Article

“Self-Focused Attention and Negative Affect: A Meta-Analysis

Valparaiso University (USA), Вальпараисо, Indiana, United States
Psychological Bulletin (Impact Factor: 14.76). 08/2002; 128(4):638-62. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT

This meta-analysis synthesized 226 effect sizes reflecting the relation between self-focused attention and negative affect (depression, anxiety, negative mood). The results demonstrate the multifaceted nature of self-focused attention and elucidate major controversies in the field. Overall, self-focus was associated with negative affect. Several moderators qualified this relationship. Self-focus and negative affect were more strongly related in clinical and female-dominated samples. Rumination yielded stronger effect sizes than nonruminative self-focus. Self-focus on positive self-aspects and following a positive event were related to lower negative affect. Most important, an interaction between foci of self-attention and form of negative affect was found: Private self-focus was more strongly associated with depression and generalized anxiety, whereas public self-focus was more strongly associated with social anxiety.

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Nilly Mor
  • Source
    • "One of the defining features of mindwandering is intrinsic spontaneous cognition (Smallwood & Schooler, 2015), which has been associated with increased attention on one's inner thoughts, emotions, and experience (Marchetti et al., 2012Marchetti et al., , 2014). Increased mind-wandering may lead to the excessive self-attention (Mor & Winquist, 2002), which may increase the risk of self-evaluation and can be related to negative emotion. Ultimately, frequent mindwandering may undermine people's self-values. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Mind-wandering is associated with adverse effects on life satisfaction. However, the potential mechanism that might explain the relationship between mind-wandering and life satisfaction is unclear. Furthermore, although attention problems are widespread among adolescents, few studies have investigated mind-wandering on a sample of youth. Thus, in this study, we first sought to validate the Chinese version of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire and then investigated the mediating role of self-esteem in the relationship between mind-wandering and life satisfaction. A total of 1331 Chinese adolescents across middle school and high school participated. The hypothesized model was tested using structural equation modeling. The results suggested that the Chinese version of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire is a suitable tool to measure the trait levels of mind-wandering for adolescents. Notably, supporting the study hypotheses, the negative association between mind-wandering and life satisfaction was mediated by self-esteem. Specifically, adolescents with a tendency to undertake mind-wandering reported low levels of self-esteem, which were then associated with decreased life satisfaction. The findings emphasized the role of youth self-esteem in explaining the relationship between mind-wandering and life satisfaction.
    Full-text · Article · Apr 2016 · Personality and Individual Differences
  • Source
    • "As an exception,Segerstrom et al. (2003, Study 2) identified that repetitive thinking can vary to the extent that it is interpersonally or intrapersonally focused and found that the effects of negative repetitive thinking on depression were most pronounced when cognition was self-rather than other-focused (Segerstrom et al., 2003, Study 3). This finding dovetails with the consistent relationship observed between self-focused attention and negative affect (Mor and Winquist, 2002) and indicates that self-focused negative thinking is particularly detrimental. Although self-focused thinking may have negative outcomes, research has yet to fully document the effects of more otherfocused thinking on adjustment. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Estimates suggest that up to half of waking life is spent daydreaming; that is, engaged in thought that is independent of, and unrelated to, one’s current task. Emerging research indicates that daydreams are predominately social suggesting that daydreams may serve socio-emotional functions. Here we explore the functional role of social daydreaming for socio-emotional adjustment during an important and stressful life transition (the transition to university) using experience-sampling with 103 participants over 28 days. Over time, social daydreams increased in their positive characteristics and positive emotional outcomes; specifically, participants reported that their daydreams made them feel more socially connected and less lonely, and that the content of their daydreams became less fanciful and involved higher quality relationships. These characteristics then predicted less loneliness at the end of the study, which, in turn was associated with greater social adaptation to university. Feelings of connection resulting from social daydreams were also associated with less emotional inertia in participants who reported being less socially adapted to university. Findings indicate that social daydreaming is functional for promoting socio-emotional adjustment to an important life event. We highlight the need to consider the social content of stimulus-independent cognitions, their characteristics, and patterns of change, to specify how social thoughts enable socio-emotional adaptation.
    Full-text · Article · Jan 2016 · Frontiers in Psychology
  • Source
    • "The majority of empirical findings are in line with these theoretical assumptions. Two meta-analyses found a medium to large cross-sectional association between various measures of self-focused attention and depression (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Mor & Winquist, 2002). Longitudinal studies revealed that rumination predicts the first onset of a depressive episode (Ito, Takenaka, Tomita, & Agari, 2006; Just & Alloy, 1997; Nolen- Hoeksema, 2000; Spasojevic & Alloy, 2001), and the extent of self-reported depression at follow up, both in inpatients with depression and (initially) non-depressed adults (e.g., Michl, McLaughlin, Shepherd, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2013; see also Aldao et al., 2010, for an overview). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Key practitioner message: Naturally occurring individual differences in first-person singular pronoun use provide an unobtrusive way to assess patients' automatic self-focused attention. Frequent use of first-person singular pronouns predicts an unfavourable course of depression. Self-focused language might offer innovative ways of tracking and targeting therapeutic change.
    Full-text · Article · Jan 2016 · Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy
Show more