Content uploaded by Karl Grammer
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Karl Grammer
Content may be subject to copyright.
79
Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X
Copyright © 2002 Neuroendocrinology Letters
ORIGINAL RESEARCH REPORT
The Scent of Fear
Kerstin Ackerl, Michaela Atzmueller & Karl Grammer
Ludwig-Boltzmann-Institute for Urban Ethology at the Institute of Anthropology/University
of Vienna.
Correspondence to: Prof. Karl Grammer, Dr. Dipl. Biol.
Institute of Anthropology/University of Vienna
Althanstrasse 14
A-1090 Vienna / Austria.
EMAIL: karl.grammer@univie.ac.at
Submitted: April 2, 2002
Accepted: April 3, 2002
Key words: fear; human pheromones; cortisol; olfactory communication;
alarm substances
Neuroendocrinology Letters 2002; 23:79–84 pii: NEL230202R01 Copyright © Neuroendocrinology Letters 2002
Abstract In this study we tried to fi nd out if fear can be detected from human body
odours.
Female subjects wore under-arm axillary pads while watching a terryfying
fi lm. Saliva cortisol samples were taken before and after the fi lm presentation
as a hormonal measure for the fear response. The fear experience itself was
measured by Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. A “neutral” fi lm,
shown one day after the “fear” fi lm, was used as a control in a repeated mea-
sures design. In part two of the experiment, the axillary pads were presented
to female subjects in a triple forced choice test. Results show that subjects
were able to discriminate between fear and non-fear axillary pads, suggesting
that women are indeed able to detect “the scent of fear”. A direct correlation
between induced fear, changes in cortisol levels and smell ratings could not
be established. Thus cortisol levels are probably not the inducer of the scent
of fear and a hypothetical fear pheromone could have other origins.
People have always used scents to
either mask their body odour or to ex-
press and emphasize their moods or ap-
pearance. The possibility that odours
can also provide relevant biological in-
formation about their “sender” has
only in recent years become the focus of
scientifi c attention. Doty [1] described
the advantages of olfactory communi-
cation as follows: the sense of smell
even works if the other two “major”
senses (visual and acoustic) are func-
tionally restricted (for example if it
is too dark or too loud). Moreover,
odourous substances can easily be
spread over large areas and can last for
a long time. Intensity, distribution, and
quality of scent marks can give infor-
mation about size, reproductional and
nutritional status, etc. of an individ-
ual without immediately drawing un-
wanted attention to the sender (e.g. de-
tection from a predator via the loud
noise, etc. of a sender). Another advan-
tage in communication through odours
lies in the fact that sender and receiver
don’t have to be in close spatial dis-
tance in order to communicate.
80
The question then becomes prominent: if olfactory
communication is useful in other animals, why is it not
used by human as well?
In recent years this assumption has undergone
major revision. Several studies indicate that humans
do indeed seem to use olfactory communication and are
even able to produce and perceive certain pheromones
(for a detailed overview see [2]). Up to now most stud-
ies were looking at human pheromones linked to the
sex hormones, the question of whether humans are or
are not able to communicate other than sex-related in-
formation through odours has yet to be raised.
Fear arises in stressful situations that are subjec-
tively perceived as threatening. The intensity of the
induced negative feeling corresponds to the subjective
perception of threat in a situation. If the negative feel-
ing becomes too intense, a seeking-reaction for stress-
relieving mechanisms is initiated [3].
Fear can be induced by external, objective threats (e.
g. predators), as well as by internal, subjective threats,
called “free fl oating anxieties”[4]. Free fl oating anxi-
eties can be generated by conscious or subconscious
memories of threatening experiences in the past, or by
the mere anticipation of a stressful situation.
The assumption that fear is a learned avoidance re-
action to potentially dangerous situations is gradually
being questioned. Recent studies show that fear may
be a genetically determined function of the nervous
system [4]. This hypothesis receives support from an
evolutionary point of view. The ability to detect and
anticipate dangerous situations seems to be crucial for
survival, and individual learning might not be entirely
quick enough to ensure survival chances. Moreover,
even potentially dangerous stimuli might be rare and
thus impossible to learn – leading an individual into
danger when the stimulus is encountered for the fi rst
time.
Panksepp [4] describes a “major fear circuit” in the
brain located within the lateral and central parts of the
amygdala in the lobus temporalis, the periaquaeductal
grey (PAG) of the diencephalon and mesencephalon,
and as “output-generating” parts the brain stem and
the medulla.
Fear leads to reactions that are both behavioral
and physiological. Behavioral reactions include either a
“freeze”, “fl ight”, or “fi ght” response. Underlying phys-
iological processes include an increase in heart rate,
muscular tension, sweating, etc., and, most important,
a response of the adrenal gland via the pituitary-hy-
pothalamus-adrenal-axis (HHN) which leads to the
release of cortisol. The cortisol level rises as a con-
sequence of the production of corticotropin releasing
hormone and ACTH [5]. Thus in many experiments the
assessment of changes in cortisol levels is used as an
indicator of potentially experienced fear. In a study by
Hubert & de Jong-Meyer [6] male subjects showed an
increase in cortisol levels (measured from saliva) while
they were watching a horror fi lm. Kirschbaum & Hell-
hammer [7] found similar results. In contrast, Hubert,
Möller, & de Jong-Meyer [8] showed that subjects expe-
rienced an increase in cortisol levels in response to a
funny movie. The authors speculate that every kind of
affective arousal and change of mood, positive as well
as negative, could be linked to cortisol secretion.
Besides fear inducing situations, everyday stressors,
the so-called “daily hassles”, can also provoke a raise
in cortisol levels [9; 10]. In fact, it seems that the mere
anticipation of a stressful experience can have this ef-
fect [11].
It thus remains doubtful that cortisol secretions
might function solely as a result of experiencing fear.
Cortisol raises in such situations could also be due to
the stressing effects of fear.
Alarm pheromones were found in fi sh as early as
1941 by Von Frisch [12] and have since then been
found in many species. Today we know that at least in-
sects, annelids, and fi sh use olfactory signals to inform
their conspecifi cs about stress, alarm, and fear. In ants,
for example, alarm substances can cause aggregation,
dispersion, or defense of the colony depending on the
sender’s status [13]. The use of alarm pheromones
has also been demonstrated in bees (Apis mellifera)
[14] and lice (Acyrthosiphon pisum) [15]. On “defec-
tive stimulation” earthworms (Lumbricus terrestris)
give off a substance that makes their conspecifi cs avoid
their area and thus potential predators [16]. Among
vertebrates, fi sh are the best known example for the
use of alarm substances: fathead minnows (Pime-
phales promelas) that have never before been con-
fronted with a pike (Esox lucius) immediately “know”
that it is a dangerous predator because the pike seems
to get marked by an “odour label” by every minnow it
actually catches [17]. Sources of alarm substances in
minnows are for example urine, feces, and mucus.
Mammals also seem to use olfactory alarm signals.
Valenta & Rigby [18] were able to show that rats can
distinguish between the odour of stressed and “re-
laxed” conspecifi cs. Carr, Martorano & Krames [19]
found that male mice prefer the smell of conspecifi cs
that have just won a fi ght over the smell conspecifi cs
that have not won a fi ght, and the smell of conspecif-
ics that haven’t been experimentally shocked, over the
smell of conspecifi cs that have been treated with elec-
tric shocks.
In predator-prey-interactions between Mongolian
jirds (Meriones unguiculatus) and cats it was shown
that the stressed mice mark the “dangerous areas”
with their scent and thus tell their conspecifi cs to avoid
these areas, while cats orientated themselves by the
scent of stressed mice in order to fi nd them [20].
If other mammals are able to warn their conspecif-
ics – or at least send out the information that they
are in a frightening situation – by emitting odourous
substances, the question becomes prominent: Do hu-
mans possess similar mechanisms? If so, how are these
Kerstin Ackerl, Michaela Atzmueller & Karl Grammer
81
Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X Copyright © 2002 Neuroendocrinology Letters
mechanisms related to cortisol release? The current
research aims to address these questions.
Methods
Collecting the odour samples
In the fi rst part of the experiment odour samples
for later assessment were gathered. In order to do so,
rectangular pads (6 x 10 x 1cm in size) of sterile cel-
lulose were treated with a solution of 40g Cetaceum in
800ml diethyl ether. Cetaceum was used because it is
odourless and is an ideal fi xans for absorbing odours.
The pads were then glued to fl eece material with pieces
of tin foil in between and stored in airtight, odourless
plastic bags.
Forty-two female test subjects, aged between 18 and
33 years (mean age: 23,39 S.D.= 3,55) watched a horror
fi lm (“Candyman”, © Tristar Pictures Inc., 1992) for
70 minutes and a “neutral” fi lm (“Lokorama”, © S. R.
Film & Video Filmproduktions Ges.m.b.H., 1990) – on
2 consecutive days, each presented at the same time of
day. The neutral fi lm showed the view from a driver’s
seat of a southern-bound train ride, originating in Vi-
enna. The duration was the same as the horror fi lm.
While watching the fi lms, participants wore the pads
under their armpits to gather the odour samples. Par-
ticipants had been told to avoid perfumes and deodor-
ants on the days of the experiment and to refrain
from smoking and eating odourous dishes. Just be-
fore the experiment participants washed their armpits
with odourless medical soap and put on cotton t-shirts
that had been washed with an odourless detergent and
stored in an airtight plastic bag.
Room temperature was kept constant (at 26° Cel-
sius) throughout the experiment. After the experi-
ment, the axillary pads were deep-frozen at –20 °C for
later assessment.
Before and after the fi lm presentations, partici-
pants fi lled out questionnaires and donated saliva
samples with “salivettes” (Fa. Sarstedt, Rommelsdorf/
Germany). The salivettes then were deep-frozen at
–20° Celsius.
The questionnaire consisted of three parts: in the
fi rst part the women were asked if they had shaved
their armpits, if they were taking oral contraceptives,
their mean cycle length, day of the cycle, and if they
were on any medication. The second part of the ques-
tionnaire consisted of Spielberger’s [3] State-Trait-
Anxiety Inventory (STAI). In the third part, partici-
pants were asked to rate the fi lm they had just been
watching on a 1–10 scale by the adjectives: a) boring,
b) frightening, c) suspenseful, and d) funny. The state
anxiety scores were later used to check if the actual
situation had evoked fear in the subjects; trait anxi-
ety scores were used to determine the subjects’ general
level of anxiety. The STAI scores range from a mini-
mum of 20 points to a maximum of 80 points.
Rating of the odour samples
In the second part of the experiment, the frozen
axillary pads containing the women’s odour samples
from both fi lms were put in odourless plastic bottles,
which were then heated to 37 °C (human body temper-
ature). These bottles were presented to independent
female raters. Three pads per donor were presented
to 62 raters (aged 18–72 years, mean age: 22,02; S.D.
=7,51). The combination of bottles for rating (two hor-
ror samples, one neutral sample and vice versa) was
chosen randomly. In a triple forced choice test the rat-
ers were asked to assess if there was a difference in the
smell of the three bottles. They were told to write down
their fi rst impression of each odour, even if there was
no consciously detectable smell. Then they had to rate
each bottle by a) intensity of smell, b) pleasantness c)
“smells like sex”, d) “smells like aggression” e) “smells
like fear” on 10 cm long analogue scales [21]. Finally
the raters had to fi ll in questionnaires concerning their
smoking habits, age, cycle day, cycle length, diseases of
the respiratory tract, etc.
Results
Fear
Analysis of State-Anxiety Scales (STAI) scores be-
fore and after the horror fi lm, shows that [t] partici-
pants actually experienced fear: the mean state anxi-
ety value before the horror fi lm is signifi cantly lower
than after the movie (T-test for dependent variables:
n=41; t=–5,18; df=40, p=0,000; Mean state value be-
fore horror fi lm: 33,71; sd=7,54; mean state value
after the fi lm: 42,33; sd=10,98). Effect size is a raise in
14,37 % of the possible range. See fi gure 1.
The Scent of Fear
Figure 1: State-Anxiety values (SA) before and after the
horror fi lm (T-test for dependent variables: n=41; t=-5,18;
df=40, p=0,000, Mean state value before horror fi lm: 33,71;
sd=7,54; mean state value after the fi lm: 42,33; sd=10,98).
4141
N=
SA
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
before horror movie
after horror movie
82
In the control situation, the “neutral” fi lm, no sig-
nifi cant changes in the subjects‘ anxiety scores before
and after the presentation could be found (T-test for de-
pendent variables: n=42; t=–0, 176; df=41; n.s.; mean
state value before fi lm: 31,93; sd=5,81; mean state
value after fi lm: 32,10; sd=5,41).
When we compare the changes in the State-Anxiety
scores before and after the fi lm presentation across ex-
perimental conditions, we also fi nd a signifi cant dif-
ference. The changes in state anxiety scores in the
horror situation are distinctly larger than in the con-
trol situation (T-test for dependent variables: n=41;
t=–14,535; df=40; p=0,000; Mean difference horror:
8,73; sd=10,8; mean difference controls: 0,17; df=6,13).
Moreover, there are signifi cant differences in the fi lm
assessment of the horror fi lm and the neutral fi lm. The
test subjects rated the horror fi lm as signifi cantly less
boring (Wilcoxon; n=41; Z=–5,579; p=0,000), signifi -
cantly more frightening (Wilcoxon; n=41; Z=–5,579;
p=0,000), signifi cantly more suspenseful (Wilcoxon;
n=41; Z=–5,579; p=0,000), and signifi cantly less funny
(Wilcoxon; n=41; Z=–2,398; p<0,02) than the neutral
fi lm. Thus we can conclude that the horror fi lm actually
induced fear in our subjects.
Cortisol
When we compare the subjects’ cortisol levels be-
fore and after each fi lm presentation, we fi nd that the
cortisol concentrations decrease signifi cantly in both
the test (Wilcoxon; n=41; Z=–3,920; p=0,000; mean
cortisol level before fi lm: 4,6; sd=3,28; after fi lm:
3,57; sd=2,54) and the control situation (Wilcoxon;
n=42; Z=–5,370; p=0,000; mean level before fi lm:
4,96; sd=3,09; mean level after fi lm: 3,19; sd= 2,06).
A correlation analysis between the changes in cor-
tisol levels and the changes in state-anxiety scores
during the horror fi lm shows no signifi cant results
(Spearman: n=41; r=–0,176; p>0,05) and no signifi -
cant correlations between state-anxiety scores and cor-
tisol levels after the horror fi lm could be found (Spear-
man: n=41; r=–0,21; p>0,05).
At this point it has to be mentioned that the well-
known circadian rhythm in cortisol concentrations
[22] was also present in the fi ndings. A signifi cant
decrease in cortisol concentrations occurred with the
daytime (see table 1). Closer examination of cortisol
changes during movie presentation reveals that the
mean decrease in cortisol differs signifi cantly between
the horror and the control situation. During the pre-
Table 1: Correlations between daytime and cortisol levels (Spearman).
n Mw Sd r
s
p
Correlation between Cortisol 41 4,60 3,28 –,745 ,000
(ng/ml) at beginning of horror fi lm
and the beginning time of horror fi lm
Correlation between Cortisol 41 3,57 2,54 –,595 ,000
(ng/ml) at the end of horror fi lm and
end time of horror fi lm
Correlation between Cortisol 42 4,96 3,09 –,676 ,000
(ng/ml) at beginning of neutral fi lm
and beginning time of neutral fi lm
Correlation between Cortisol 42 3,19 2,06 –,652 ,000
(ng/ml) at end of neutral fi lm
and the end-time of neutral fi lm
Table 2: Differences in the qualitative assessment of the odour samples (U-test).
Film n Mw Sd MR Z p
no/strong odour horror 638 47,38 29,34 667,30 –2,948 <,01
neutral 635 42,63 28,57 606,56
pleasantness horror 635 35,96 21,80 613,52 –2,044 <,05
neutral 633 42,11 20,97 655,54
aggression horror 633 23,01 26,20 653,58 –1,965 <,05
neutral 633 20,72 25,43 613,42
sex horror 635 21,06 24,73 636,35 –,181 >,05
neutral 633 21,39 24,39 632,64
fear horror 634 25,61 27,02 649,58 –1,574 >,05
neutral 632 23,76 26,39 617,37
Kerstin Ackerl, Michaela Atzmueller & Karl Grammer
83
Neuroendocrinology Letters ISSN 0172–780X Copyright © 2002 Neuroendocrinology Letters
sentation of the horror fi lm, the subjects’ cortisol levels
dropped signifi cantly less than during the control fi lm
(Wilcoxon; n=41; Z=–2,138; p<0,05; mean difference
horror fi lm: 1,03; sd=1,84; mean difference control:
1,76; sd=1,58).
Odour assessment experiment
In order to fi nd out if women were able to detect a
difference between the “neutral” smell and the “fright-
ened” smell of the pads, we used a triple forced choice
test. This setting implies that a right decision by
chance would happen with a probability of 33%. We
found that the odour assessments were signifi cantly
more often correct than it could be expected by mere
chance probability (One-Sample T-test: n=41; t=3,305;
p=0,002; Mean=42,67; Sd=18,73).
Changes in participants’ cortisol levels during the
acquisition of the odour samples had no infl uence on
odour assessment, as no signifi cant correlations could
be found between changes in cortisol levels in both the
horror and the neutral fi lm situation and the percent-
age of correct decisions (Horror fi lm: n=20; r=0,132;
p>0,05; Neutral fi lm: n=21, r=–0,148; p>0,05).
If we look at the qualitative rating of the odour sam-
ples, we fi nd signifi cant differences between the assess-
ments of the “horror” and the “neutral” samples (see
table 2). We fi nd that the odour samples from the hor-
ror fi lm presentation were rated as signifi cantly more
odourous (U-Test: n=635; Z= 2,948; p<0,01; MR hor-
ror fi lm= 667,3; MR neutral fi lm= 606, 56), and less
pleasant (U-Test: n= 633; Z= 2,044; p< 0,05; MR hor-
ror fi lm= 613,52; MR neutral fi lm= 655,54) than the
samples taken after the presentation of the neutral
video. Although there are no differences in the rat-
ings “smells like fear” (U-Test; n= 632; Z=1,574;
p>0,05; MR horror fi lm= 649,58; MR neutral fi lm=
617,37) and “smells like sex” (U-Test: n=633; Z=
–0,181; p>0,05; MR horror fi lm=6363,35; MR neu-
tral fi lm=632,64) between the test situations, the
women rated the samples from the horror fi lm as more
“aggressive”, than the neutral ones (U-test: n=633;
Z=1,965; p<0,05; MR horror fi lm= 653,58; MR neu-
tral fi lm= 613,42).
Discussion
The fi rst question of interest in this study is if the
test subjects were really frightened by the test situa-
tion. The results of the STAI show that the subjects
experienced a signifi cant increase in their state anxiety
levels in the test situation. We can therefore conclude
that the horror fi lm was indeed suitable to evoke fear
in the female subjects. This result is consistent with
the result of Hubert & de Jong-Meyer [23] who found
that “[…] unpleasant fi lm stimuli reliably induce nega-
tively valenced mood changes.”
Moreover, the fear-evoking effect of the horror video
is reinforced by the results of the subjects’ fi lm as-
sessments, as they rated the horror movie as signifi -
cantly less boring, more frightening, suspenseful and
less funny than the neutral fi lm.
Changes in cortisol levels have been used as an in-
dicator for the experience of fear in many studies. In
our experiment, saliva samples were drawn in an in-
terval of about 70 minutes, just before and after the
fi lm presentation. Contrary to our expectations, the
mean cortisol concentration exhibited a decline in both
the test and the control situation. This result becomes
clearer if we look at our signifi cant negative correlation
between cortisol levels and daytime, a well-known and
often described connection (e.g. [22]). Hubert and de
Jong-Meyer [6] found similar results: in their study,
the subjects’ cortisol levels dropped continuously for
forty minutes after the start of the experiment and
then slowly raised again, but never again reached the
starting point, not even after 180 minutes. The authors
explain this result by the circadian rhythm of cortisol,
with highest levels in the morning and lowest ones in
the evening.
It is important to notice that in our experiment the
decline in cortisol levels was signifi cantly less during
the presentation of the horror fi lm than during the
presentation of the neutral one. This result indicates
that the HHN axis was indeed stimulated by the horror
fi lm and thus “worked against” the natural decline of
cortisol with daytime. The search for a dedicated “fear
pheromone” seems to be promising – the result that
there are no correlations between the amount of expe-
rienced fear and cortisol, nor between the ability to de-
tect fear and cortisol levels exclude a mere effect due to
cortisol alone.
In the second part of the experiment, the odour
assessments, we fi nd that the female subjects were
indeed able to differentiate between the odour sam-
ples from the horror fi lm and the neutral fi lm situa-
tion. The expected 33% of correct assessments by pure
chance were signifi cantly exceeded in our experiment.
This fi nding can be interpreted as a strong hint that
humans could use olfactory signals to inform their en-
vironment about their internal state, in our case, fear.
In addition to this effect, we fi nd differences in the
qualitative assessments of the odour samples between
the test and the control situation. The women rated
the smell of the pads from the horror situation as sig-
nifi cantly stronger and more unpleasant than the pads
from the neutral situation, and the odour reminded
them of “aggression”. This can be taken as another
hint that the odour we release in fearful situations is
indeed special. Additionally, Panksepp [4] assumes a
strong link between the “fear-” and the “rage-circuits”
of the brain, which could also lead us to the assumption
that the odour production that perhaps accompanies
such emotions could have a similar link.
A summary of the results of this study suggests
that humans are indeed able to smell their conspecif-
ics’ fear, and can tell a difference between “fear” and
The Scent of Fear
84
REFERENCES
1 Doty RL. Odor guided behavior in mam-
mals. Experentia 1986; 42: 257–71.
2 Kohl J, Atzmueller M, Fink B, & Grammer K.
Human Pheromones: Integrating Neuro-
endocrinology and Ethology. Neuroendo-
crinol Lett 2001; 22(5): 319–31.
3 Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE.
Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inven-
tory. Palo Alto. Consulting Psychologists
Press; 1983.
4 Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: The
foundations of human and animal emo-
tions. New York, Oxford. Oxford Plenum
Press; 1998.
5 Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary
cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine re-
search: Recent developments and ap-
plications. Psychoneuroendocrinol 1994;
19(4): 313–33.
6 Hubert W, de Jong-Meyer R. Emotional
stress and saliva cortisol response. J Clin
Chem Clin Biochem 1989; 27: 235–7.
7 Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Response
variability of salivary cortisol under psy-
chological stimulation. J Clin Chem Clin
Biochem 1989; 27: 237.
8 Hubert W, Möller M, de Jong-Meyer R.
Film-induced amusement changes in sa-
liva cortisol levels. Psychoneuroendocri-
nol 1993; 18: 265–72.
9 Ockenfels M, Smyth J, Porter L, Kirsch-
baum C. Der Einfl uß alltäglicher Stresso-
ren (daily hassles) auf die Cortisolkonzen-
tration im Speichel. Verhaltenstherapie
1995; 5 (suppl 1); 16–20.
10 Van Eck M, Berkhof H, Nicolson N, Sulon
J. The effect of perceived stress, mood
states, and stressful daily events on
salivary cortisol. Psychosom Med 1996;
58(5): 447–58.
11 Smyth J, Ockenfels MC, Porter L, Kirsch-
baum C, Hellhammer DH, Stone AA.
Stressors and mood measured on a mo-
mentary basis are associated with sali-
vary cortisol secretion. Psychoneuroen-
docrinol 1998; 23(4): 353–70.
12 Von Frisch, K. Über einen Schreckstoff der
Fischhaut und seine biologische Bedeu-
tung: Z vergl Physiol 1941; 29: 46–145.
13 Wilson, EO. The sociogenesis of insect
colonies. Science 1985; 228: 1489–95.
14 Nunez J, Almeida L, Balderrama N, Giurfa
M. Alarm pheromone induces stress anal-
gesia via an opioid system in the honey-
bee. Physiol Behav 1997; 63(1): 75–80.
15 Mc Allister MK, Roitberg BD. Adaptive
suicidal behaviour in pea aphids. Nature
1987; 328: 797–9.
16 Ressler RH, Cialdini RB, Ghoca ML, Kleist
SM. Alarm pheromone in the earthworm
Lumbricus terrestris. Science 1968; 161:
597–9.
17 Brown GE, Chivers DP, Smith R, Jan F.
Localized defecation by pike: A response
to labelling by cyprinid alarm phero-
mone? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1995; 36(2):
105–10.
18 Valenta JG, Rigby MK. Discrimination of
the odor of stressed rats. Science 1968;
161: 599–601.
19 Carr WC, Martorano RD, Krames L. Re-
sponses of mice to odors associated with
stress. J Comp Physiol Psych 1970; 71(2):
223–8.
20 Cocke R, Thiessen DD. Chemocommuni-
cation among prey and predator species.
Anim Learn Behav 1986; 14: 90–2.
21 Aitken RCB. A growing edge of measure-
ment of feelings. P Roy Soc Med 1969;
62: 989–93.
22 Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary
cortisol in psychobiological research:
An overview. Neuropsychobiology 1989;
22(3): 150–69.
23 Hubert W, de Jong-Meyer R. Saliva corti-
sol responses to unpleasant fi lm stimuli
differ between high and low trait anxious
subjects. Neuropsychobiology 1992; 25:
115–20.
“non-fear” from body odours. It seems likely that this
olfactory signal could be a “pheromone”, as it is hard
to think of other plausible biological explanations for
its production. But of course it has to be stated at this
point, that our experiment cannot prove the fear-pher-
omone hypothesis, as a pheromone – per defi nition –
has to evoke a reaction in the receiver, either a change
in behaviour, or/and in physiology. This question could
only be answered by follow-up studies.
Kerstin Ackerl, Michaela Atzmueller & Karl Grammer
What we know at the moment, as many studies in
the last few years have pointed it out, is that the human
sense of smell has by far been underestimated in the
past and that humans –like other animals- use olfac-
tory signals for the transmission of biologically relevant
information. Our fi ndings give yet another hint that
olfactory communication in humans is of great impor-
tance.