M 6.4
>.01
22
6
TOPÁL,
MIKLÓSI,
CSÁNYI, AND D
Ó
KA
Figure 4.
Means of individual factor scores of the Anxiety (top panel), Acceptance (middle panel),
and Attachment (bottom panel) factors in the five cluster groups.
during an individual's life. Another possible explanation for
the attachment behavior could be a tendency on the part of
human breeders to select dogs that behave in social situa-
tions similarly to humans, especially children. The result of
such a process is the domesticated dog that simulates many
human (infant) behavior patterns such as attachment.
As was the case with children and human-reared chimpan-
zees (Bard, 1991), the observed behavioral changes in
owner-dog dyads fulfilled the operational criteria of attach-
ment (Gewirtz, 1972; Rajecki et al., 1978). A dog's use of
the owner as a secure base and its specific reaction to
separation from and reunion with the owner are clear
behavioral
manifestations of the attachment of dogs to
human.
This secure-base effect was revealed by the dogs' in-
creased exploration and by more frequent playing in the
presence of the owners (caregivers), just as in children and
chimpanzees. In the separation episodes, dogs stood at the
door for considerable lengths of time; the fact that this
behavior was not reduced by the presence of the stranger
suggests dogs' strong preference for their owners in stress
situations.
This reaction seems to be analogous to the
"searching response" described in young children (Ains-
worth, 1969), monkeys (Kaufmann & Rosenblum, 1969),
and chimpanzees (Bard, 1991) that was interpreted as an
effort to maintain the attachment-comfort bond (Gewirtz,
1961; Rheingold, 1961; Walters & Parke, 1964). The dogs'
specific reaction to reunion with the owner was active and
i
mmediate contact seeking and a tendency toward contact
maintenance for the returning owner (COSO, DELO, and
DCONTO) but not the stranger.
None of the independent variables (age, sex of humans,
and gender of dogs) seems to account for the dogs'
considerable behavioral variability in the Strange Situation
Test.
However, dogs living in large families exhibited less
proximity-seeking behavior toward the owner (SBYS) and
tended to behave more passively (PASS) in the situation.
This effect can be attributed to the differences in socializa-
tion of particular dogs because in larger families, pets
probably form multiple attachments to some members of the
family.
On this basis, these dogs will show less clinging
behavior toward the owner.
Moreover, the breed-specific differences, usually re-
garded as a major source of behavioral variability in dogs
(Scott & Fuller, 1965), also had only a slight effect on the
behavior of the dogs in the test. The comparison of mixed
breed (heterogeneous) versus one purebred (homogeneous)
group (Belgian Shepherds) showed only small differences.
Because cluster analysis did not rank the individuals of
different breeds into groups of distinct categories, perhaps
the variability of the attachment behavior among breeds is
similar.
However, with regard to the behavioral parameters
used in this study, one breed might be found to show a
different kind of attachment compared with other breeds.
This possibility underlines the necessity of the use of a
multivariate approach because it reduces the effect of
existing breed differences on the evaluation of attachment.
As was found in other studies of applied factor analysis
(J.
P Connel & Goldsmith, 1982; Miller et al., 1990), the
strange situation behavior was influenced by different vari-
ables, including the dogs' reaction to a separation from the
owner, the unfamiliar environment that was more or less
stressful for dogs, and the dogs' responsiveness to the
stranger. The results of our factor analysis support the notion
that the strange situation behavior could be explained by
three
major hypothetical variables, including the dogs'
reaction to separation from the owner (Factor 3: Attach-
ment), the unfamiliar environment (Factor 1: Anxiety) that
was more or less stressful for dogs, and the dogs' responsive-
ness to the stranger (Factor 2: Acceptance). In contrast with
earlier attempts to validate traditional A, B, or C categoriza-
tion (avoidant, secure, or resistant) by post hoc cluster
analysis (D. B. Connel, 1976; Gardner & Thompson, 1983),
we used this multivariate analysis to establish categories of
dog-human relationships. The results of the exploratory
cluster analysis showed that dogs could be separated into at
least three or at most five major groups (see Figure 3).
ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS
227
Although these groups differed with respect to many behav-
ioral variables, the difference in the factorial variables was
more pronounced. Starting from the principle that factorial
variables can be divided into three statistically distinct
categories (L, M, or H) we found five distinguishable groups
that differed by at least one factorial variable from each
other. Still, our findings support the view of J. P Connell and
Goldsmith (1982) that group classification is best perceived
as representing an underlying continuum in three dimensions.
Dogs that belonged to Cluster Group 1 were characterized
by low anxiety in the stressful situation, but they differed in
the interrelationship between their acceptance and attach-
ment. The low levels of attachment to the owners in Group
1 a contrast with their medium level of acceptance of the
stranger. Dogs in Group lb showed higher levels of attach-
ment to the owner than acceptance of the stranger, suggest-
ing that the former was more preferred for reducing stress in
these dogs. Although one might suspect there was a differ-
ence in socialization of the dogs in the two subgroups, at the
present time we have no evidence for this.
Cluster Group 2 was characterized by high anxiety related
to a stressful situation, high levels of acceptance regarding
the separation, and high levels of contact seeking and
contact maintenance toward the entering owner. These dogs
seemed not to distinguish between owner and stranger,
which could have been the result of their marked stress
during separation episodes. In sum, these dogs expressed the
most extreme behavior of all subjects observed.
Cluster Group 3 was characterized by medium levels of
anxiety and acceptance. There was a clear separation in two
subgroups (3a and 3b) of the level of attachment to the
owner.
Whereas dogs in Group 3a showed low levels of
attachment toward the owner, Group 3b was characterized
by significantly higher attachment behavior in this situation.
One might suppose well-socialized dogs should belong into
this latter group (3b). We suspect that the dogs of Group 3a
tended to avoid close -contact with the owner and the
stranger, which was a more effective strategy for them in
reducing stress during separation episodes.
Regarding the total sample, all of the five cluster groups
and subgroups are represented by a considerable proportion
of subjects (10%-35%). In other words, all these groups
represent a relatively common form of attachment behavior,
but the level of expression was influenced by the stressful-
ness of the strange situation.
Dogs in Group lb seemed to attach only to the owner.
Dogs of Groups 2 and 3b both seemed to bond to humans,
not just to a particular person (medium and high scores of
Acceptance and Attachment). However, they differed in
sensitivity to stress, and thus their reactions toward the
owner also differed: The extremely anxious dogs (Group 2)
showed more expressed attachment behavior and acceptance
than the less anxious ones (Group 3b).
Dogs in two groups (la and 3a) avoided the owner (low
Attachment scores). The difference between them was the
level of anxiety. Compared with Group 2, the relative
owner-avoidant behavior of nonanxious Group l a does not
unconditionally mean a lack of attachment on the part of
these dogs because we would not expect the activation of
22
8
TOPÁL, MIKLÓSI, CSÁNYI, AND
DÓKA
attachment behavior under a condition
of low
stress.The
dogsof
Group 3a that were more susceptible to stress, but in
spite
of
this fact behaved in a very avoidant manner, can be
referred to as the nonattached group
of
our sample. The
moderately anxious dogs
of
Group 3a seemed to bond to
humans in general but not to the owner.
Because all
of
our subjects were more or less socialized
dogs living in families, we did not find dogs that reject
humans in general (low Attachment,
low
Acceptance).
We
suppose that extremely unsocialized dogs (feral dogs or
some dogs from shelters) would prove to be more avoidant
toward humans.
The establishment
of
these categories gives us an impor-
tant tool for examining the effect
of
socialization
of
dogs and
the genetic influences (breed differences) on the attachment
of
dogs to owners. Furthermore, we are now able to measure
the temporal stability
of
owner-dog
relationship and its
development. All these issues should be the targets for future
studies.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency and
attachment: A theoretical review of the infant-mother relation-
ship.
Child Development, 40,
969-1025.
Ainsworth,
M. D. S. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A
comparison. In J. L. Gewirtz (Ed.),
Attachment and dependency
(pp. 97-137). Washington DC: Winston.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978).
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange
situation.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and
exploratory behavior of one-year olds in a strange situation. In
B.
M.
Foss
(Ed.),
Determinants of infant behavior (Vol.
4, pp.
111-136). London: Methuen.
Arend, R., Gove, F. L., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Continuity of
individual adaptation from infancy to kindergarten: A predictive
study of ego-resiliency and curiosity in preschoolers.
Child
Development, 50,
950-959.
Bard,
K. A. (1983). The effect of peer separation in young
chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatol-
ogy, 5,
25-37.
Bard, K. A. (1991). Distribution of attachment classifications in
nursery chimpanzees.
American Journal of Primatology, 24,
88.
Barker, S. B., & Barker, R. T. (1988). The
human-canine
bond:
Closer than family ties?
Journal of Mental Health Counseling,
10,46-56.
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother.
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39,
350-373.
Bowlby, J. (1969).
Attachment and loss: Vol. I. Attachment.
New
York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1972).
Attachment.
Middlesex, England: Penguin
Books.
Cairns, R. B. (1966). Attachment behavior of mammals.
Psychologi-
cal Review, 73,
409-426.
Cairns, R. B., & Werboff, J. (1967, November 24). Behavior
development in the dog: An interspecific analysis.
Science, 158,
1070-1072.
Cohen, L. J. (1974). The operational definition of human attach-
ment.
Psychological Bulletin, 81,
207-217.
Collis, G. M. (1995, September).
Health benefits of pet ownership:
Attachment vs. psychological support.
Paper presented at
Ani-
mals, health and quality of
life:
Seventh International Confer-
ence on Human Animal Interactions,
Geneva, Switzerland.
Connel, D. B. (1976).
Individual differences in infant attachment:
An investigation into stability, implications and relationships to
structure of early language development.
Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY.
Connel, J. P., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1982). A structural modeling
approach to the study of attachment and strange situation
behaviors. In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.),
The develop-
ment of attachment and
affiliative
systems (pp.
67-83). New
York: Plenum Press.
Coppinger, R. J., Glendinning, E., Torop, C., Matthay, C., Suther-
land,
M., & Smith, C. (1987). Degree of behavioral neoteny
differentiates canid polymorphs.
Ethology, 75,
89-108.
Cox, R. P. (1993). The human/animal bond as a correlate of family
functioning.Clinical Nursing Research, 2,
224-231.
Cynic, L. S., Reite, M. L., & Shucard, D. W. (1982). Animal models
of human behavior: Their application to the study of attachment.
In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.),
The development of
attachment and affiliative systems (pp.
31-42).
New York:
Plenum Press.
Elliot,
O.,
& Scott, J. P (1961). The development of emotional
distress reactions to separation in puppies.
Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 99,
3-22.
Fisher, A. E. (1955).
The effects of differential early treatment on
the social and exploratory behavior of puppies.
Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, University
Park Campus.
Frederickson,
E. (1952). Perceptual homeostasis and distress
vocalization in the puppy.
Journal of Personality, 20,
472-477.
Freud, A. (1946). The psychoanalytic study of infantile feeding
disturbances.
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2,
119-132.
Gardner, W. P., & Thompson, L. A. (1983, June).
A cluster analytic
evaluation of the strange situation classification system.
Paper
presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Detroit, MI.
Gewirtz, J. L. (1961).
Learning
analysis of the effects of normal
stimulation, privation and deprivation on the acquisition of
social motivation and attachment. In B. M.
Foss
(Ed.),Determi-
nants of infant behavior (pp.
213-299). London: Methuen.
Gewirtz, J. L. (1972). Attachment, dependence, and a distinction in
terms of stimulus control. In J. L. Gewirtz (Ed.),
Attachment and
dependency (pp.
139-177). Washington, DC: Winston.
Gubemick, D. J. (1981). Parent and infant attachment in mammals.
In D. J.
Gubemick
& P H. Klopfer (Eds.),
Parental care in
mammals (pp.
243-300). London: Plenum Press.
Hoffman, H. S., & Ratner, A. M. (1973). A reinforcement model of
i
mprinting: Implications for socialization in monkeys and men.
Psychological Review, 80,
527-544.
Kaufmann, I. C., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1969). Effects of separation
from mother on the emotional behavior of infant monkeys.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 159,
681-695.
Kraemer, G. W (1992). A psychobiological theory of attachment.
Behavioral Brain and Sciences, 15,
493-541.
Kretchmer, K. R., & Fox, M. W. (1975). Effects of domestication
on animal behavior.
The Veterinary Record, 96,
102-108.
Lorenz, K. (1966).
On aggression.
New York: Harcourt, Brace &
World,
Inc.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants
as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situa-
tion. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, and E. M. Cummings
(Eds.),
Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and
intervention (pp.
121-160). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Martin, P, & Bateson, P. (1986).
Measuring behavior. Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of
adaptation in second year: The relationship between quality of
attachment and later competence.
Child Development, 49,
547-
556.
Miller, L. C., Bard, K. A., Juno, C. J., & Nadler, R. D. (1986).
Behavioral responsiveness of young chimpanzees
(Pan trog-
lodytes)
to a novel environment.
Folia Primatologica, 47,
128-142.
Miller, L. C., Bard, K. A., Juno, C. J., & Nadler, R. D. (1990).
Behavioral responsiveness to strangers in young chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes). Folia Primatologica, 55,
142-155.
Millot, J. L. (1994). Olfactory and visual cues in the interaction
systems between dogs and children.
Behavioural Process, 33,
177-188.
New, J. C., (1995, September). Quality of life of companion
animals. In
Animals, health and quality of life: Seventh Interna-
tional Conference on Human Animal Interactions (pp.
24-25),
Geneva, Switzerland.
Pettijohn, T. F.,
Wont, T. W., Ebert, P D., & Scott, J. P. (1977).
Alleviation of separation distress in 3 breeds of young dogs.
Developmental Psychobiology, 10,
373-381.
Rajecki, D. W., Lamb, M. E., & Obmascher, P. (1978). Toward a
general theory of infantile attachment: A comparative review of
aspects of the social bond.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3,
417-464.
Rheingold, H. L. (1961). The effect of environmental stimulation
upon social and exploratory behavior in the human infant. In
1.
In leaving during reunion episodes, the stranger must be
unobtrusive and never interfere with the reunion (i.e., say nothing
to the owner or dog, do not move between them, and leave quietly).
If necessary, the stranger can wait to exit.
2. The stranger should never position herself between the dog
and the owner, especially during reunions.
3. The stranger should never sit in the owner's chair.
4.
When playing, the stranger should take her cue from the dog
and do something similar.
5. In Episodes 3 and 6, if the dog is upset, the stranger should try
to reassure it by petting and then distract it with toys.
6.
At the end of Episodes 3 and 6, the stranger should never be
ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS
Appendix
Instructions to the Stranger
22
9
B.
M.
Foss
(Ed.),
Determinants of infant behaviour (pp.
143-171). London: Methuen.
Ross, S., Scott, J. P, Chemer, M., & Denenberg, V H. (1960).
Effects of restraint and isolation on yelping in puppies.
Animal
Behavior, 8,
1-5.
Scott, J. P., & Fuller, J. L. (1965).
Genetics and the social behavior
of the dog.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sears, R. R., Whiting, J. W. M., Novlis, V., & Sears, P S. (1953).
Some child rearing antecedents of aggression and dependency in
young children.
Genetic Psychology Monographs, 47, 135-247.
Serpell, J. (1995).
The domestic dog.
Cambridge, England: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1973). An opponent-process
theory of motivation II: Cigarette addiction. Journal of Abnor-
mal Psychology, 81,
158-171.
Voith, V L., Wright, J. C., & Danneman, P J. (1992). Is there a
relationship between canine behavior problems and spoiling
activities, anthropomorphism, and obedience training?
Applied
Animal Behavior Science, 34,
263-272.
Walters, R. H., & Parke, R. D. (1964). Social motivation, depen-
dency, and susceptibility to social influence. In L. Berkowitz
(Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp.
232-276). New York: Academic Press.
Waters, E.,
Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment,
positive affect, and competence in the peer group: Two studies in
construct validation.
Child Development, 50,
821-829.
Wickler,
W. (1976). The ethological analysis of attachment:
Sociometric,
motivational and sociophysiological aspects.
Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 42, 12-28.
playing or interacting with the dog so that the dog is not distracted
when the owner returns.
7. In Episode 2, if the owner is not in her or his chair, the stranger
may remind the owner to move to the chair.
8. The stranger must learn to remain calm in the presence of very
distressed dogs and must not feel distressed if she cannot calm the
dog.
Received February 10, 1997
Revision received December 12, 1997
Accepted December 18, 1997 v
... Research from the past several decades indicates that the close relationship that exists between dogs and their owners has the characteristics of an attachment bond (Topál et al., 1998). This is demonstrated through differences in behavior exhibited toward their primary caretaker when compared with unfamiliar people: seeking proximity and contact with the owner and the appearance of stress responses when separated (e.g., Prato-Previde et al., 2003;Topál et al., 2005). ...
... Most of them are framed within the measurement of the attachment bond or the study of the attachment styles of dogs, which are parallel to those observed in children (see Nagasawa et al., 2009;Payne et al., 2015;Udell et al., 2021). This is because two of the most popular tests of attachment adapted for dogs, namely the Ainsworth Strange Situation test (Topál et al., 1998) and the Secure Base Test (e.g., Udell & Brubaker, 2016), alternate episodes of presence and absence of the owner. Additionally, other studies have assessed the reactions of dogs to brief separation from their owners using different procedures (Kinnaird & Wells, 2022;Mariti et al., 2018;Pierantoni et al., 2022;Pongrácz et al., 2020). ...
... It should be noted that a common characteristic of the majority of these studies is that the dogs were tested in novel environments (i.e., in a laboratory). This methodological approach is commonly used in attachment studies as leaving the dog alone briefly in an unfamiliar environment is often used as part of the Strange Situation intended to elicit temporary separation distress (Topál et al., 1998). While it is often assumed that a novel environment is necessary to establish separation distress during the alone phase of attachment tests (leading up to the reunion phase where attachment styles and the secure base effect are evaluated), many dogs also exhibit stress when left alone in their homes. ...
... Dogs became a popular model species for investigation of the evolution of social cognition due to their socio-cognitive capacities [4], which are considered to be not only human-compatible [5] but often analogous to human cognitive phenotypes [6,7], for example (interspecific) social learning [8], empathy [9], and rule following [10]. These socio-cognitive features, along with the dependency-based, asymmetrical dog-human relationship [11], strongly contributed to the formation of a species that today is considered to be one of the few 'true companion animals' [12]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background While complex dog–human coexistence has been deeply investigated, there is a relative scarcity of similar knowledge regarding dog–dog interactions. Social learning, a fundamental synchronizing mechanism between dogs and humans, was recently found to be influenced by the functional breed selection of dogs: with the cooperative breeds being more effective learners from a human demonstrator than the independent working breeds were. Here, we investigated whether these differences would also be present when dogs had to learn from another dog and how to effectively perform a detour around a transparent V-shaped obstacle. We tested dogs from 28 independent and 19 cooperative breeds in three consecutive trials. In the control groups, all dogs had to detour on their own the obstacle. In the dog demonstration groups, in trial 1, the subjects had to detour on their own, but before the next two trials, a trained dog showed them the solution. Results We found that the performance of the two breed groups was the same in the without demonstration groups. However, after observing the dog demonstrator, the independent dogs learned the task more successfully than the cooperative breeds did. In the case of the independent working breeds, detour latencies significantly dropped along the consecutive trials, and these dogs also showed higher rate of successful detours after observing the demonstrator dog’s action than in the control group. Conclusions This is the first study where the consequences of functional breed selection were confirmed in a scenario that involved conspecific social learning in dogs. The results fit well to the ecologically valid framework of the evolutionary past of dog breed formation, in which cooperative breeds were selected for their interactivity with humans, whereas independent breeds often had to work together with their conspecifics. Graphical Abstract
... Qualitative research may be helpful to better understand how owners feel about their relationship with their pets and how it relates to their mental health, and how this compares to their human relationships. It would also be useful to explore how the dog, and in particular the dog's attachment to their owner [55], may be impacting this relationship and contributing to the owner's mental health. Our dataset is also available for any researchers interested in exploring the relationship between variables in the study further (available online: https://osf.io/69zjp/?view_only=9e3bdcf3fbed4e9dbce8ca3e370d06f7, accessed on 27 May 2024). ...
Article
Full-text available
It is unclear how pet ownership is related to mental health, with some previous research suggesting pet owners have better mental health, while other research suggests they have worse mental health. Some researchers have suggested that it may be more useful to investigate the bond people feel with their pets and how this may impact mental health; however, this too has led to mixed results. This study examined how owners’ attachment to their dogs was associated with mental health and how this compared to their attachment relationships with other humans in a sample of 607 dog owners. Our findings indicate that both strong and insecure attachments to dogs are linked to poorer mental health outcomes, as was having an insecure attachment style in their human relationships. The adverse impact of strong attachment to dogs on mental health was mediated by these owners having an anxious attachment style toward other people, which in turn was associated with poorer mental health. The relationship between a strong attachment to dogs and poorer mental health was also moderated by gender, with this relationship being significant in women but not significant for men. Together, these results suggest a possible risk to mental health for owners who form a strong attachment to their dogs to compensate for anxious attachments in human relationships.
Article
Full-text available
It is still largely unknown to what extent domestication, ancestry, or recent functional selection are responsible for the behavioral differences in whether dogs look back to a human when presented with a difficult task. Here, we tested whether this ubiquitous human-related response of companion dogs would appear differently in subjects that were selected for either cooperative or independent work tasks. We tested N = 71 dogs from 18 cooperative and 18 independent breeds. Subjects learned in a five-trial warming-up phase that they could easily obtain the reward from a container. In trial six, the reward became impossible to take out from the locked container. When the task was easy, both breed groups behaved similarly, and their readiness to approach the container did not differ between the last ‘solvable’ and the subsequent ‘unsolvable’ trial. Task focus, looking at the container, touching the container for the first time, or interacting with the container with a paw or nose did not differ between the breed groups, indicating that their persistence in problem solving was similar. However, in the ‘unsolvable’ trial, cooperative dogs alternated their gaze more often between the container and the humans than the independent dogs did. The frequency of looking back was also higher in cooperative dogs than in the independent breeds. These are the first empirical results that suggest, in a balanced, representative sample of breeds, that the selection for different levels of cooperativity in working dogs could also affect their human-dependent behavior in a generic problem-solving situation.
Chapter
Full-text available
Bu bölümünde, evcil hayvanlara bağlanma ve bağımlılık arasındaki ilişki, evcil hayvanların bağımlılık tedavisindeki terapötik etkileri, bağımlılık tedavisinde hayvan destekli terapinin rolü üzerinde durulmaktadır.
Article
Full-text available
Olfactory information plays an important role in the attachment and bonding processes for both humans and non-human animals. Odor cues obtained via individual body odor facilitate attachment and bonding processes across species with regard to both mate selection and mother-infant bonding. The purpose of the present paper is to summarize the role of odor as it pertains to bond formation and maintenance in the mother-infant bond for human infants and non-human animal infants, and for mate selection among human adults and non-human animals. We then synthesize this summary with literature on attachment and existing evidence for the relationships between olfaction and attachment processes. Finally, we suggest avenues for areas of future research.
Chapter
In this chapter, we describe psychiatric disorders related to a dysregulation of the attachment axis. We distinguish various autonomy-related disorders: autonomy disorder due to lack of detachment, autonomy disorder due to rupture of attachment, and secondary autonomy disorder. Finally, we describe a dysfunctional attachment syndrome, which encompasses disorders in which attachment either does not occur or is of poor quality. For each disorder, we provide detailed information on clinical picture, diagnosis, treatment, prognosis, and follow-up, and clinical cases are included to enhance the text.
Chapter
This chapter explores neurophysiological development and ontogenetic behaviors. The periods of behavioral development and aging are discussed. The ontogeny of attachment, fear reactions, acquisition of self-control, as well as inter- and intraspecific social integration are the subjects of particular attention. The four axes—attachment, fear, self-control, and social integration—constitute the four pillars of development, and, in the course of our work, diseases as well as all behavioral anomalies will be categorized according to these four directions
Chapter
Parental care is one strategy which helps ensure the survival of offspring and thereby enhances the parents’ reproductive success (see Klopfer, this volume; Pianka, 1970). In mammals, parental care usually involves behavioral interactions between parents and offspring. One form these interactions can take is that of parent and infant attachment, the subject of this chapter.
Chapter
The purpose of this chapter is to present a new methodological strategy for examining the organization of behaviors thought to reflect the attachment system. The structural modeling approach offers great promise for precise specification and evaluation of hypotheses concerning the organization and development of the parent—infant attachment bond. This approach is presented as an alternative to traditional methods of analyzing attachment behaviors, as represented by the ABC classification system (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978).
Chapter
Adult nurturance is a critical factor in establishing dependency in children and in facilitating the socialization process. This chapter presents the concept of a dependency or social drive, and focuses on various classes of responses that have been linked, in the child-training and social-psychological literature, with the concept of a dependency drive. The relationships between variables, such as social deprivation, dependency, self-esteem, and various measures of social influence can be largely understood in terms of eliciting and modification of orienting and attending responses, and the behavioral effects of variations in emotional arousal. To understand fully the nature of dependency behavior as a socially significant variable requires, an understanding of the acquisition of social judgments involving the labeling of behavior as dependent, and the conditions under which these judgments are evoked. The concept of dependency motive is not characteristics of human agents, but constructs, by means of which human beings order social phenomena, and evaluate behavior in terms of its acceptability or nonacceptability within a given cultural context. The motivational interpretations of behavior involve complicated evaluations having reference to the complex stimulus events as well as to the consequences that an agent's behavior produces for others. Therefore, it is not surprising that evaluative judgments in terms of the intent or motive of the agent, rather than the consequences of the act, become relatively more frequent as a child grows older, and increasingly conforms to the standards to which the child is exposed.
Article
This article describes a neurobiological basis for the "first attachment" of the primate infant to its caretaker. The infant normally internalizes a neurobiological "image" of the behavioral and emotional characteristics of its caregiver that later regulates important features of its brain function. Current models of sensorimotor analysis and its relation to emotion suggest that sensorimotor stems are also habit and memory systems, their functional status and lability regulated in part by biogenic amine systems. The intertwined development of neural and social functions can sometimes go awry. If the attachment process fails or the caregiver is incompetent, the infant may become socially dysfunctional. This helps explain the developmental psychopathology and later vulnerability to adult psychopathology that result from disruptions of social attachment.
Article
This study dealt with the effects of restraint, isolation and companionship on the yelping behaviour of puppies. Thirty-five puppies of various complex hybrid and pure breeds, aged 3 to 6 weeks, were used. The condition of restraint was produced with a small triangular wooden box, open at the top and front, which was covered with wire mesh. All tests were in the home pen, and yelps were recorded with a hand tally counter.In the first experiment the effects of restraint (R) against non-restraint (NR) were tested as modified by the condition of alone (A) and together (T). Twenty-three puppies, aged 4 weeks, were randomly assigned to one of the basic groups of R or NR. They were then given a series of 5 minute tests, ATTA or TAAT, under the basic conditions of R or NR. The yelping behaviour of the restrained (R) group was found to be significantly higher than the non-restrained (NR) group, and the alone (A) condition produced more yelping than the together (T) condition. Both of these differences were significant beyond the 0·01 level. Both isolation and restraint increase yelping. Adding a companion reduces yelping by 50–60.per cent.The second experiment was performed with twelve puppies, aged 3 to 6 weeks, randomly assigned to the extreme conditions of the first experiment, those of R-A and NR-T. The effects of test duration upon yelping behaviour was studied with 10 trials, one per day, of 10 minutes each. Statistical analysis of these results demonstrated that there was a similar and significant decrease (0·01 level) in the mean number of yelps in both the R-A and the NR-T groups. Comparison of means for the first 5 minutes against the second 5 minutes made on trials 1 and 10, showed a decrease in yelping behaviour significant at the 0·05 level. It was concluded that repetition of the experience reduces yelping, probably because of learning and adjustment to the situation.