ArticlePDF Available

The efficacy of the 'mind map' study technique

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To examine the effectiveness of using the 'mind map' study technique to improve factual recall from written information. To obtain baseline data, subjects completed a short test based on a 600-word passage of text prior to being randomly allocated to form two groups: 'self-selected study technique' and 'mind map'. After a 30-minute interval the self-selected study technique group were exposed to the same passage of text previously seen and told to apply existing study techniques. Subjects in the mind map group were trained in the mind map technique and told to apply it to the passage of text. Recall was measured after an interfering task and a week later. Measures of motivation were taken. Barts and the London School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of London. 50 second- and third-year medical students. Recall of factual material improved for both the mind map and self-selected study technique groups at immediate test compared with baseline. However this improvement was only robust after a week for those in the mind map group. At 1 week, the factual knowledge in the mind map group was greater by 10% (adjusting for baseline) (95% CI -1% to 22%). However motivation for the technique used was lower in the mind map group; if motivation could have been made equal in the groups, the improvement with mind mapping would have been 15% (95% CI 3% to 27%). Mind maps provide an effective study technique when applied to written material. However before mind maps are generally adopted as a study technique, consideration has to be given towards ways of improving motivation amongst users.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The efficacy of the ‘mind map’ study technique
Paul Farrand,
1
Fearzana Hussain
2
& Enid Hennessy
3
Objectives To examine the effectiveness of using the
‘mind map’ study technique to improve factual recall
from written information.
Design To obtain baseline data, subjects completed a
short test based on a 600-word passage of text prior to
being randomly allocated to form two groups: ‘self-
selected study technique’ and ‘mind map’. After a
30-minute interval the self-selected study technique
group were exposed to the same passage of text previ-
ously seen and told to apply existing study techniques.
Subjects in the mind map group were trained in the
mind map technique and told to apply it to the passage
of text. Recall was measured after an interfering task
and a week later. Measures of motivation were taken.
Setting Barts and the London School of Medicine and
Dentistry, University of London.
Subjects 50 second- and third-year medical students.
Results Recall of factual material improved for both
the mind map and self-selected study technique
groups at immediate test compared with baseline.
However this improvement was only robust after a
week for those in the mind map group. At 1 week, the
factual knowledge in the mind map group was greater
by 10% (adjusting for baseline) (95% CI –1% to
22%). However motivation for the technique used was
lower in the mind map group; if motivation could have
been made equal in the groups, the improvement with
mind mapping would have been 15% (95% CI 3% to
27%).
Conclusion Mind maps provide an effective study
technique when applied to written material. However
before mind maps are generally adopted as a study
technique, consideration has to be given towards ways
of improving motivation amongst users.
Keywords
ANOVA
;Education, medical, undergraduate/
*methods; educational measurement, *methods;
London; *motivation; problem-based learning,
*methods.
Medical Education 2002;36:426–431
Introduction
Educational materials
1
have recently emerged which
aim to improve memory for medical information by
representing facts in the form of ‘mind maps’. Mind
mapping
2
is a study technique in which information
from a variety of sources is converted into a diagram-
matic representation of the important key words asso-
ciated with a study topic. During production, an image
representing the main study topic is initially drawn in
the centre of the mind map. Extending from this central
image are several major branches containing keywords
representing the topic subheadings, which are accom-
panied by an image whenever possible. The important
detail included under each subheading is written upon
smaller branches projecting from the subheadings with
more detailed information being connected to this
information. By undergoing this process, information
initially contained within passages of text becomes
hierarchically organized, with the most general infor-
mation being presented in the centre of the mind map
and material of increasing detail being presented at the
extremes. When the mind map is read, the central
image forms the starting point and the branch to the top
right-hand of the central image is the first branch
inspected. When this branch has been inspected the
other branches are covered in a similar manner, work-
ing in a clockwise fashion. Throughout the whole
process, imagery, colour and the visual-spatial
arrangement of the material are emphasized. Whilst
1
Department of Human Science, Barts and The London School of
Medicine and Dentistry, Queen Mary College, University of London,
London, UK (Now at Institute of Health Studies, University of
Plymouth)
2
Barts and The London School of Medicine and Dentistry, Queen
Mary College, University of London, London, UK
3
Department of Environmental and Preventive Medicine, Barts and
The London School of Medicine and Dentistry, Queen Mary College,
The University of London, London, UK
Correspondence: P A Farrand, Senior Lecturer, Institute of Health
Studies, University of Plymouth, Earl Richards Road North, Exeter,
Devon, EX2 6AS, UK. Tel.: 01392 475 123; Fax: 01392 475 151;
E-mail: p.farrand@plymouth.ac.uk
Research papers
426 Blackwell Science Ltd MEDICAL EDUCAT I O N 2002;36:426–431
many of the components used in mind maps have been
individually incorporated into commonly used study
techniques, their efficacy of use when combined within
a single study technique has not been examined.
Study techniques which have separately incorporated
imagery, colour or the visual-spatial arrangement of
keywords have each been reported to significantly
improve recall when compared with simple note taking
or rote rehearsal. For example, spatially arranging a
series of keywords into a distinctive pattern prior to
making associations, as would be common in study
techniques such as spider diagrams, significantly
improved recall.
3
The use of study techniques based
upon imagery, such as the method of loci,
4
improved
the recall of 40 words in a study in which students were
trained to associate each word with a particular campus
location during learning.
5
Furthermore, study
techniques based upon visual imagery have been shown
to be even more effective when colour was additionally
used to enhance the self-generated visual image.
6,7
That recall improves when study techniques incor-
porate the use of these components is, however, not
surprising. Even when used separately, these compo-
nents can all be seen to support cognitive processes
which have been reported to improve memory. The
theory of levels of processing
8,9
proposes that the level of
processing used whilst learning new material dictates
the success with which it will later be recalled. The
theory suggests that a deeper level of processing, such as
that achieved when meaning is extracted from incoming
information, will result in a better level of recall than
shallow processing, used when information is simply
rehearsed or written down. The elaboration of new
material has been proposed as another mechanism to
improve recall.
10
Elaboration requires associations to be
made between the new incoming information and
information already resident in memory (proactive
facilitation). When such associations are made, the
connection to information which has already been learnt
supports the learning and retention of the new infor-
mation. Additionally, the distinctiveness of information
influences its memorability. New information which is
more distinct, or which is made so during processing, is
recalled more easily than information for which the
memory trace resembles that of information already
resident in memory.
11
Whilst the effectiveness of different study techniques
for improving memory has been shown empirically, the
implications of the results for the design of educational
materials have been disappointing. Often the study
techniques have been limited in application to the recall
of ordered lists of keywords.
12,13
When attempts have
been made to apply these strategies to improving the
recall of written information, they have been largely
unsuccessful.
14,15
Furthermore, whilst many of these
study methods have been shown to improve memory
performance on a test of immediate recall, longer-term
improvements have been more elusive.
16
The limitations of many of the commonly adopted
study techniques are particularly problematic if such
techniques are applied to medical curricula based on
problem-based learning (PBL). Due to the emphasis in
PBL upon a self-directed approach,
17
students need a
study technique which will help them improve their
memory for written material whilst complementing the
deeper level of learning obtained with PBL. Such a study
technique is particularly important given that a current
weakness of PBL is that students perform worse on tests
requiring the recall of factual material when compared
with students undertaking lecture-based curricula.
18
Additionally, the greater degree of organization of
information afforded by mind maps may potentially
overcome the further weakness of PBL in that know-
ledge acquired tends to remain largely unorganized.
19
Although mind mapping may be a particularly
effective study technique within PBL curricula, its
efficacy needs to be established before such a technique
is recommended to students. This paper examines
whether mind mapping is efficacious as a study tech-
nique and whether it overcomes many limitations
identified with previous study techniques.
Methods
Sample
A total of 50 second- and third-year medical under-
graduate students, attending Barts and The London
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Lon-
don, volunteered to take part in the study after viewing
a recruitment poster. Their mean age was 20Æ1 years,
and there were 31 women and 19 men. Participants
Key learning points
Mind maps are an effective study technique when
used to improve factual recall from written mate-
rial.
Concern exists regarding levels of motivation
amongst the medical students using mind maps.
Whilst the mind map technique would seem to be
particularly suited to problem-based learning
(PBL) curricula, effective training is required to
both encourage and motivate students in its use.
Blackwell Science Ltd MEDICAL EDUCATI O N 2002;36:426–431
The ‘mind map’ study technique P Farrand et al. 427
were alternately assigned to the between subjects
conditions (mind map vs. self-selected study technique)
based on the order in which they presented for the
study. The allocation of students resulted in groups
with similar characteristics with the exception that there
were slightly fewer men in the mind map group (see
Table 1). There was no subject drop-out from any of
the sessions.
Materials
A 600-word sample of text was taken from an article
which had appeared in Scientific American. The topic of
the sample was unusual forms of transportation and had
been chosen to minimize the possibility that subjects
had prior knowledge about the material to be tested.
Three question sets containing 15 questions each were
developed from the study text. All questions were of a
similar length and required the recall of a specific piece
of information presented in the text (for example, ‘Who
ordered the cancellation of the atomic plane program?’).
Question sets were extensively piloted to ensure that
ceiling or floor effects did not arise, and each set
reflected a similar level of difficulty. The order of
presentation of the question sets was fully randomized
by both subject and session throughout the study.
Procedure
During the baseline session of the experiment, subjects
received the study text and were told to study it using
their existing study techniques. Exclusively, the tech-
niques spontaneously used were to write down the key
words, re-read or underline key words. No participant
used a method resembling mind mapping at this stage.
After 10 minutes the study text was collected and
subjects were given a 5-minute mental arithmetic test to
prevent rehearsal of the study text. During recall, sub-
jects were allowed 5 minutes to complete one of three
question sets. At the end of the baseline session, sub-
jects in the self-selected study technique condition were
informed about session 1 of the experiment and asked
to return 30 min later. Subjects in the mind map
condition however, were given a 30-minute lesson in
the mind map technique. The lesson used material
totally unrelated to the study text to demonstrate the
best ways to produce and memorize mind maps and
gave subjects the opportunity to ask questions about the
technique.
During session 1, subjects were exposed to the study
text again for an additional 10-minute period. Subjects
in the mind map condition were advised to divide the
time between reading the study text and producing a
mind map and studying it. Subjects forming the self-
selected study technique condition were advised to
divide their time between reading the text and using
their existing study methods. None of the subjects in
the self-selected study technique condition used mind
maps as their preferred method of study. Following a
mental arithmetic task all subjects were again given
5 minutes to complete the question set. Additionally all
subjects were requested to complete a 5-point scale
which assessed level of motivation in studying the
material (1 ¼very unmotivated, 5 ¼very motivated).
At the end of this session the mind maps were collected
from the mind map group and all the subjects were told
about session 2 and requested to attend 1 week later.
At session 2, all subjects were given 5 minutes to
complete the final question set without any additional
exposure to the study text.
Results
Analysis of variance (
ANOVA
) was performed, with
group (mind map vs. self-selected study technique) as
the independent variable and number of items correct
at session 1 and session 2 separately as the dependent
variables. In both cases baseline was included as a
covariate to account for differences between groups,
and in neither case were there statistically significant
differences in the mean change from baseline in the
number of correct items, although it approached
significance for the results after 1 week (P¼0Æ07)
(Table 2). Non-adjusted data are presented in Fig. 1.
Significant differences however, existed between the
levels of self-reported motivation for the groups
(t
df¼48
¼2Æ35, P¼0Æ02), with the mind map group
surprisingly reporting lower levels of motivation (mean-
¼2Æ8, SD ¼0Æ67) than the self-selected study technique
group (mean ¼3Æ2, SD ¼0Æ78) group. As there were
differences in motivation, the original data were reanal-
ysed with motivation used as an additional covariate.
With adjustment for baseline performance and
motivation, a significant difference in performance was
found between the mind map and the self-selected
Table 1 Characteristics of the mind map and self-selected study
technique groups
Mind map Self-selected study technique
Number 25 25
Mean age, years 20 20
Male, % 32 44
Baseline score 5 4Æ8
The ‘mind map’ study technique P Farrand et al.
428
Blackwell Science Ltd MEDICAL EDUCAT I O N 2002;36:426–431
study groups. For both session 1 and session 2, the
mind map group recalled significantly more correct
items than the self-selected study group, showing a
clear study advantage when the mind map was used,
which was robust over a 1-week time delay (see
Table 2).
That motivation may have differentially affected the
mind map and self-selected study technique groups is
further supported by a different pattern of partial
correlations with baseline performance controlled.
Motivation was significantly correlated with perform-
ance at both session 1 and session 2 for the self-selected
study technique group (r
df¼22
¼0Æ57, P¼0Æ002;
r
df¼22
¼0Æ45, P¼0Æ039, respectively). Correlations
were, however, smaller for the mind map group, only
reaching full significance at session 1 (r
df¼22
¼0Æ41,
P¼0Æ023; r
df¼22
¼0Æ30, P¼0Æ07, respectively). Cau-
tion should be exercised when interpreting these partial
correlations, however, because of the small sample size
for each group (n¼25). (Additionally, all analyses
were further replicated with sex as a covariate, but this
did not change any of the conclusions.)
Discussion
Analysis of the data indicates that, as a strategy to
improve memory for written information, the mind map
technique has the potential for an important improve-
ment in efficacy. With the mind map technique there is
an estimated increase of 10% without any change in
motivation, and an increase of 13% if there were such a
change. Mind maps are unlikely to worsen the learning
process as the lower end of the 95% confidence interval
excluded a deterioration of more than 1%, and it is likely
in practice that those who did not like the technique
would continue with the one they currently used. At
both session 1 and session 2, the mind map technique
resulted in correct recall of a statistically significantly
higher number of items than the self-selected study
technique, when adjustments for baseline score and
motivation were made. Importantly, the recall ad-
vantage with the mind map technique was robust over a
week, resulting in a 24% proportional increase in correct
recall when compared with baseline score. Recall
performance in the self-selected study technique was
actually a little worse (–6%) over the week. That
improvements in recall at a week re-test remained evi-
dent with just a single exposure to the mind map tech-
nique suggests a real benefit of this study technique
compared with other memory strategies where benefits
have been confined only to tests of immediate recall.
16
That differences exist between the groups in
memory performance suggests that improvements that
arise with mind maps are likely to be dependent upon
Table 2 Difference in mean correct answers (95% CI) between mind map and self-selected study technique groups, at session 1 (day 1)
and session 2 (day 7)
Difference between mean
correct answers
% difference in proportion of
correct answers
Pvalue
Session 1(day 1)
Adjusted for baseline 0Æ8()0Æ6to2Æ3) 5Æ5()4Æ3to15Æ9) 0Æ26
Adjusted for baseline and motivation 1Æ7(0Æ3to3Æ1) 11Æ3(2Æ1to20Æ4) 0Æ016
Session 2 (day 7)
Adjusted for baseline 1Æ6()0Æ2to3Æ4) 10Æ7()1Æ1to22Æ5) 0Æ07
Adjusted for baseline and motivation 2Æ3(0Æ5to4Æ1) 15Æ3(3Æ3to27Æ3) 0Æ013
Figure 1 Mean unadjusted correct recall (maximum score ¼15),
using mind map and self-selected study techniques.
The ‘mind map’ study technique P Farrand et al. 429
Blackwell Science Ltd MEDICAL EDUCATI O N 2002;36:426–431
improvements in cognitive processing. Whilst it is
beyond the scope of this study to identify the nature of
the cognitive processes supported when mind maps are
used, it is likely that mind maps encourage a deeper
level of processing than that obtained with the other,
more conventional study techniques adopted in the
self-selected study technique. A deeper level of infor-
mation processing has been associated with better
academic performance by medical students.
20
It has
also been acknowledged, however, that fostering a
deep level of learning is very difficult because students
do not spontaneously adopt strategies that foster such
learning.
21
A highly surprising finding was that motivation in the
self-selected study technique group was significantly
higher than that in the mind map group, even though
participants in the latter group received training in the
mind map technique.
Straightforward relationships between training and
motivation have been extensively reported,
22,23
with
previous research also indicating that that the provision
of task training is often accompanied by increased
motivation in engaging with and using a study tech-
nique.
24
Training is believed to focus individuals
towards directing their efforts to working on the task
given rather than dividing their attention between other
tasks, and to increase the amount of time the individual
chooses to spend completing an activity.
25
For both
groups, the well-established relationship between
motivation and recall performance
26
was supported.
This relationship was weaker for the mind map group,
however, and was only marginally significant (P¼0Æ07)
at session 2.
A possibility which might account for the lower level
of motivation in the mind map group is that there is
resistance to using memory strategies as study aids.
Students, especially males, have been reported to be
reluctant to use many memory strategies, preferring to
adopt more conventional techniques such as rehearsal,
repetition and summary elaboration, techniques
exclusively adopted by the students in the self-selected
study group.
27
Motivation in the mind map group may
therefore have been lower than in the self-selected study
group as students were trained and instructed to use a
strategy they were already reluctant to employ, which
may potentially raise other issues such as compliance in
using this study technique.
28
Higher levels of motiva-
tion might have been obtained if the medical students
used as participants had believed the mind map method
might improve their exam results, or if they had used it
to study medically related information. Previous
research indicates that students are only motivated in
their use of memory strategies when they are also
interested in the material being studied.
29
It should be
noted, however, that if successful efforts were made to
counter the reductions in motivation encountered when
the mind map technique was used, significant further
recall benefits of mind mapping would be expected.
This paper has shown the efficacy of using mind
maps as a study aid, even when use has been limited to
a single exposure. The increased use of mind maps, and
the emergence of educational materials supporting the
use of mind maps, within medical curricula, should
therefore be cautiously welcomed. The mind map
technique would seem to be particularly suited to
medical curricula based around PBL, as both approa-
ches support, and encourage students to adopt a deeper
level of learning. Nevertheless, before mind maps were
recommended as a study technique, a way of providing
effective training would need to be established so that
students were encouraged and enthusiastic about
adopting this approach in preference to other more
conventional study techniques. With this aim in mind,
one possibility would be the incorporation of a mind
map training course into the first few terms of the
medical curriculum, accompanying other sessions in
study skills often provided during the initial stages of a
PBL curriculum.
Acknowledgements
Thanks are due to Richard Rowe for comments on an
earlier draft of this paper.
Contributors
Contributions to the paper were as follows: Fearzana
Hussain originated the idea behind the research and
conducted the experimentation; Paul Farrand designed
the experiment and wrote the paper, whilst Enid
Hennessy conducted the statistical analysis.
Funding
The research was completed without external funding.
References
1 McDermott P, Clarke DN. Mind Maps in Medicine. Edin-
burgh: Churchill Livingstone; 1998.
2 Buzan T, Buzan B. The Mind Map Book. London: BBC
Books; 1997.
3 Bellezza FS. The spatial arrangement mnemonic. J Educ
Psychol 1983;75:830–7.
4 Bellezza FS, Goverdhan RB. Mnemonic devices and natural
memory. Bull Psychon Soc 1978;11:277–80.
The ‘mind map’ study technique P Farrand et al.
430
Blackwell Science Ltd MEDICAL EDUCAT I O N 2002;36:426–431
5 Ross J, Lawrence KA. Some observations on memory artifice.
Psychon Sci 1968;13:107–8.
6 Jamieson DG, Schimpf MG. Self-generated images are more
effective mnemonics. J Ment Imag 1980;4:25–33.
7 Bower G. Organizational factors in memory. Cognit Psychol
1970;1:18–46.
8 Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: a framework for
memory research. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 1972;11:671–84.
9 Lockhart RS, Craik FIM. Levels of processing: a retrospective
commentary on a framework for memory research. Can
J Psychol 1990;44:87–112.
10 Anderson JR, Reder LM. An elaborative processing explan-
ation of depth of processing. In: LS Cermack, FIM Craik, eds.
Levels of Processing in Human Memory. Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum;
1979.
11 Eysenck MW, Eysenck MC. Effects of processing depth,
distinctiveness and word frequency on retention. Br J Psychol
1980;71:263–74.
12 Tess DE, Hutchinson RL, Treloar JH, Jenkins CM. Bizarre
imagery and distinctiveness: implications for the classroom.
J Ment Imag 1999;23:153–70.
13 Brooks JO, Friedman L, Pearman AM, Gray C, Yesavage JA.
Mnemonic training in older adults: effects of age, length of
training and type of cognitive pretraining. Int Psychogeriatr
1999;11:75–84.
14 De Beni R, Mo A, Cornoldi C. Learning from texts or lec-
tures: loci mnemonics can interfere with reading but not with
listening. Eur J Cognit Psychol 1997;9:401–15.
15 Hunter IHL. Imagery, comprehension and mnemonics.
J Ment Imag 1977;1:65–72.
16 Thomas MH, Wang AY. Learning by keyword mnemonic:
looking for long-term benefits. J Exp Psychol App
1996;2:330–42.
17 Davis MH, Harden RM. AMEE Medical education guide no.
15. Problem based learning: a practical guide. Med Teacher
1999;21:130–40.
18 Albernese MA, Mitchell S. Problem-based learning: a review
of literature on its outcomes and implementation issues. Acad
Med 1993;68:52–81.
19 Hemker HC. Critical perceptions of problem-based learning.
Adv Health Sci Educ 1998;3:71–6.
20 McManus IC, Richards P, Winder BC, Sprotson KA. Clinical
experience, performance in final examinations, and learning
style in medical students: prospective study. Br Med
J1998;316:345–50.
21 Sandberg J, Yvonne B. Deep learning is difficult. Instruct Sci
1997;25:15–36.
22 Stipek DJ. Motivation and instruction. In: DC Berliner, RC
Calfee, eds. Handbook of Educational Psychology. London:
Prentice Hall International; 1996.
23 Lepper MR. Motivational considerations in the study of
instruction. Cognit Instruct 1988;5:289–309.
24 Ford ME. Motivating Humans: Goals, Emotions and Personal
Agency Beliefs. London: Sage; 1992.
25 Kanfer R. Motivation theory. In: MD Dunnette, ML Hough,
eds. Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, vol. 1.
California: Consulting Psychologists’ Press; 1996.
26 Eagly AH, Chen S, Chaiken S, Shaw Barnes K. The impact of
attitudes on memory: an affair to remember. Psychol Bull
1999;125:64–89.
27 Soler MJ, Ruiz JC. The spontaneous use of memory aids at
different educational levels. Appl Cognit Psychol 1999;10:41–51.
28 Campos A, Loepoz A, Perez MJ. Non-compliance with
instructions in studies of the use of imagery as a memory aid.
Imag Cognit Personal 1999;18:241–9.
29 Krapp A. Interest, motivation and learning: an educational-
psychological perspective. Eur J Psychol Educ 1999;14:23–40.
Received 14 April 2000; editorial comments to authors 1August 2000
and 21 June 2001; accepted for publication 14 August 2001
The ‘mind map’ study technique P Farrand et al. 431
Blackwell Science Ltd MEDICAL EDUCATI O N 2002;36:426–431
... Therefore, to correct the mentioned limitations, we chosed to model our data by a mind-map [15] that can help us to make new connections between features that we may not have previously considered. By visually representing the data, we may be able to identify relationships and patterns that are not immediately apparent from looking at the data in a more traditional format. ...
... To summarize in this paper, we present the following major contributions: (1) Data modeling by mind-maps to highlight hidden patterns that can also be generalized for other case studies; we utilize a mind-mapping [15] technique to represent stroke and identify pertinent variables. This technique enables the extraction of relevant features and provides a semantic understanding of the data. ...
Article
According to the Global Health Observatory (GHO), stroke ranks second worldwide in causing dementia, right after Alzheimer's disease. The mortality rate linked to dementia resulting from stroke is high because symptoms are often recognized late, and stroke can be misinterpreted as other brain disorders. Early detection and diagnosis of stroke is crucial. Therefore, increasing awareness of stroke symptoms and implementing preventive measures becomes imperative. Prompt intervention by healthcare professionals can improve outcomes and reduce long-term complications of stroke. The research introduces an innovative approach for early stroke prediction using a fuzzy scoring-based Decision Support System. This approach encompasses three main modules: Mind-map based Data Modeling, Fuzzy scoring computing, and ML-based Decision System. By incorporating fuzzy logic, the approach extracts valuable knowledge from imprecise and uncertain data. Combining the fuzzy stroke risk model with a machine learning-based decision support system aims to enhance stroke prediction accuracy and improve preventive measures and patient outcomes. The approach's effectiveness was validated using real clinical data and tested with various machine learning classifiers, including K-Nearest Neighbour (KNN), Logistic Regression (LR), Decision Tree (DT), Artificial Neural Network (ANN), and Support Vector Machine (SVM). The results showed a strong correlation between stroke cases and computed risk-scoring values. In comparison to predictions without fuzzy scoring and other related works, the stroke risk prediction using the proposed approach demonstrated higher accuracy, making it a promising method for early stroke detection and prevention.
... 4. Developing critical thinking: thinking for oneself, evaluating information critically, considering how information might be applied in novel situations (Chen et al., 2011;Daly, 2001;Daley et al., 1999;Garwood et al., 2018;Kaddoura et al., 2016;Lee et al., 2013;Taylor & Littleton-Kierney, 2011;Vacek, 2009;Wheeler & Collins, 2003). 5. Assisting with problem solving: utilizing what has been learned to solve complex questions (González et al., 2008). 6. Assisting with long term recall: establishing stable memory of the material learned rather than short-term memory that is quickly forgotten after the course is over (Farrand et al., 2002;Lee et al., 2013;Nicoara et al., 2020;Schwendimann, 2015). 7. Assisting with application outside of the classroom: in this case, application within a clinical environment (Aliyari et al., 2019;Daley, 2004;Harrison & Gibbons, 2013;Daley et al., 1999;Noon, 2013;Kaddoura et al., 2016). ...
Article
Heavy content, high volume courses commonly create challenges for undergraduate students. Two such courses at our university are the introductory semesters of anatomy and physiology for first-year nursing students, taught by biology instructors. Despite the vast literature indicating the efficacy of using concept maps as a tool for learning, it is still not commonly implemented in the field. An adaptation called skeleton maps, were developed and implemented to assist student learning. This qualitative SoTL study reports on student perceptions of the learning gains of utilizing these maps, under six themes that fall into behavioural, cognitive, and opinion-based categories. The results of this study indicate a strong evidence base for using concept-map based strategies within similar courses.
... Many studies have highlighted the crucial role of mind mapping in fostering divergent thinking. However, the assessment of mind mapping's effectiveness on students' divergent thinking skills has primarily relied on traditional testing methods, which focus mainly on completed translation works, judged purely based on the scores obtained by students [20,21]. These traditional assessment methods may not fully capture the dynamic and creative nature of divergent thinking, nor accurately reflect the true impact of mind mapping on students' ability to think outside the box and generate novel ideas. ...
Article
Full-text available
As a translation strategy, mind mapping has a positive influence on the development of students’ divergent thinking skills. However, research on how mind mapping affects divergent thinking has primarily relied on traditional testing methods, which mainly focus on completed translation tasks. There is still a lack of research using dynamic and procedural experimental methods. An electroencephalogram (EEG) is one of the most effective tools for identifying and analyzing cognitive processing. In this study, we utilized EEG data to explore the effects of mind mapping on divergent thinking skills, with a primary focus on the dynamics of the alpha band power, which is closely related to divergent thinking. The participants were university students learning foreign languages in Dalian, China. One group received foreign language translation teaching integrated with mind mapping training (experimental group), while the other group received regular foreign language translation teaching (control group). The task-related alpha band power of participants engaged in divergent thinking tasks shows that participants in the experimental group exhibited superior divergent thinking skills, as evidenced by greater elevations in alpha band power within the anterior frontal lobe (M = 0.582, SD = 0.301), frontal lobe (M = 0.312, SD= 0.103), frontocentral lobe (M = 0.183, SD = 0.256), and temporal lobe (M = 0.205, SD = 0.176). In addition, they displayed significantly greater alpha band activity in the right cerebral hemisphere (M = 0.418, SD = 0.271), further highlighting the unique neural patterns linked with divergent thinking. The empirical results indicate that mind mapping training is an effective method for enhancing students’ divergent thinking skills.
... It was determined that the mind mapping technique has an effect on recall in the acquisition of grammar structures in teaching Turkish as a foreign language. In 4 sub-themes, it was concluded that it has an effect on recall by providing easy recall, reducing forgetting, keeping the subjects in mind like a photograph on the diagram, and using different colors and branches (Farrand, Hussain, & Hennessy, 2002;Çamlı, 2009;Gömleksiz & Fidan, 2013;Harbi, 2013;Bawaneh, 2018;Normawati, 2020;Serevina & Heluth;. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: As in other languages, Turkish also consists of words formed by the combination of sounds and sounds and meaningful integrity formed by the combination of words in a ruleful way. Therefore, it needs some rules for both the formation of sounds, the formation of words and the formation of sentences, which constitutes grammar. In order to learn a foreign language fully and properly, it is necessary to master the rules of grammar. Methods and techniques designed for learning something in the best and shortest time are important and necessary for teaching grammar in foreign language teaching. The aim of this study is to examine the effectiveness of mind mapping technique, which is a different and new technique, in teaching grammar structures in teaching Turkish as a foreign language. Design/Methodology/Approach: In the study, mind mapping diagrams were used to teach grammar structures in teaching Turkish as a foreign language. According to this diagram, language consists of three components: form, function and use. For this reason, all grammar structures to be explained in the lesson were organized according to these three components and diagrams were prepared. Grammar structures were taught through the diagrams whose instructor opinions were taken. “Interview technique”, one of the qualitative data collection tools, was used in the study. For this, a semi-structured interview form was used. At Sakarya University TÖMER, a total of 22 learners at the A2 level were taught with the mind mapping technique for 7 weeks, and then the learners were interviewed one by one with a semi-structured interview form. Content analysis was used to analyze the data. The data obtained through content analysis were divided into themes and sub-themes. Findings: When the mind mapping technique in teaching grammar structures in teaching Turkish as a foreign language was analyzed according to learner opinions, 5 themes and 23 sub-themes were identified. Highlights: According to the opinions of the learners, positive results such as the mind mapping technique being effective in grammar teaching, being useful, having a structure that offers ease of teaching as well as contributing to recall and developing a positive attitude towards the lesson were determined.
... However, the knowledge gained immediately after the intervention was significantly higher for both topics in the mind map group compared to the conventional reading group, with a statistically significant P value. These results were consistent with findings from the study by Bhat et al. [18] In contrast, studies conducted by D'Antoni et al. and Kalyanasundaram et al. yielded different outcomes or trends. The study found that the mean scores in the immediate post-test were higher in the mind map group, although this difference was not statistically significant. ...
Article
Full-text available
BACKGROUND Nursing education, rooted in conventional methods like didactic lectures and clinical experiences, is evolving to incorporate innovative learning strategies. This study delves into the effectiveness of mind mapping, an interactive technique, in enhancing information retrieval skills among nursing students. Departing from linear note-taking, mind maps visually organize information, aligning with contemporary learning theories emphasizing visual and interactive modalities. MATERIALS AND METHODS Methodologically, a quasi-experimental design with a crossover intervention was employed, involving 144 nursing students in Pune City, Maharashtra; the time taken to conduct the study was 2 months. The study focused on acquired immunodeficiency syndrome and chronic renal failure (CRF), employing pre-tests, post-tests, and long-term retention assessments. RESULTS Results indicated significantly higher knowledge gains and retention with mind mapping, substantiated by effect size analyses. Student perceptions, assessed through a Likert scale, revealed widespread agreement on mind mapping’s clarity, effectiveness, and positive impact on understanding and retention. Challenges included the need for faculty and student training, highlighting the importance of integration and support in educational institutions. CONCLUSION This study explores the effectiveness of mind mapping as a learning technique in nursing education, aiming to enhance knowledge acquisition, retention, and critical thinking skills, departing from traditional approaches. Results indicate that mind mapping significantly improves knowledge retention compared to conventional methods. Hence, we strongly recommend mind mapping as an integral learning technique in nursing education; this will impact on enhancing knowledge acquisition, retention, and critical thinking skills among nursing students, surpassing the outcomes of traditional methods.
... Thanks to Mind Mapping, the teacher can also stimulate the attention of students and actively engage them in building their own map, which will motivate them and develop their self-confidence. In fact, a comparative study conducted by Farrand et al. [16] on two groups of medical students showed that the use of heuristic maps or Mind Mapping develops long-term memory and stimulates motivation. The conceptualization of a concept map can be done either with pen and paper or with freely downloadable software on the Internet, such as Freemind or Coggle. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
In the ever-evolving landscape of education, adapting to the rapid flow of information is paramount. This chapter immerses itself in the realm of active pedagogy, dissecting its neuroeducational underpinnings that hold the potential to redefine the future of learning. At its core is an examination of neuroplasticity, unraveling the intricate ways in which the brain adapts and absorbs knowledge. The chapter intricately analyzes the interplay between brain plasticity and dynamic techniques such as Mind Mapping, Problem-Based Learning, and emotional intelligence. By cultivating a profound understanding of the evolving nature of learners’ brains, this holistic approach seeks to reshape the very foundations of education. The narrative unfolds, emphasizing the pivotal role of neuroeducation, presenting evolving models, and showcasing practical resources such as mind mapping and emotional intelligence, providing educators with a comprehensive toolkit for transformative teaching.
Article
Full-text available
Technical Design Documents (TDD) are commonly adopted documents in the software development industry that describe the technical solution implementation design. These documents are informal and lack strict requirements regarding their content. The structure often differs from organization to organization. Lack of standardized structure often results in low-quality design documents that can hinder successful technical solution implementation. In this paper, we applied a grounded theory method to analyze various organization-specific TDD structures and proposed a standardized structure for this document. In addition to the plain text structure, we visualized it in the form of a mindmap to facilitate the adoption of this template. Organizations can use the proposed TDD template structure as a basis for the company-specific structure of TDD and may include all or part of the sections proposed.
Article
Full-text available
Many theories of the effects of attitudes on memory for attitude-relevant information would predict that attitudinally congenial information should be more memorable than uncongenial information. Yet, this meta-analysis showed that this congeniality effect is inconsistent across the experiments in this research literature and small when these effects are aggregated. The tendency of the congeniality effect to decrease over the years spanned by this literature appeared to reflect the weaker methods used in the earlier studies. The effect was stronger in 2 kinds of earlier experiments that may be tinged with artifact: those in which the coding of recall measures was not known to be blind and those that used recognition measures that were not corrected for bias. Nonetheless, several additional characteristics of the studies moderated the congeniality effect and suggested that both attitude structure and motivation to process attitude-relevant information are relevant to understanding the conditions under which people have superior memory for attitudinally congenial or uncongenial information.
Article
Full-text available
Subjects compliance with instructions was evaluated in tests of the efficacy of normal or bizarre imagery as memory aids. High-school students (total sample size = 500) were presented with lists of thirty-two word pairs, and instructed to form normal or bizarre images as aids to remembering the words in each pair. Subjects also completed a questionnaire designed to evaluate the type of imagery used in each case. On the basis of the completed questionnaires, two independent judges then classified each subject as a “complier” or “non-complier.” The proportion of non-compliers was significantly larger among subjects instructed to use bizarre images than among subjects instructed to use normal images. Recall rates were significantly higher 1) among subjects who used bizarre images than among subjects who used normal images. 2) among compliers than among non-compliers, and 3) immediately after presentation of the word list than one week later.
Article
Full-text available
An attempt was made to make a mnemonic device, the method of loci, function more like natural memory. This was done by using familiar loci provided by the subject and by making learning incidental. After comparing the three conditions used in the experiment, it was concluded that recall of the verbal material presented to the subjects depended on the associa- tion of the loci with the words presented and also depended on the retrieval of the loci so that they could act as recall cues. The operation of natural memory seems to be similar to the method of loci for the following reasons: (1) The routine of people’s lives automatically provides loci that are associated with less predictable events. (2) Perception rather than imagery associates loci with these events. (3) The accessibility in memory of familiar loci allows them to be used as retrieval cues for recent, but less predictable, episodic events.
Article
In this study, we explore to what extent loci mnemonics can help learning from textual information and examine the extent and possible causes of the oral presentation e€ ect (Cornoldi & De Beni, 1991) ; that is, the loci method is more useful when the presentation modality is oral rather than written. The hypothesis presented is that the e€ ect is due to selective interference between reading and visuo-spatial imagery in the use of loci mnemonics. We carried out three experiments, varying the modalities of presentation and of testing. In a series of sessions, subjects were trained in the use of loci mnemonics and verbal repetition, and were then tested for memory of texts presented in either a written or oral form. Our results showed that loci mnemonics can also enhance memory for texts when presentation time is controlled, if the modality of presentation is oral. This last result supports the selective interference explanation of the oral presentation e€ ect.
Book
Rationale for Motivational Systems Theory Theoretical Foundation for Motivational Systems Theory The Living Systems Framework Defining Motivation and Its Role in Effective Human Functioning Personal Goals Directing and Organizing Behavior Through Cognitive Representations of Desired and Undesired Outcomes Personal Agency Beliefs and Emotional Arousal Processes Regulating Behavior Through the Integration of Cognition and Affect Integration of Historical and Contemporary Theories of Motivation Goals, Emotions, and Personal Agency Beliefs How to 'Motivate' People General Principles and Specific Applications to Enduring Problems in Child and Adolescent Development, Education, Business, and Counseling and Everyday Living Summary of Motivational Systems Theory