ArticlePDF Available

Suspects, Lies, and Videotape: An Analysis of Authentic High-Stake Liars

Authors:

Abstract

This study is one of the very few, and the most extensive to date, which has examined deceptive behavior in a real-life, high-stakes setting. The behavior of 16 suspects in their police interviews has been analyzed. Clips of video footage have been selected where other sources (reliable witness statements and forensic evidence) provide evidence that the suspect lied or told the truth. Truthful and deceptive behaviors were compared. The suspects blinked less frequently and made longer pauses during deceptive clips than during truthful clips. Eye contact was maintained equally for deceptive and truthful clips. These findings negate the popular belief amongst both laypersons and professional lie detectors (such as the police) that liars behave nervously by fidgeting and avoiding eye contact. However, large individual differences were present.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Suspects, Lies, and Videotape: An Analysis of Authentic High-Stake Liars
Samantha Mann; Aldert Vrij; Ray Bull
Law and Human Behavior; Jun 2002; 26, 3; ABI/INFORM Global
pg. 365
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
... Therefore, this study, resulting from a collaboration between researchers in linguistics and criminology, aims to examine how suspects use language to conceal the truth during investigative interviews. The work falls within the long tradition of studies on lying but deviates from it in its approach: while most studies on the subject have been conducted in the field of psychology (DePaulo et al., 2003;Hartwig et al., 2011;Mann et al., 2002;Vrij et al., 2014), this study is based on forensic linguistics (Coulthard et al., 2020;Gibbons, 2003;Shuy, 1993). The field of forensic linguistics involves the application of linguistic knowledge and theory, in the present case discourse analysis, to forensic, legal, or criminal contexts (Coulthard, 2010;Lagorgette, 2010). ...
... The importance of being able to detect lies, particularly those that involve crimes as they have implications for public safety, has led to extensive research on the topic (e.g. DePaulo et al., 2003;Hartwig et al., 2011;Laforest et al., 2020;Mann et al., 2002;Vrij et al., 2014). Most studies of deception detection rely on experimental designs conducted with volunteers, typically university students in psychology, who are asked to produce a narrative of events they have experienced or events they have never experienced but are trying to present as true (i.e. ...
... In an attempt to overcome the limitations of laboratory studies, some researchers have focused on lies produced in the context of high-stakes, real-world cases where the consequences for the (alleged) deceiver can be severe: emergency calls (Harpster et al., 2009;Laforest, 2012;Laforest et al., 2020;Miller et al., 2021), investigative interviews with suspects (Mann et al., 2002;Shuy, 1998), criminal trials (Fornaciari and Poesio, 2013), or plea appeals in the media for the return of a missing loved one (Brinke and Porter, 2012;Wright Whelan et al., 2014). The methodologies used in these studies are similar: the discourse of individuals who have provided accurate information is compared to that of individuals who, post-investigation, were found guilty of the alleged crime or of being an accomplice to it. ...
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this study is to investigate how individuals convicted of online sexual crimes use language to deceive police investigators during investigative interviews. Discursive and interactional cues that may indicate deception were identified based on the suspects’ responses to questions asked by the police investigator, that is, whether the questions had a high or low potential to elicit deceit. To refine the selection of deceptive answers to be analyzed, the information that was contradicted or questioned during the interview was then targeted for each suspect. The results show that seven elements – grammatical negations, argumentative markers, uncertainty markers, use of the conditional, ignorance markers, sincerity markers, and volubility – distinguish between truthful and deceptive responses. Findings suggest that these elements are used as strategies to convince the investigator of the truthfulness of the (deceptive) statements being made and to avoid providing information that could be refuted during the interview.
... Attention estimation has proven to be of great value in important areas such as driver fatigue detection [3], [4], advertising and product design [5], mental health disorders [6], lie detection [7], [8], human-computer interfaces [9], education [10], etc. ...
... These tasks were designed to induce changes in students' attention and evaluate the cognitive load associated with each situation. Among the tasks included in the acquisition protocol, the task of committing fraud/copying was included, as previous research demonstrated that this activity requires a higher cognitive load [8]. Students were presented with diverse scenarios to engage in copying responses, like using different electronic devices (mobile phones, laptops), employing "cheat sheets," interacting with peers to obtain answers, and more. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This work introduces an innovative method for estimating attention levels (cognitive load) using an ensemble of facial analysis techniques applied to webcam videos. Our method is particularly useful, among others, in e-learning applications, so we trained, evaluated, and compared our approach on the mEBAL2 database, a public multi-modal database acquired in an e-learning environment. mEBAL2 comprises data from 60 users who performed 8 different tasks. These tasks varied in difficulty, leading to changes in their cognitive loads. Our approach adapts state-of-the-art facial analysis technologies to quantify the users' cognitive load in the form of high or low attention. Several behavioral signals and physiological processes related to the cognitive load are used, such as eyeblink, heart rate, facial action units, and head pose, among others. Furthermore, we conduct a study to understand which individual features obtain better results, the most efficient combinations, explore local and global features, and how temporary time intervals affect attention level estimation, among other aspects. We find that global facial features are more appropriate for multimodal systems using score-level fusion, particularly as the temporal window increases. On the other hand, local features are more suitable for fusion through neural network training with score-level fusion approaches. Our method outperforms existing state-of-the-art accuracies using the public mEBAL2 benchmark.
... Attention estimation has proven to be of great value in important areas such as driver fatigue detection [3], [4], advertising and product design [5], mental health disorders [6], lie detection [7], [8], human-computer interfaces [9], education [10], etc. ...
... These tasks were designed to induce changes in students' attention and evaluate the cognitive load associated with each situation. Among the tasks included in the acquisition protocol, the task of committing fraud/copying was included, as previous research demonstrated that this activity requires a higher cognitive load [8]. Students were presented with diverse scenarios to engage in copying responses, like using different electronic devices (mobile phones, laptops), employing "cheat sheets," interacting with peers to obtain answers, and more. ...
Article
Full-text available
This work introduces an innovative method for estimating attention levels (cognitive load) using an ensemble of facial analysis techniques applied to webcam videos. Our method is particularly useful, among others, in e-learning applications, so we trained, evaluated, and compared our approach on the mEBAL2 database, a public multi-modal database acquired in an e-learning environment. mEBAL2 comprises data from 60 users who performed 8 different tasks. These tasks varied in difficulty, leading to changes in their cognitive loads. Our approach adapts state-of-the-art facial analysis technologies to quantify the users’ cognitive load in the form of high or low attention. Several behavioral signals and physiological processes related to the cognitive load are used, such as eyeblink, heart rate, facial action units, and head pose, among others. Furthermore, we conduct a study to understand which individual features obtain better results, the most efficient combinations, explore local and global features, and how temporary time intervals affect attention level estimation, among other aspects.We find that global facial features are more appropriate for multimodal systems using score-level fusion, particularly as the temporal window increases. On the other hand, local features are more suitable for fusion through neural network training with score-level fusion approaches. Our method outperforms existing state-of-the-art accuracies using the public mEBAL2 benchmark.
... As a result of this increased cognitive demand, less resources are left to maintain the fluency of the stream of speech, causing liars to appear less fluent. Indeed, a number of studies provide evidence that lies are associated with more disfluencies (DePaulo et al., 1982;Mann et al., 2002;Porter & ten Brinke, 2010;Vrij & Winkel, 1991;Wright Whelan et al., 2014). In the earliest of these studies (DePaulo et al., 1982), participants were asked to either lie or tell the truth about liking or disliking a person from their personal lives. ...
... Authors reported more ah-filled pauses during deceptive speech compared to truthful speech. Mann et al. (2002) compared the speech of 16 suspects during police interviews. They reported that suspects employed longer pauses when lying compared to when speaking the truth. ...
... While studies consistently show weak effects and minimal differences between truth-tellers and liars, misconceptions about the diagnostic value of these non-verbal behaviours persist among legal actors and laypersons 5,[16][17][18][19] . Adding to this complexity, even researchers studying deception hold conflicting views on the universality and consistency of non-verbal cues across situations 20 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Non-verbal behaviour, such as facial expressions and body language, plays a critical role in assessments of witnesses’ credibility that inform legal decisions in cases involving crime. While prior research has primarily focused on associations between non-verbal cues and deception, this study investigates the relation between non-verbal cues and recall memory accuracy in honestly reported mock eyewitness testimonies. Using a sample of 36 video-recorded eyewitness testimonies about a violent crime (n = 680 statements), we examined whether non-verbal cues were associated with statement accuracy (correct vs. incorrect), witness credibility as rated by independent observers, and witnesses’ self-reported confidence. Additionally, we explored whether these associations differed for native vs. non-native speaking witnesses. Results revealed no associations between non-verbal cues and statement accuracy or perceived credibility. Furthermore, while non-native speakers were perceived as less credible, these perceptions were not related to non-verbal cues. Our findings contradict common beliefs by showing that non-verbal behaviour is not reliably related to accuracy or perceived credibility in eyewitness testimonies, highlighting the need for caution in their use in high-stakes legal contexts.
... Deception is a set of actions and behaviours that aim to mislead or conceal the truth from others. When deception is detected, consequences can occur that raise the stakes for the deceptive person, which influences their behaviour [1]. Deception detection has been a concern for several entities besides the police. ...
Article
Full-text available
Detecting deceptive behaviour for surveillance and border protection is critical for a country’s security. With the advancement of technology in relation to sensors and artificial intelligence, recognising deceptive behaviour could be performed automatically. Following the success of affective computing in emotion recognition from verbal and nonverbal cues, we aim to apply a similar concept for deception detection. Recognising deceptive behaviour has been attempted; however, only a few studies have analysed this behaviour from gait and body movement. This research involves a multimodal approach for deception detection from gait, where we fuse features extracted from body movement behaviours from a video signal, acoustic features from walking steps from an audio signal, and the dynamics of walking movement using an accelerometer sensor. Using the video recording of walking from the Whodunnit deception dataset, which contains 49 subjects performing scenarios that elicit deceptive behaviour, we conduct multimodal two-category (guilty/not guilty) subject-independent classification. The classification results obtained reached an accuracy of up to 88% through feature fusion, with an average of 60% from both single and multimodal signals. Analysing body movement using single modality showed that the visual signal had the highest performance followed by the accelerometer and acoustic signals. Several fusion techniques were explored, including early, late, and hybrid fusion, where hybrid fusion not only achieved the highest classification results, but also increased the confidence of the results. Moreover, using a systematic framework for selecting the most distinguishing features of guilty gait behaviour, we were able to interpret the performance of our models. From these baseline results, we can conclude that pattern recognition techniques could help in characterising deceptive behaviour, where future work will focus on exploring the tuning and enhancement of the results and techniques.
... A related study employed a similar methodology among 16 criminal suspects, nine of whom were charged with theft, four charged with murder, two charged with arson, and one charged with attempted rape. Ten of the 16 offenders were well known to the police based on prior contacts (Mann, Vrij, and Bull 2002). Truthfulness and lying were determined based on corroborating or conflicting data from reliable witness statements and forensic evidence. ...
Article
Full-text available
Confessions are an important evidentiary part of the legal process, and false confessions have been notable contributors to wrongful convictions. However, academic research in the psychology and law field primarily relies on student or volunteer samples in staged exercises, methodological features that lack ecological validity for replicating police interrogation or the pressures distinctive to high stakes crime investigations. Here, we provide an integrative review of research and data on false confessions during police interrogations with distinctions of key concepts, relevant case law pertaining to confessions including several U.S. Supreme Court decisions, updating the typology of false confessions, the quantification of false confessions, risk factors for false confessions, interrogation risk factors for false confessions, validity threats to false confessions research, and recommended directions for informing courts and the law.
Article
Full-text available
Prior research has shown that the rate of eye blinking is inversely related to cognitive load. In the present study, as in the prior work, some subjects were never told that their eyeblinks were being monitored. However, other subjects were deliberately informed. Each group of subjects performed both easy and difficult arithmetic. Awareness of being monitored reduced the blinking rate. However, the rate was still inversely related to difficulty of the task, even for the informed subjects. The methodological and theoretical implications of these findings were discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The role of social or communication skills in the ability to deceive was investigated. Thirty-eight student volunteers were administereda number of standardized social skill instruments. Subjects were then videotaped while giving short, persuasive messages. Messages were of three types: attitude-consistent (truthful), counterattitudinal (deceptive), and neutral. The videotaped messages were viewed by groups of judges who made ratings of the subject's believability for each message. Expressive and socially tactful subjects were more successful deceivers, whereas socially anxious subjects were less successful at the deception task. These results were attributed to an honest demeanor bias in the socially skilled subjects and a deceptive demeanor bias in the socially anxious subjects. Implications for future research on individual differences in deception skill were discussed.
Chapter
This chapter provides a theoretical analysis of why some lies fail while others succeed and presents a discussion of its implications for the design and interpretation of research on deception. New findings on the measurement of facial expression, body movement, voice and verbal behavior are reported which show that the combination of facial and vocal measures allows highly accurate identification of deceptive behavior. Other data show, however, that most people do not make use of the behavioral cues most relevant to detecting deception, but instead they depend upon those behaviors least valuable in the detection of deception.
Article
Previous research suggests that liars are not aware that they tend to decrease their movements during deception. Moreover, it is unclear how liars will behave if someone informs them about their behavioral rigidity during deception, and to what extent several processes (tension, attempted behavioral control, and cognitive effort) are associated with deception. In the present experiment, subjects were interviewed twice. During one interview, they told the truth, and during the other interview, they lied. In the information-present condition, before both interviews, subjects were told that deception is usually associated with a decrease in movements. In the information-absent condition, no information was given. The results revealed that whereas subjects believed that they increased their movements during deception, a decrease in movements, in fact, occurred. Provision of information about deceptive behavior had no effect. The results also showed that a decrease in movements was associated with attempted control and cognitive load processes, and occurred independently from the tension experienced by deceivers.
Article
Do people lie more to people they like than to people they dislike? Forty-eight female participants were experimentally induced to like or dislike an art student who had painted one of the paintings that participants liked and one that they disliked. According to their own self-reports and objective measures of the content of the discussions, participants favored the art students they liked with kind lies. When discussing the paintings with liked (compared to disliked) artists; participants conveyed more liking for all of the paintings, exaggerated their liking for the paintings they disliked that were the artists' own work, and were especially unlikely to say explicitly that they disliked those paintings. Participants favored the art students they liked not only by what they said or did not say about the artists' own work, but also by what they communicated about the other artists' work This was the case even though they liked the paintings equally in both conditions and were explicitly instructed to be polite to all the art students. Judges who watched videotapes of the discussions noticed many of the differences in the ways that participants communicated about paintings that were and were not the artists' own work, but they did not notice differences in communications with the liked versus the disliked artists even though these interactions did objectively differ.
Article
All deception studies published to date have been laboratory studies. In such studies people lied only for the sake of the experiment, consequently the stakes were usually low. Although research has shown that most spontaneous lies told in real life are trivial, such studies tell us little about lies where the stakes are high (such as police/suspect interviews). In Study 1, we discuss the behaviour of an actual suspect while he was interviewed by the police in a murder case. Although the man initially denied knowing and killing the victim, substantial evidence obtained by the police showed that he was lying. On the basis of this evidence, the man confessed to killing the victim and was later convicted for murder. To our knowledge there has been no other study published that has analysed the behaviour of a liar in such a high-stake realistic setting. The analysis revealed several cues to deception. In Study 2, we exposed 65 police officers to six fragments (three truthful and three deceptive) of the interview with the murderer and asked them to indicate after each fragment whether the man was lying or not. The findings revealed that the participants were better at detecting truths (70% accuracy) than lies (57% accuracy). We also found individual differences among observers, with those holding popular stereotypical views on deceptive behaviour, such as ‘liars look away’ and ‘liars fidget’ performing least effectively as lie catchers. Copyright © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.