ArticlePublisher preview available
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Two analyses were conducted to examine gender differences in global self-esteem. In analysis I, a computerized literature search yielded 216 effect sizes, representing the testing of 97,121 respondents. The overall effect size was 0.21, a small difference favoring males. A significant quadratic effect of age indicated that the largest effect emerged in late adolescence (d = 0.33). In Analysis II, gender differences were examined using 3 large, nationally representative data sets from the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). All of the NCES effect sizes, which collectively summarize the responses of approximately 48,000 young Americans, indicated higher male self-esteem (ds ranged from 0.04 to 0.24). Taken together, the 2 analyses provide evidence that males score higher on standard measures of global self-esteem than females, but the difference is small. Potential reasons for the small yet consistent effect size are discussed.
Psychological
Bulletin
1999, Vol. 125,
No. 4,
470-500
Copyright 1999
by the
American Psychological Association, Inc.
0033-2909/99/53.00
Gender
Differences
in
Self-Esteem:
A
Meta-Analysis
Kristen
C.
Kling,
Janet
Shibley
Hyde,
Carolin
J.
Showers,
and
Brenda
N.
Buswell
University
of
Wisconsin—Madison
Two
analyses were conducted
to
examine gender differences
in
global self-esteem.
In
Analysis
I, a
computerized literature search yielded
216
effect
sizes, representing
the
testing
of
97,121 respondents.
The
overall
effect
size
was
0.21,
a
small difference
favoring
males.
A
significant quadratic
effect
of age
indicated that
the
largest
effect
emerged
in
late
adolescence
(d
=
0.33).
In
Analysis
II,
gender differences
were
examined using
3
large, nationally representative data sets
from
the
National Center
for
Education
Statistics (NCES).
All of the
NCES
effect
sizes, which collectively summarize
the
responses
of
approximately
48,000
young Americans, indicated higher male self-esteem
(ds
ranged
from
0.04
to
0.24).
Taken together,
the 2
analyses provide evidence that males score higher
on
standard measures
of
global self-esteem than females,
but the
difference
is
small.
Potential
reasons
for the
small
yet
consistent
effect
size
are
discussed.
Despite
the
advances
of
feminism, escalating levels
of
sexism
and
violence—from
undervalued intelligence
to
sexual harassment
in el-
ementary
school—cause
girls
to
stifle
their creative spirit
and
natural
impulses,
which ultimately, destroys their self-esteem
(Pipher,
1994,
bodkjacket).
The
above quotation
from
Reviving
Ophelia,
a
book that spent
over
135
weeks
on the New
York
Times paperback bestseller list,
is
characteristic
of the
coverage
of
gender
and
self-esteem
in the
popular press. Other examples abound. Magazines
offer
headlines
such
as
"Teenage
turning point: Does adolescence herald
the
twilight
of
girls'
self-esteem?" (Bower, 1991)
and
"The great teen
Kristen
C.
Kling, Janet Shibley Hyde, Carolin
J.
Showers,
and
Brenda
N.
Buswell, Psychology Department, University
of
Wisconsin—Madison.
Carolin
J.
Showers
is now at the
Psychology Department, Purdue
University.
Portions
of
this research were presented
at the
1996 meeting
of the
American
Psychological Association
in
Toronto
and at the
1997
meeting
of
the
Association
for
Women
in
Psychology
in
Pittsburgh.
We
gratefully acknowledge
the
following undergraduates
for
their help
with
the
enormous task
of
assembling
the
articles
for the
database:
Tara
Dall,
Anne
Garvey,
Dana
Glassburn,
Helena
Rushansky,
Melissa
Kuczyn-
ski,
Nikki
Levine,
Ngu-Mui
Lu, and
Nikki
Onorato.
We
also
thank Larry
Hedges
and
Paul
LePore
for
their assistance
with
the
national data sets;
Craig Rypstat,
the
Psychology Department computer programmer,
for his
help
constructing
the
electronic database; Charles Kling
for his
mathemat-
ical insights;
and Bob
Krueger
for his
comments
on
earlier
drafts
of
this
article. Kristen
C.
Kling would like
to
thank
the
Department
of
Education
for
providing
her
with
the
opportunity
to
participate
in a
week-long training
seminar
on the
National Education Longitudinal Study
of
1988. Finally,
we
wish
to
acknowledge
the
authors
who
sent
us
additional data regarding
their samples
in
response
to
letters
from
us.
Although
we
cannot thank each
of
you
individually
as we
would like,
we
sincerely appreciate your
efforts
and
are
grateful
to you for
making this
a
better
meta-analysis.
Correspondence concerning this article should
be
addressed
to
Kristen
C.
Kling,
who is now at the
Psychology Department, University
of
Min-
nesota—Twin
Cities,
75
East River Rd., Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455,
or
to
Janet Shibley Hyde, Psychology Department, University
of
Wisconsin
Madison,
1202
W.
Johnson St., Madison, Wisconsin
53706.
Electronic
mail
may be
sent
to
klin0186@tc.umn.edu
or
tojshyde@facstaff.wisc.edu.
girls
self-esteem robbery"
(Larsen,
1992).
The New
York
Times,
in
covering
the
results
of a
study conducted
by the
American Asso-
ciation
of
University Women
(AAUW),
reported that
"Little
girls
lose their self-esteem
on way to
adolescence, study finds" (Daley,
1991).
Books
such
as
School girls:
Young
women,
self-esteem,
and
the
confidence
gap
(Orenstein,
1994)
and
Women
and
self-esteem
(Sanford
&
Donovan, 1985), detailing women's
and
girls'
prob-
lems with self-esteem,
are
published
in the
popular press. Under-
lying
these
claims
is an
assumption that females have substantially
lower self-esteem than males. Does
the
large body
of
scientific
data
on
this question support such
an
assertion?
Although
many researchers have investigated gender
differ-
ences
in
self-esteem, there
is a
pressing need
to
synthesize their
results. Indeed,
a
close inspection
of
individual
studies reveals
contradictory
findings.
For
example,
many
researchers have
found
that
males score higher than females
on
standard measures
of
self-esteem
(Allgood-Merten
&
Stockard,
1991; Feather, 1991;
Fertman
&
Chubb, 1992), whereas others have
found
no
difference
(Greene
&
Wheatley,
1992; Simpson, Gangestad,
&
Lerma,
1990),
and
still others have
found
that females report higher levels
of
self-esteem (Connell, Spencer,
&
Aber, 1994;
Ma &
Leung, 1991).
Meta-analysis,
a
collection
of
statistical
techniques
designed
to
summarize
quantitatively
a
body
of
literature,
is an
appropriate
technique
to
answer
the
question,
"Do
males
and
females
differ
in
their
level
of
self-esteem?"
Two
major
reviews
of
gender
differences
in
self-esteem con-
ducted
prior
to the
advent
of
meta-analysis (Maccoby
&
Jacklin,
1974;
Wylie,
1979) suggested that there
was no
consistent gender
difference,
yet
they
differed
in
their rationale. Whereas Maccoby
and
Jacklin
(1974)
concluded that males
and
females have equiv-
alent levels
of
self-esteem, Wylie (1979) determined that
the
studies
she
reviewed were
too
disparate
to
come
to a
conclusion
about
the
presence
or
absence
of
gender differences. Since
the
publication
of
these reviews, criticisms
of the
traditional,
narrative
review process (for
a
review,
see
Beaman,
1991)
have prompted
researchers
to
develop quantitative methods
for
combining studies.
Recently, researchers have used meta-analysis
to
examine gen-
der
differences
in
self-esteem. Feingold's
(1994)
review
of
gender
470
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... In general, throughout schooling, girls tend to report higher levels of school engagement (Wang et al., 2011), value education more than boys (Roeser & Eccles, 1998), and perform better in scholastic achievement assessed by teachers across subject areas, including math and science (Voyer & Voyer, 2014). A meta-analysis in global self-esteem, a core component of positive identity, mostly based on White, middle-class samples (Kling et al., 1999), found a gender difference favoring boys throughout adolescence, with the gender difference peaking at ages 15-18. However, a 5-year longitudinal study of mainly Black and Latinx adolescents in a public high school in New York City found no overall gender difference, with a steady increase in self-esteem with age in both genders, although boys tended to report higher selfesteem than girls at age 16 (Greene & Way, 2005). ...
... However, in contrast with boys who reported increased positive identity from 15 to 17 years after no substantial decrease from 12 to 15 years, girls reported a decline in positive identity from 12 to 15 years without improvement at age 17. This gender difference in positive identity is partially in line with overall findings on global self-esteem favoring boys, with the largest gender difference observed during late adolescence (Kling et al., 1999). While a variety of factors may contribute to reduced positive identity in girls, the decline in the perception of girls' own physical attractiveness during adolescence (Harter, 1993), exacerbated by social media (Twenge & Martin, 2020), compared to boys' relatively stable and positive perception, may partially explain the difference. ...
... On the other hand, the improvements in positive identity as well as in positive values from ages 15 to 17 reported in boys could also be a reflection of societal expectations and socialization for boys to be more competitive and dominant than girls (Martin, 1996) and to be socially desirable to girls given the increased interest in mating during adolescence (Brown et al., 2020). Although the gender difference was of small magnitude, given the lack of understanding of the possible cumulative impact of the small differences on later adjustment in adulthood (Kling et al., 1999), findings from ours and others collectively corroborate a need for T A B L E 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations between the four indicators of internal assets by sex, with male adolescents above the diagonal (n = 174) and female adolescents below the diagonal (n = 197). gender-informed approaches to foster positive identity in adolescent girls. ...
Article
Full-text available
Utilizing a birth cohort of primarily Black, low‐income urban adolescents ( N = 371), the present study investigated how internal assets might change over time with sex/gender as a moderator. Internal assets (commitment to learning, positive values, social competencies, positive identity) were assessed at ages 12, 15, and 17 via the Developmental Assets Profile. All internal asset domains decreased from 12 to 15 years in both boys and girls, except positive identity in boys. For girls, these decreases, except in social competencies, remained unchanged from 15 to 17 years, whereas boys improved in all domains during the same period except commitment to learning. Across all three assessments, girls reported greater commitment to learning than boys. Boys reported higher positive identity than girls at age 17. Greater parental attachment was associated with higher scores in all domains of internal assets.
... Several explanations have been proposed for this gender difference in self-esteem. 30 For example, men and women do not have the same presumed roles in society, and self-esteem is presented in society as a predominantly masculine characteristic. Furthermore, peer interactions and socialization reinforce gender stereotypes and create gender differences in self-esteem. ...
Article
Sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) is an innate personality trait that affects approximately 30% of the population. It is characterized by more intense, complex, and rapid cognitive processing of stimuli, along with heightened emotional reactivity. This study examines the influence of SPS on self-esteem and affective dependence in adolescents and emerging adults, a critical developmental period marked by significant physical, psychological, emotional, and social changes. Adolescence is a sensitive developmental period, and self-esteem is strongly correlated with SPS during this period, with low self-esteem acting as a trigger for affective dependence. Hence, we aim to explore the relationship between these three variables. Our hypotheses were tested on 100 adolescents and young adults aged 15 – 20 years who completed an online questionnaire assessing SPS, self-esteem, and affective dependence. The results confirmed the influence of gender on sensitivity and self-esteem (p<0.01) and the impact of romantic relationship experience on affective dependence scores (p<0.05). The variables were significantly correlated (p<0.001). Sensitivity and self-esteem influenced affective dependence (p<0.001), and self-esteem played a mediating role between sensitivity and affective dependence. Our findings support the hypothesis that higher SPS is a vulnerability factor in the development of affective dependence. However, this study has some limitations, such as a small sample size, which limits generalizability. Therefore, further studies on a larger cohort can be conducted to validate these findings.
... Mean responses were significantly higher for men than women in items for "good" sense of self (Men, p=6.3, SD= 1.3; Women, p = 6.1, SD= 1.3) and stabhty of self-esteem (Men, p=5.5, SD= 1.5; Women, p=5.2, SD= 1.6). Higher self-esteem among men than women is consistent with findings of previous studes (1,3,4,5,9,19). The lower self-esteem for women is explained, at least in part, by their placing significantly more emphasis on specific conditional worth items: 10 ("For the most part, I wish I were someone else"), 22 ("People close to me know my self-acceptance goes up and down with my weight"), and 23 ("I could like myself more if I had a better body"). It may also be attributed to significantly lower agreement with certain unconditional worth items: 13 ("The value of my life is not affected by the responsibilities I am grven"), 19 ("I like myself even if people are critical of my looks"), 15 ("When things go wrong, I still accept myself"), and 21 ("I am grateful for my body"). ...
Article
Full-text available
Self-esteem scales tend to represent conditional perceptions of self-esteem. The Worth Index has been previously validated as an effective measure for identifying both conditional and unconditional perceptions of worth. We administered the Worth Index to 1,161 college-aged students at Brigham Young University to assess their current perceptions of unconditional and conditional worth. Both men and women agreed more strongly with items involving unconditional worth than conditional worth. A negative correlation was found between ratings of unconditional and conditional worth. Women had lower unconditional worth scores than men, which was related to being more likely to wish they were someone else, desire to have a better body, and to being concerned about weight. Agreement with two overview measures of self-esteem involving a “good” sense of self and stability of self-esteem was positively associated with agreement on statements about unconditional worth. This research shows the importance of incorporating unconditional worth into self-esteem measures.
Article
Background/Objectives: Intelligence and personality are the most researched constructs in the scientific literature in the field of psychology. Also, the relationship between them has been studied, with controversial results. The present study examines the relationship between intelligence and personality, considering, on the one hand, the influence of gender and, on the other, the predictive capacity of intelligence for personality traits. Methods: The sample consisted of 1166 participants between 11 and 16 years old from the Canary Islands. The Big Five Personality Questionnaire for Children, Adolescents and Adults (MASK-5) and the Adaptive Test of General Intelligence (Matrices-TAI) were used. For the analysis, a quantitative approach with an ex post facto, cross-sectional design was used. The analysis was carried out with SPSS v.26 and Jamovi v.2.3.21. Results: The findings revealed non-significant gender differences in intelligence and personality. The regression model between the two constructs was only significant for the dimension consciousness (F(1,1164) = 3.906, p = 0.048; ɳp2 = 0.003) and its facet perseverance (F(1,1164) = 5.953, p = 0.015; ɳp2 = 0.005), but the effect size was small in both cases. When considering girls and boys separately, the model was not significant for boys, whereas for girls, the dimension consciousness (F(1,595) = 6.148, p = 0.013; ɳp2 = 0.010) and its facet achievement (F(1,595) = 8.227, p = 0.004; ɳp2 = 0.014), as well as the facet humility (agreeableness) (F(1,595) = 6.472, p = 0.011; ɳp2 = 0.011), were significant, but again, with small effect sizes. Nevertheless, low observed power results for the whole sample and the boys sample suggest the need to increase the sample size. Conclusions: These findings are discussed, and future lines of research in this field of study are proposed.
Article
Full-text available
Background Loneliness is a growing public health problem worldwide that can affect mental and emotional health and increase mortality. This study examined the relationship between body mass index (BMI), body image flexibility, body appreciation, and loneliness among Chinese workers, aiming to propose effective measures for reducing loneliness and promoting mental health. Methods A total of 464 workers were recruited for this study. Participants completed the Chinese version of the Body Image Acceptance and Action Questionnaire, the BMI measure, a Standard Chinese translation of the Body Appreciation Scale, and the Chinese version of the short-form UCLA Loneliness Scale (ULS-8). Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS. Results There were gender differences in body appreciation and body image flexibility scores were observed. Loneliness levels were higher among female workers than male workers. Male workers scored higher than female workers in body appreciation. Female workers scored significantly higher than male workers in body image flexibility and loneliness. Based on multiple regression analyses of the factors affecting loneliness scores, body image flexibility score (β = -0.19, p < 0.01) and body appreciation score (β = -0.25, p < 0.01) were significant predictors of loneliness. Conclusion As middle-aged individuals exhibit higher levels of loneliness. Policymakers should prioritize workplace mental health programs that combine body positivity workshops, sleep hygiene education, and peer support networks. For example, incorporating body image flexibility training into existing employee wellness programs to reduce isolation. In addition, public campaigns to combat weight stigma, such as media literacy programs, are urgently needed to address the social factors that contribute to loneliness.
Article
In England, there has been a shift away from ‘aspiration raising’ in widening participation (WP) activities aimed at improving access to higher education (HE) for students from low socio‐economic status groups. This shift is partly based on the notion that students from these groups do not inherently “aim lower” However, understanding the factors that drive aspirations towards HE remains crucial for addressing geographical gaps in HE participation that cannot be explained by prior attainment. This study aimed to inform WP practices using rich questionnaire data from 602 year 10 students across six schools in post‐industrial English towns known for high community need and poor opportunities, contextualised within a Theory of Possible Selves framework. The questionnaire examined students' HE aspirations, education perceptions, and self‐esteem. Findings revealed that only 39.4% of the students in this cohort aspired to university at age 18, with significant gender differences: 46.6% of females versus 29.8% of males ( p < 0.001), thus challenging the notion of pre‐set high aspirations and underlining the importance of locality‐specific and socio‐cultural context. Having role models, particularly close relatives, who have been to university is valuable, underscoring the importance of fostering similar social capital for students without such connections. Commitment to schooling and academic self‐concept were significant predictors of HE aspiration, with two specific elements standing out for WP consideration: understanding the value of education and being confident in solving problems. Findings have important policy implications, which are discussed along with practical recommendations for tailoring WP interventions, considering contextual and socioeconomic factors.
Chapter
It Takes More Than a Candidate remains the only systematic account of the gender gap in political ambition. Based on national surveys of more than 10,000 potential candidates in 2001, 2011, and 2021, the book shows that women, even in the highest tiers of professional accomplishment, are substantially less likely than men to demonstrate ambition to seek elective office. The gender gap in persists across generations and over time, despite society's changing attitudes toward women in politics. Women remain less likely to be recruited to run for office, less likely to think they are qualified to run, and less likely to express a willingness to run for office in the future. In the twenty years since It Takes a Candidate was first published, the book remains timely and eye-opening, highlighting the challenges women face navigating the candidate emergence process and providing insight into the persistent gender gap in political ambition.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose was to adapt a brief tool, the Physical Self Inventory-Version b, for the assessment of physical self in adults. The inventory is a refined version of the previously validated version, which was derived from the Physical Self Inventory (25 items). This French validation of the Physical Self-Perception Profile of Fox and Corbin includes a global self-esteem scale. As in the earlier version the current one has 6 single items to assess dimensions of global self-esteem, physical self-worth, sport competence, physical condition, attractive body, and physical strength. Rating is performed on a visual analog scale. Items in the new inventory were stated in the first person and in more general terms to be acceptable to a wider range of subjects. An item of measurement error was added. Analysis of responses of 185 men and 148 women to the new version supported the hierarchical structure. Significant correlations between the scores and constructs like masculinity, neuroticism, and depression indicated external validity. The new inventory showed acceptable psychometric properties for use in idiographic studies.
Chapter
There is a strong need to understand the mental health in the LGBTQ population in India. This study aimed to understand resilience and self-esteem on the basis of sexual orientation (heterosexual, homosexual, bisexual, and asexual) in young adults. The main goals of this study are to: (a) understand resilience and self-esteem based on sexual orientation; (b) understand the relationship between resilience and self-esteem based on sexual orientation; and (c) to analyze the differences between the resilience and self-esteem of individuals based on sexual orientation. This study indicates that heterosexual individuals report higher resilience and self-esteem than homosexual, bisexual, and asexual individuals. Heterosexual and bisexual individuals report significantly higher resilience as compared to homosexual individuals. Heterosexual individuals have reported significantly higher self-esteem than the homosexual group. Heterosexual and bisexual individuals exhibit strong and positive correlation between resilience and self-esteem. There is also a strong and positive relationship between the resilience and self-esteem of both male and female heterosexual individuals. These findings indicate that homosexual individuals need to be focused on in mental health research so that appropriate interventions and sociocultural support can be provided to increase both their resilience and self-esteem. Additionally, asexual individuals are a majorly under-studied cohort.
Article
Full-text available
Human affects and emotions are reflected in own eyes and metaphorized as impressions of the eyes such as “Eyes are smiling/not smiling.” Especially, when human has strongly interest or highly motivation, the metaphor is “The eyes are twinkling,” and others are fascinated and attracted to such twinkling eyes. Therefore, it is expected that it will lead to the investigation of factors that fascinate humans or the development of social robots and interfaces that attract humans by reproducing the twinkling eyes using an engineering approach. In this study, we proposed a twinkling eyes expression method that focuses on the reflection on the eyeball surface. This method simulates the specular reflection by superimposing a self-shadow on the pupil as a pseudo self-image. Furthermore, we developed a twinkling eyes interface by applying the proposed method to the pupil response system that has been developed. This interface can express the pupil response and the shade of self-shadow depending on the distance between the interface and the talker. The communication experiments were conducted under conditions with or without a self-shadow and with or without pupil dilation. The results demonstrated that the developed interface was effective, especially for male.
Article
Full-text available
In compartmentalized self-organization, positive and negative self-beliefs are separated into distinct categories (i.e., self-aspects), so that each self-aspect contains primarily positive or primarily negative beliefs. In evaluatively integrative organization, self-aspect categories contain a mixture of positive and negative beliefs. Positive-compartmentalized individuals recovered easily from a sad mood when they could reflect on personally important, pure positive self-aspects. When situational factors maintained the activation of pure negative self-aspects, compartmentalization seemed to perpetuate the negative mood. These studies suggest that people with a positive-compartmentalized self (who usually report high self-esteem and positive mood) have a hidden vulnerability to intense negative states. The advantages of an evaluatively integrated self may require having the opportunity to reflect on (and integrate) positive and negative beliefs about the self.
Article
Full-text available
Conventional wisdom has regarded low self-esteem as an important cause of violence, but the opposite view is theoretically viable. An interdisciplinary review of evidence about aggression, crime, and violence contradicted the view that low self-esteem is an important cause. Instead, violence appears to be most commonly a result of threatened egotism—that is, highly favorable views of self that are disputed by some person or circumstance. Inflated, unstable, or tentative beliefs in the self’s superiority may be most prone to encountering threats and hence to causing violence. The mediating process may involve directing anger outward as a way of avoiding a downward revision of the self-concept.
Article
Full-text available
Social behavior is ordinarily treated as being under conscious (if not always thoughtful) control. However, considerable evidence now supports the view that social behavior often operates in an implicit or unconscious fashion. The identifying feature of implicit cognition is that past experience influences judgment in a fashion not introspectively known by the actor. The present conclusion—that attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes have important implicit modes of operation—extends both the construct validity and predictive usefulness of these major theoretical constructs of social psychology. Methodologically, this review calls for increased use of indirect measures—which are imperative in studies of implicit cognition. The theorized ordinariness of implicit stereotyping is consistent with recent findings of discrimination by people who explicitly disavow prejudice. The finding that implicit cognitive effects are often reduced by focusing judges’ attention on their judgment task provides a basis for evaluating applications (such as affirmative action) aimed at reducing such unintended discrimination.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis surveyed 177 usable sources that reported data on gender differences on 21 different measures of sexual attitudes and behaviors. The largest gender difference was in incidence of masturbation: Men had the greater incidence (d = .96). There was also a large gender difference in attitudes toward casual sex: Males had considerably more permissive attitudes (d = .81). There were no gender differences in attitudes toward homosexuality or in sexual satisfaction. Most other gender differences were in the small-to-moderate range. Gender differences narrowed from the 1960s to the 1980s for many variables. Chodorow's neoanalytic theory, sociobiology, social learning theory, social role theory, and script theory are discussed in relation to these findings.
Article
Full-text available
Violence against women has only recently been addressed in national policy and legislation. Responses by most societal institutions to women assaulted by male partners are still based primarily on a lack of knowledge about the prevalence, severity, and outcomes of violence perpetrated by men against female intimates. Although well suited to make a vital contribution, psychologists still rarely involve themselves in proactive interventions with women victims. This article reviews empirical literature on the physical and sexual assault of women by their male intimates and discusses potential physical and psychological outcomes, with a focus on linking what is known about abused women's reactions with the rich literature on survivors' responses to trauma. Implications for research, treatment interventions, and policy are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Three studies examined whether categorical organization of knowledge about the self explains variance in self-esteem and depression beyond that which is accounted for by sheer amount of positive or negative content. Compartmentalization is the tendency to organize positive and negative knowledge about the self into separate, uniformly valenced categories (self-aspects). As long as positive self-aspects are activated, access to negative information should be minimized. Compartmentalization was associated with high self-esteem and low depression scores for individuals whose positive self-aspects were important; when negative self-aspects were important, compartmentalization was correlated with low self-esteem and high depression scores. An analysis of self-aspect labels showed that individuals with compartmentalized organization define negative self-aspects in especially narrow terms. A possible relationship between compartmentalized organization and cognitive complexity is discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Belief certainty and belief importance represent 2 relatively independent forms of investment in the self-concept. Three studies suggested that whereas certainty is associated with epistemic (i.e., rational or informational) factors, importance is more closely associated with emotive (i.e., emotional and motivational) factors. A 4th study explored the implications of certainty and importance for the temporal stability of people's self-views and revealed that whereas belief certainty was associated with the stability of both positive and negative beliefs, belief importance was associated with the stability of positive beliefs only. The implications of belief investment for the verification–enhancement debate and for the structure and measurement of the self-concept are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examines the lack of strong correlations among existing self-report measures of narcissism. A principal-components analysis of 6 MMPI narcissism scales resulted in 2 orthogonal factors, 1 implying Vulnerability–Sensitivity and the other Grandiosity–Exhibitionism. Although unrelated to each other, these 2 factors were associated with such core features of narcissism as conceit, self-indulgence, and disregard of others. Despite this common core, however, Vulnerability–Sensitivity was associated with introversion, defensiveness, anxiety, and vulnerability to life's traumas, whereas Grandiosity–Exhibitionism was related to extraversion, self-assurance, exhibitionism, and aggression. Three alternative interpretations of these results are considered, and an argument for the distinction between covert and overt narcissism is made.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined daily stress processes among 75 married couples across 20 assessments during a 6-month period. The somatic and psychological effects of common everyday hassles were investigated. Overall, there was a significant relationship between daily stress and the occurrence of both concurrent and subsequent health problems such as flu, sore throat, headaches, and backaches. The relationship of daily stress to mood disturbance was more complex. The negative effects of stress on mood were limited to a single day, with the following day characterized by mood scores that were better than usual. Furthermore, striking individual differences were found in the extent to which daily stress was associated with health and mood across time. Participants with unsupportive social relationships and low self-esteem were more likely to experience an increase in psychological and somatic problems both on and following stressful days than were participants high in self-esteem and social support. These data suggest that persons with low psychosocial resources are vulnerable to illness and mood disturbance when their stress levels increase, even if they generally have little stress in their lives.
Article
The Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale (RSE) is a widely used measure of global self‐esteem. Although its psychometric properties have found considerable support, its relationship to a multidimensional scale of self‐concept has yet to be investigated. The sample for this study consisted of 150 adolescents randomly drawn in equal numbers and equated by gender from grades 8 to 12. Along with the RSE, Harter's Self‐Perception Profile for Adolescents was administered to assess the adolescents' self‐concept in nine separate domains. Correlational and cross‐validation multiple regression analyses found that the RSE total score and both its factor scores were strongly related to Global Self‐Worth, supporting Rosenberg's conclusions that his scale is a measure of global self‐esteem and that its two identified factors are essentially measuring one rather than two different constructs. Other findings include a gender difference, with females reporting significantly lower RSE scores, and modest correlational support for a grade level rise found in the literature.