ArticlePDF Available

Five-Factor Model of Personality and Job Satisfaction: A Meta-Analysis

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study reports results of a meta-analysis linking traits from the 5-factor model of personality to overall job satisfaction. Using the model as an organizing framework, 334 correlations from 163 independent samples were classified according to the model. The estimated true score correlations with job satisfaction were -.29 for Neuroticism, .25 for Extraversion, .02 for Openness to Experience, .17 for Agreeableness, and .26 for Conscientiousness. Results further indicated that only the relations of Neuroticism and Extraversion with job satisfaction generalized across studies. As a set, the Big Five traits had a multiple correlation of .41 with job satisfaction, indicating support for the validity of the dispositional source of job satisfaction when traits are organized according to the 5-factor model.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Five-Factor Model of Personality and Job Satisfaction: A Meta-Analysis
Timothy A. Judge
University of Florida Daniel Heller and Michael K. Mount
University of Iowa
This study reports results of a meta-analysis linking traits from the 5-factor model of personality to
overall job satisfaction. Using the model as an organizing framework, 334 correlations from 163
independent samples were classified according to the model. The estimated true score correlations with
job satisfaction were .29 for Neuroticism, .25 for Extraversion, .02 for Openness to Experience, .17 for
Agreeableness, and .26 for Conscientiousness. Results further indicated that only the relations of
Neuroticism and Extraversion with job satisfaction generalized across studies. As a set, the Big Five traits
had a multiple correlation of .41 with job satisfaction, indicating support for the validity of the
dispositional source of job satisfaction when traits are organized according to the 5-factor model.
Research on the dispositional source of job satisfaction has had
a spotty history in job satisfaction research. The personological
basis of job satisfaction was considered in the earliest treatments of
job satisfaction. Hoppock (1935), for example, noted a strong
correlation between workers’ emotional adjustment and their lev-
els of job satisfaction. Similarly, Fisher and Hanna (1931) con-
cluded that a large part of dissatisfaction resulted from emotional
maladjustment. With some noteworthy exceptions (P. C. Smith,
1955; Weitz, 1952), these early considerations of the dispositional
source of job satisfaction lay dormant until the 1980s, when a
series of provocative studies (Arvey, Bouchard, Segal, & Abra-
ham, 1989; Staw, Bell, & Clausen, 1986; Staw & Ross, 1985) led
to renewed interest in the relationship. In the past 15 years, an
expanding literature has accumulated, giving general support to the
argument that job satisfaction is, in part, dispositionally based
(House, Shane, & Herold, 1996). Despite this widespread accep-
tance, a broad array of traits has been investigated, and there has
been little integration in the literature. As Spector (1997) noted,
“Although many traits have been shown to correlate significantly
with job satisfaction, most research with personality has done little
more than demonstrate relations without offering much theoretical
explanation” (p. 51).
One factor that has impeded theoretical explanations of the
dispositional source of job satisfaction is the lack of a framework
describing the structure and nature of personality. Thousands of
traits have been invented in the history of personality research, and
scores of traits have been studied in relation to job satisfaction. As
Arvey, Carter, and Buerkley (1991) commented, “There is confu-
sion regarding which person variables should be examined. A
formidable array of person variables have been discussed as pos-
sible determinants of job satisfaction in the research literature” (p.
377). When specific traits have been selected for inclusion in
studies of employee attitudes, it generally has been in a piecemeal
fashion. Advances in personality research, however, provide the
potential for assimilation and integration.
One typology that has been in this research literature is the
positive affectivity (PA)-negative affectivity (NA) taxonomy of
affective temperament (D. Watson, 2000). Research by Watson,
Tellegen, and colleagues suggests that affective disposition is
composed of two facets: PA and NA. High-PA individuals are
predisposed to experience positive emotionality (e.g., joy, excite-
ment, enthusiasm), whereas high-NA individuals are predisposed
to experience negative emotions (e.g., guilt, anger, fear; D.
Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). In a meta-analysis of the
relation of affectivity to job satisfaction, Connolly and Viswesva-
ran (2000) reported true score correlations of PA and NA with job
satisfaction of .49 (k15) and .33 (k27), respectively.
Although the PA-NA typology has proven to be quite useful in
investigating the dispositional source of job satisfaction, several
limitations exist. First, given the higher correlations of PA with job
satisfaction, it is surprising that more research has been focused on
NA, in many cases to the exclusion of PA (e.g., Levin & Stokes,
1989; Necowitz & Roznowski, 1994). Second, the issue of the
independence of PA and NA continues to be debated in the
literature (Russell & Carroll, 1999a, 1999b; D. Watson & Telle-
gen, 1999). Some argue that it is inappropriate to treat PA and NA
as separate concepts: that the traits represent opposite ends of
single bipolar construct or that a circumplex is required to take the
relationships among the concepts into account (Carroll, Yik, Rus-
sell, & Barrett, 1999). Others suggest that PA-NA may assess, at
least in part, current levels of happiness, affect experienced, or life
satisfaction (Judge & Locke, 1993). Finally, the PA-NA taxonomy
includes only two traits. Other traits may exist that are theoretically
and empirically relevant to job satisfaction.
Within the last 20 years, consensus has emerged that a five-
factor model of personality, often termed the Big Five (Goldberg,
1990), can be used to describe the most salient aspects of person-
ality. The five-factor structure has generalized across measures,
cultures, and sources of ratings (McCrae & John, 1992). Although
Timothy A. Judge, Department of Management, University of Florida;
Daniel Heller, Department of Psychology, University of Iowa; Michael K.
Mount, Department of Management and Organizations, University of
Iowa.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Timothy
A. Judge, Department of Management, Warrington College of Business,
University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida 32611. E-mail: tjudge@ufl.edu
Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2002, Vol. 87, No. 3, 530–541 0021-9010/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.87.3.530
530
the five-factor model has been researched in many areas of
industrial-organizational psychology, most notably with respect to
job performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991), the relationship of the
five-factor model to job satisfaction is much less studied. A num-
ber of studies have investigated relations between an isolated facet
of the five-factor model (especially Neuroticism) and job satisfac-
tion. However, there is a virtual dearth of research that has linked
the complete taxonomy to job satisfaction. Furthermore, unlike job
performance, where more than a half-dozen meta-analyses have
been conducted using the Big Five framework, we are aware of no
prior meta-analysis of the relationship of the Big Five traits to job
satisfaction. This is unfortunate because the five-factor model may
provide needed integration to this literature.
Accordingly, the purpose of this study is to investigate the
relationship between the five-factor model of personality and job
satisfaction. In conducting a meta-analysis of the relationship
between the Big Five traits and job satisfaction, we use the Barrick
and Mount (1991) meta-analysis of the relationship of the Big Five
traits to job performance to guide our investigation. Before de-
scribing the meta-analysis, we discuss potential linkages between
the Big Five traits and job satisfaction.
Relationships of the Big Five Traits With Job Satisfaction
Neuroticism
Because of their essentially negative nature, neurotic individuals
experience more negative life events than other individuals (Mag-
nus, Diener, Fujita, & Pavot, 1993) in part, because they select
themselves into situations that foster negative affect (Emmons,
Diener, & Larsen, 1985). To the extent that such situations occur
on or with respect to the job, they would lead to diminished levels
of job satisfaction. Neuroticism has been described as the primary
source of NA, and the link between NA and job satisfaction was
documented in Connolly and Viswesvarans (2000) meta-analysis.
Extraversion
Whereas Neuroticism is related to the experience of negative
life events, extraverts are predisposed to experience positive emo-
tions (Costa & McCrae, 1992), and positive emotionality likely
generalizes to job satisfaction, as demonstrated by Connolly and
Viswesvarans (2000) meta-analysis of PAjob satisfaction rela-
tionships. Evidence also indicates that extraverts have more friends
and spend more time in social situations than do introverts and,
because of their social facility, are likely to find interpersonal
interactions (such as those that occur at work) more rewarding (D.
Watson & Clark, 1997).
Openness to Experience
Openness to Experience is related to scientific and artistic
creativity (Feist, 1998), divergent thinking, low religiosity, and
political liberalism (see McCrae, 1996, for a review). None of
these psychological states seem to be closely related to job satis-
faction. Furthermore, DeNeve and Cooper (1998) noted that
Openness to Experience is a double-edged swordthat predis-
poses individuals to feel both the good and the bad more deeply
(p. 199), rendering its directional influence on affective reactions
like subjective well-being or job satisfaction unclear.
Agreeableness
McCrae and Costa (1991) argued that Agreeableness should be
related to happiness because agreeable individuals have greater
motivation to achieve interpersonal intimacy, which should lead to
greater levels of well-being. Indeed, they found that Agreeableness
was positively related to life satisfaction, although at a relatively
low level (mean r.16). Assuming these same communal moti-
vations exist on the job, then the same process should operate with
respect to job satisfaction. Organ and Lingl (1995) apparently
agreed, commenting that Agreeableness involves getting along
with others in pleasant, satisfying relationships(p. 340).
Conscientiousness
Organ and Lingl (1995) argued that Conscientiousness should
be related to job satisfaction because it represents a general work-
involvement tendency and thus leads to a greater likelihood of
obtaining satisfying work rewards, both formal (e.g., pay, promo-
tions) and informal (e.g., recognition, respect, feelings of personal
accomplishment). Indirectly, the subjective well-being literature
also suggests a positive relationship between Conscientiousness
and job satisfaction (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998).
Moderators of the PersonalityJob
Satisfaction Relationship
Although there is good reason to believe that four of the Big
Five traits are related to job satisfaction across studies (the excep-
tion being Openness), several possible moderators of the relation-
ship exist. First, because the personalityjob satisfaction relation-
ship might be stronger in cross-sectional studies, we investigated
whether the relationship varied according to cross-sectional versus
longitudinal research designs. Second, with few exceptions (e.g.,
Eysencks measures of Neuroticism and Extraversion; Eysenck &
Eysenck, 1968), there were no direct measures of the Big Five
traits before the revision of the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa
& McCrae, 1992). Accordingly, we analyzed whether the
personalityjob satisfaction correlation varied depending on
whether a direct or indirect measure (using Barrick & Mounts,
1991, coding scheme; see Method section) of the Big Five traits
was used. Finally, because the nature of job satisfaction measures
has been argued to affect their relationships with other variables
(Brief & Roberson, 1989), on an exploratory basis, we also esti-
mated the magnitude of the personalityjob satisfaction correlation
by job satisfaction measure.
Method
Rules for Inclusion in the Meta-Analysis
To identify all possible studies of the relationship between the Big Five
traits and job satisfaction, we searched the PsycINFO database (1887
2000) for studies (articles, book chapters, dissertations, and unpublished
reports) that referenced personality and job satisfaction. In addition to
searching for keywords such as personality, Big Five, Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, and Neuroti-
cism, we searched for a list of additional traits and measures that were
included in Barrick and Mounts (1991) review. These efforts resulted in
the identification of 1,277 abstracts (including doctoral dissertations). Of
these 1,277 abstracts, 737 were obtained by searching for the keywords
531
PERSONALITY AND JOB SATISFACTION
personality and job satisfaction.An additional 540 records were obtained
by using names of personality inventories, common specific traits, and the
Big Five traits in combination with job satisfaction. In reviewing these
abstracts, we eliminated most because (a) they did not appear to measure
any discernible personality trait, (b) they assessed a trait that was not
classifiable in terms of the five-factor model, or (c) it was clear that they
did not report data (e.g., as was the case with most book chapters). We also
used several raw data sets that were available to the authors.
For the remaining 430 journal articles and doctoral dissertations, we
examined each study to determine whether it contained the necessary
information. Eighty-two articles and 53 doctoral dissertations met these
criteria. Several studies contained multiple independent samples. Thus, in
all, 163 independent samples and 334 correlations were included in the
analyses. Several exclusionary rules were established. Reasons for exclud-
ing studies at this level fell into several categories. First, many studies
failed to report the data necessary to obtain a correlation (e.g., studies that
reported percentages or proportions or means with no standard deviations,
studies that provided only a narrative summary of the results or reported
other measures of association that could not be converted to correlations;
e.g., analysis of variance results). Second, we excluded studies that in-
cluded traits that did not fall within Barrick and Mounts (1991) classifi-
cation of existing measures into the Big Five traits. Specifically, we
excluded studies wherein the personality measure was a combination of
more than one trait or could not clearly be identified as a personality trait
subsumed within the five-factor model. Thus, such traits as field depen-
dence, Machiavellianism, Type A, or typologies such as the MyersBriggs
Type Indicator (MBTI) types were not included (studies that reported
individual MBTI traits, e.g., I/E, rather than types, e.g., ISTP, were includ-
ed). Similarly, traits such as hostility or impulsivity that have been treated
inconsistently by Big Five researchers also were excluded.
Personality measures were classified according to the coding procedure
developed and used by Barrick and Mount (1991). Specifically, in their
meta-analysis, they classified personality measures based on an examina-
tion of the measures and decisions made by six expert judges (see pp. 89
of Barrick & Mount, 1991, for a detailed description of the classification
procedures). For example, the Dominance and Sociability scales from the
California Psychological Inventory (see Gough, 1988) were classified by
the experts as measures of Extraversion, and the Warm and Suspicious
(reverse-scored) scales from the 16 P-F were classified as measures of
Agreeableness. We followed their classification closely, with the following
exceptions: (a) Obviously, direct measures of the Big Five traits, such as
those using the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992) or
other direct measures of the Big Five traits, were included
1
; (b) two studies
using measures of dispositional optimism were included because research
suggests such measures assess Neuroticism (T. W. Smith, Pope, Rhode-
walt, & Poulton, 1989); (c) four studies using measures of trait anxiety
were included because, again, research indicates that these measures assess
Neuroticism (Zuckerman, Joireman, Kraft, & Kuhlman, 1999); (d) three
studies were included because the traits assessed appeared to closely
correspond to Openness: rigidity (rigid is an adjective descriptor of low
openness; Digman, 1989, p. 202), flexibility (measures of flexibility cor-
relate significantly with Openness to Experience; Costa & McCrae, 1992,
p. 47), and adaptation-innovation (adaptive [Digman, 1989, p. 203] and
innovative [Goldberg, 1992, p. 35] are trait markers of Openness to
Experience).
2
In terms of job satisfaction, consistent with the recommendations of
meta-analytic researchers (Matt & Cook, 1994), we defined the population
to which we wished to generalize a priori as consisting of normal employed
adults. Accordingly, satisfaction in primary studies needed to be measured
at the individual (as opposed to group) level, and satisfaction needed to
occur in a natural job setting. Thus, consistent with Barrick and Mount
(1991), studies involving military or laboratory participants were excluded.
Satisfaction needed to be assessed with global or overall measures (general
perceptions of ones job). If satisfaction was measured with reference to
specific facets of the job situation (as is the case with the Job Descriptive
Index; P. C. Smith, Kendall, & Hulin, 1969), we computed an equally
weighted composite of overall satisfaction using the Spearman-Brown
prophecy formula (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).
Meta-Analytic Procedures
We used the meta-analytic procedures of Hunter and Schmidt (1990) to
correct observed correlations for sampling error and unreliability in mea-
sures of personality and job satisfaction. Correlations were corrected indi-
vidually. When authors of original studies reported an overall internal
consistency reliability for personality or job satisfaction, we used this value
to correct the observed correlation for attenuation. When reliabilities for
personality or job satisfaction measures were not reported, we used the
mean reliability for job satisfaction or the relevant Big Five trait for those
studies that did report a reliability estimate.
3
Finally, three original studies
used single-item measures of job satisfaction; consequently, no internal
consistency reliabilities were reported. In these cases, we used meta-
analytically derived estimates of the reliability of single-item measures of
job satisfaction (Wanous, Reichers, & Hudy, 1997). Hence, we assumed a
reliability of .68 for single-item satisfaction scales.
In addition to reporting estimates of the mean true score correlations, it
is also important in meta-analysis to describe variability in the correlations.
Accordingly, we report 80% credibility intervals and 90% confidence
intervals around the estimated population correlations. Although some
meta-analyses report only confidence intervals (e.g., Ernst Kossek &
Ozeki, 1998) whereas others report only credibility intervals (e.g., Vinchur,
Schippmann, Switzer, & Roth, 1998), it is important to report both because
each tells us different things about the nature of the correlations. Confi-
dence intervals provide an estimate of the variability around the estimated
mean correlation; a 90% confidence interval excluding zero indicates that
we can be 95% confident that the average true correlation is nonzero (5%
of average correlations would lie beyond the upper limit of the distribu-
tion). Credibility intervals provide an estimate of the variability of indi-
vidual correlations across studies; an 80% credibility interval excluding
zero indicates that 90% of the individual correlations in the meta-analysis
excluded zero (for positive correlations, 10% are zero or less and 10% lie
at or beyond the upper bound of the interval). Thus, confidence intervals
estimate variability in the mean correlation, whereas credibility intervals
estimate variability in the individual correlations across the studies.
The moderators were determined by examining the articles and coding
the necessary information. For most of the moderators, this information
was easily obtained (e.g., longitudinal vs. cross-sectional design). In terms
of measures, most articles reported the measure of personality and job
satisfaction. Thirty-eight percent of correlations involved a direct(ex-
plicitly labeled) measure of the Big Five traits. This percentage varied
1
Barrick and Mount (1991) included few direct measures of the Big
Five traits because, at that time, few were available. The situation has
changed appreciably since then, although still only a minority of the
correlations in our study used direct measures of the Big Five traits.
2
Although some scholars have argued that PA and NA can be integrated
into the five-factor model, such that PA is synonymous with Extraversion
and NA is synonymous with Neuroticism (Brief, 1998; D. Watson, 2000),
we did not include PA and NA in our analysis for several reasons. First,
although there is evidence supporting Briefs and Watsons assertions
regarding PA and NA, direct tests are lacking. Second, because other
reviews have discussed these variables extensively, we believe they war-
rant separate consideration from the Big Five traits included here.
3
The mean reliability for measures of job satisfaction was .83. The mean
reliabilities for measures of each of the Big Five traits were as follows:
Neuroticism .82; Extraversion .72; Openness to Experience .67;
Agreeableness .66; Conscientiousness .71.
532 JUDGE, HELLER, AND MOUNT
across the traits, from 52% direct measures for Extraversion to 20% direct
measures for Conscientiousness. This is to be expected because no direct
measures of Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness existed be-
fore the revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992),
which is why Barrick and Mount (1991) included only indirect measures in
their meta-analysis and why we followed their coding scheme. Measures of
job satisfaction were classified into the following categories: the Brayfield
and Rothe (1951) measure (17%), the Hoppock (1935) Job Satisfaction
Blank (8%), the Job Descriptive Index (P. C. Smith et al., 1969) (13%),
Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (Weiss, Dawis, England, & Lofquist,
1967) (17%), other validated measures (21%), and ad hoc (previously
unvalidated) measures (24%).
Results
Results of the meta-analyses relating the Big Five traits to job
satisfaction are provided in Table 1. Neuroticism (
⫽⫺.29) was
the strongest correlate of job satisfaction, followed closely by
Conscientiousness (
.26) and Extraversion (
.25). Both the
confidence intervals and credibility intervals excluded zero for two
traits: Neuroticism and Extraversion. For two other traitsCon-
scientiousness and Agreeablenessthe confidence intervals ex-
cluded zero, indicating that we can be confident that these average
correlations are distinguishable from zero. However, the 80%
credibility interval included zero for these traits, suggesting that
the relationship of Conscientiousness and Agreeableness with job
satisfaction does not fully generalize across studies (e.g., in about
10% of studies, the relationship between Conscientiousness and
job satisfaction was zero or negative). Finally, Openness to Expe-
rience showed a weak correlation with job satisfaction (
.02)
that was indistinguishable from zero.
Although not reported in Table 1, results also revealed that
sampling error and other statistical artifacts explained only a small
percentage of the variability in the correlations across studies.
Across the five traits, only 16.1% of the variability in the corre-
lations was explained by sampling error and other statistical arti-
facts (26.5% of the variability was explained using the alternative
weighting procedure described later).
In the meta-analyses, the sample sizes varied dramatically,
from 5 to 2,900. As Huffcutt, Roth, and McDaniel (1996) noted, a
concern with weighting studies by the sample size (Nweighting) in
meta-analysis is that a handful of studies may dominate the anal-
ysis. Accordingly, they developed an alternative weighting proce-
dure that assigned a weight of 1 to a study with a the sample size
was 75 or less, 2 if the sample size was between 75 and 200, and 3
if the sample size was 200 or more. We used this weighting
procedure to determine whether it would yield a different result
from N-weighted analyses. This alternative weighting procedure
did change some of the results, although not dramatically. The
results were as follows: Neuroticism,
⫽⫺.31; Extraversion,
.25; Openness to Experience,
.02; Agreeableness,
.19;
Conscientiousness,
.28. Thus, the Huffcutt et al. (1996)
weighting procedure produced similar, although slightly higher,
correlations.
As Brief (1998) noted, it is important to investigate the dispo-
sitional correlates of job satisfaction in an integrated framework.
Accordingly, we sought to determine the multivariate relationship
of the set of Big Five traits to job satisfaction. Using Hunters
(1992) regression program, we regressed job satisfaction on the
Big Five traits.
4
To form the correlation matrix that served as input
into the program, we used the meta-analytic estimates of the
relationship between the Big Five traits and job satisfaction in
Table 1 and Ones, Viswesvaran, and Reisss (1996) meta-analytic
estimate of the intercorrelations among the Big Five traits. The
sample size we used for the regressions was equal to the average
sample size of all studies in the analysis (Viswesvaran & Ones,
1995): 280. Given that the N-weighted analysis produced results
that were somewhat different from Huffcutt et al.s (1996) weight-
ing procedure, we also estimated the regression using the meta-
analytic results produced from their weighting scheme.
The regression results are provided in Table 2. As is shown,
regardless of which weighting method was used, three Big Five
traitsExtraversion, Conscientiousness, and Neuroticismwere
significant predictors of job satisfaction. Although these three
traits were significant regardless of the method of weighting used
to estimate the correlations, the results were slightly stronger for
the analysis using Huffcutt et al.s (1996) weighting procedure.
Perhaps the most meaningful statistic was the strong and signifi-
cant multiple correlation (R.41 in the N-weighted analysis; R
.43 in the analysis using the Huffcutt et al. weighting scheme)
between the five-factor model and job satisfaction.
4
The regression model implicitly assumes that the causal direction of
the personalityjob satisfaction relationship is from personality to job
satisfaction. Given that roughly 50% of the variance in the Big Five traits
is inherited (Jang, Livesley, & Vernon, 1996), and that any heritability in
job satisfaction is likely due to personality (Judge, 1992), this assumption
seems reasonable.
Table 1
Meta-Analysis of the Relationship of Personality to Job Satisfaction
Trait
Average 80% CV 90% CI
kN r
SD
Lower Upper Lower Upper
Neuroticism 92 24,527 .24 .29 .16 .50 .08 .33 .26
Extraversion 75 20,184 .19 .25 .15 .06 .45 .22 .29
Openness to Experience 50 15,196 .01 .02 .21 .26 .29 .05 .08
Agreeableness 38 11,856 .13 .17 .16 .03 .37 .12 .22
Conscientiousness 79 21,719 .20 .26 .22 .02 .55 .21 .31
Note. k number of correlations; Ncombined sample size;
estimated true score correlation; SD
standard deviation of true score correlation; CV credibility interval; CI confidence interval.
533
PERSONALITY AND JOB SATISFACTION
Moderators of the Personality–Job Satisfaction
Relationship
The moderator results by job satisfaction measure are provided
in the Appendix. As is shown in the Appendix, across the traits,
personalityjob satisfaction correlations tended to be higher for
several measures, most notably the Brayfield and Rothe (1951)
measure. It also is noteworthy that the personalityjob satisfaction
correlations generally were not lower for ad hoc, or previously
unvalidated, measures of job satisfaction. Table 3 provides the
results of the two methodological moderators. Direct and indirect
measures of personality did not differ much in their relation with
job satisfaction; there was a slight tendency for job satisfaction to
correlate more strongly with indirect measures (significant only in
the case of Conscientiousness). Similarly, and somewhat surpris-
ingly, personalityjob satisfaction correlations did not differ much
by cross-sectional versus longitudinal research designs; only in the
case of Agreeableness was the difference significant.
5
Discussion
Results of this meta-analytic review suggest that the five-factor
model is a fruitful basis for examining the dispositional source of
job satisfaction. In particular, the traits of Neuroticism, Extraver-
sion, and Conscientiousness displayed moderate correlations with
job satisfaction. We are aware of no published primary studies
focusing on the relationship of the five-factor model to job atti-
tudes, much less a meta-analytic review of the relationship of the
five-factor model to job satisfaction. Thus, these results fill an
important void in the literature.
Neuroticism emerged as the strongest and most consistent cor-
relate of job satisfaction. It also is the Big Five trait that has been
studied most often in relation to job satisfaction. The validity of
Neuroticism came as no surprise to us. We also were not surprised
that Extraversion displayed nonzero relationships with job satis-
faction across studies. Emotional stability (low neuroticism) and
Extraversion are key aspects of the happy personality(DeNeve
& Cooper, 1998); one would expect that the factors that cause
emotionally stable and extraverted individuals to be happy in life
would also lead them to be happy in their jobs. As Tokar, Fischer,
and Subich (1998) noted in their qualitative review, Greater job
satisfaction is related to lower neuroticism and its variants, as well
as to higher extraversion and related traits(p. 144). Our findings
provide quantified support to this conclusion.
Although the positive effects of Conscientiousness in terms of
job performance have been clearly demonstrated (Barrick &
Mount, 1991), the potential positive effects of conscientiousness in
terms of job satisfaction have been virtually ignored in the litera-
ture (see Organ & Lingl, 1995). Our results suggest that this is an
oversight. Of the Big Five traits, Conscientiousness displayed the
second strongest correlation with job satisfaction. However, it is
important to note that the 80% credibility interval for Conscien-
tiousness (just) included zero. Of the 79 Conscientiousness corre-
lations, 9 were negative, although it should be noted that 7 of
these 9 correlations ranged from .12 to .02. Given this, and that
the average sample size for these nine correlations was relatively
small (median of 60), sampling error may explain these results.
6
Furthermore, the average correlation for Conscientiousness was
distinguishable from zero, as was the effect in the regression
analyses.
Finally, the other two traitsAgreeableness and Openness to
Experiencedisplayed relatively weak correlations with job sat-
isfaction. Although the mean Agreeableness correlation was non-
zero, the correlations were highly variable across studies; more
than one of five Agreeableness correlations were negative, and a
5
On an exploratory basis, we also investigated with the personalityjob
satisfaction relations varied by the Holland (1985) occupational types. For
example, Conscientiousness may be related to job satisfaction most
strongly in conventional and realistic occupations (those that are practical
and require orderliness and those that require conforming behavior, respec-
tively), Openness may be most related to satisfaction in investigative
occupations (those requiring trouble shooting or creation of new knowl-
edge), and Agreeableness and Extraversion may be most related to satis-
faction in social occupations. In coding occupations based on the primary
occupational type in Hollands (1985) RIASEC typology (only studies
based on a single occupation were coded), however, we found limited
support for these expected relations. On the one hand, Conscientiousness
was strongly related to job satisfaction in Realistic and Conventional jobs.
However, the other expected moderating effects were not supported, and
some unexpected results were observed. One explanation for these findings
is that the primary RIASEC codes, at the study level, are too gross to fully
capture vocational type.
6
One of the 16 (6.25%) correlations for studies using direct measures of
conscientiousness was negative (and another was zero), whereas 8 of the 63
(12.7%) correlations based on indirect measures of conscientiousness were
negative. If the .00 correlation noted previously had been .01, the
proportions would be nearly the same (12.5% vs. 12.7%).
Table 2
Regression of Job Satisfaction on Big Five Personality Traits
Trait
N-weighted correlations Huffcutt et al. (1996) weighting
/RSE T
/RSE T
Neuroticism .20 .06 3.38* .21 .06 3.68*
Extraversion .21 .06 3.80* .21 .06 3.80*
Openness to Experience .04 .06 0.71 .04 .06 0.75
Agreeableness .04 .06 0.61 .05 .06 0.86
Conscientiousness .20 .06 3.40* .21 .06 3.67*
Multiple R.41 .06 7.70* .43 .06 8.27*
Note.
standardized regression coefficient; TTvalue (
/SE).
*p.01.
534 JUDGE, HELLER, AND MOUNT
roughly equal number were between .00 and .10. Openness to
Experience displayed a small and highly variable correlation with
job satisfaction. Indeed, 24 of the Openness to Experience corre-
lations were negative and 26 were positive.
Given the obvious connections between job and life satisfaction
(Spector, 1997), it is worthwhile to compare our findings with
DeNeve and Coopers (1998) meta-analytic findings regarding the
correlations between the Big Five traits and life satisfaction (see
their Table 7, p. 210). Accordingly, we took their findings and
corrected them for measurement error.
7
Results of this comparison
are provided in Figure 1. As Figure 1, the results are quite similar.
Indeed, the rank-order correlation is perfect (Pearson r.94). The
only area of divergence is with respect to Openness to Experience
which correlates more strongly with life satisfaction than job
satisfaction. It is unclear why this would be the case, although we
note that in DeNeve and Coopers meta-analysis, Openness to
Experience displayed a weaker correlation with happiness (aver-
age uncorrected r.06) compared with life satisfaction (average
uncorrected r.14). In general, however, the results are quite
similar, suggesting that the cognitive, affective, and behavioral
factors that lead to personalityjob satisfaction relations may be
similar to those that lead to personalitylife satisfaction relations.
Given the strength of the relation between job and life satisfaction
(Tait, Padgett, & Baldwin, 1989), the parallelism in the results
makes sense.
The moderator analyses, especially those by measure of job
satisfaction, did reveal variability in personalityjob satisfaction
correlations. However, most of the moderators did not follow
expectations or did not reveal significant differences. In general,
the moderator results do not appear to undermine the validity in the
personalitysatisfaction correlations across studies. One potential
moderator we were not able to explore is whether personalityjob
satisfaction correlations varied by job satisfaction facet. Too few
studies reported facet correlations to make such an analysis prac-
ticable here. Future research investigating personalityjob satis-
faction relations by facet might reveal interesting insights and
show that the traits display differential associations with job sat-
isfaction facets. For example, following the principle of correspon-
dence (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977), it may be that traits narrower than
the Big Five better predict job satisfaction facets. Such an ap-
proach would comport with those who advocate a focus on traits
more specific than the Big Five (e.g., Schneider & Hough, 1995)
and would provide a more complicated, but perhaps also more
complete, understanding of personalitysatisfaction relations.
Although the results linking the Big Five traits to job satisfac-
tion are impressive, other frameworks could explain the disposi-
tional source of job satisfaction. For example, Connolly and
Viswesvarans (2000) results indicate that PA and NA display
moderately strong correlations with job satisfaction. Indeed, the
correlations involving PA are stronger than those reported in this
study. However, two factors argue in favor of the five-factor
model. First, researchers have suggested that PA represents Extra-
version and NA Neuroticism in the five-factor model, thus sub-
suming PA and NA within the five-factor model (e.g., Brief, 1998;
D. Watson & Clark, 1997). Because the five-factor model contains
an additional trait that is relevant to job satisfaction (Conscien-
tiousness) than does the PA-NA typology, it may be a more useful
framework. At the very least, PA-NA would need to be supple-
mented with Conscientiousness if the maximum prediction of job
satisfaction is to be obtained. Second, PA and NA are quasidispo-
sitional in that they are assessments of mood or affective traits
(D. Watson, 2000, p. 15), are less stable than other dispositional
measures (Judge & Bretz, 1993), and may to some degree be
confounded with life satisfaction (Judge, Locke, Durham, &
Kluger, 1998). Nevertheless, given the empirical validity of both
frameworks, and their similarity, future integrative research is
needed.
Another framework that may also explain the personological
basis of job satisfaction is Judge, Locke, and colleaguesconcept
of core self-evaluations. According to Judge and colleagues, core
7
As noted earlier, DeNeve and Cooper (1998) did not correct their
meta-analytic results for unreliability. As Schmidt and Hunter (1996)
indicated, it is critical that estimates be corrected for unreliability if one is
to avoid distortion in findings, meta-analytic or otherwise. Accordingly, as
in our meta-analysis, we corrected DeNeve and Coopers meta-analytic
estimates for unreliability in personality and life satisfaction. Reliability of
the Big Five traits was estimated using the results from a 2000 meta-
analysis (Viswesvaran & Ones, 2000). The average internal consistencies
of the Big Five traits in this meta-analysis were as follows: Neuroticism
.78; Extraversion .78; Openness .73; Agreeableness .75; Consci-
entiousness .78. To arrive at a reliability for life satisfaction, we
searched the PsycINFO literature for articles using what we believe to be
the most widely used life satisfaction measure: the Satisfaction With Life
Scale (Pavot & Diener, 1993). This search resulted in 32 correlations
contained in 27 articles. The average internal consistency reliability,
weighted by sample size, was .80. The studies used in this meta-analysis
are not listed in this article, but are available on request.
Table 3
Methodological Moderators of the PersonalityJob Satisfaction Relationship
Trait
Measure of Big Five trait Research design
Direct (D) Indirect (I) Cross-sectional (CS) Longitudinal (L)
Neuroticism .27 .31 .29 .32
Extraversion .24 .27 .26 .16
Openness to Experience .02 .01 .02 .09
Agreeableness .14 .21 .18
L
.23
CS
Conscientiousness .17
I
.31
D
.27 .18
Note. Table entries are average correlation between Big Five traits and job satisfaction, corrected for
measurement error. Subscripts indicate significant differences in correlations. Across the cells, the number of
correlations ranged from k2(n221) to k87 (n23,888).
535
PERSONALITY AND JOB SATISFACTION
self-evaluations is a broad personality trait that is manifested in
self-esteem, locus of control, generalized self-efficacy, and (low)
neuroticism. Judge et al. (1998) showed that these four traits
loaded on the same underlying construct and that the trait was a
significant predictor of job satisfaction, even controlling for PA
and NA. At the same time, it is not clear how core self-evaluations
fits into the five-factor framework. In Judge et al.s (1998) frame-
work, Neuroticism is subsumed within the core self-evaluation
framework. However, Neuroticism itself is a broad construct and
one of the most venerable in psychology. Thus, it is entirely
possible that the core self-evaluations traits should be cumulated
together, as argued by Judge et al., but under the concept of
Neuroticism rather than core self-evaluations. This is an issue for
future research.
Drawing from the tripartite (cognitive, affective, and behav-
ioral) categorization of attitudes (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993), the Big
Five traits may influence job satisfaction through each of these
processes. Cognitively, these traits may influence how individuals
interpret characteristics of their jobs, as is the case when individ-
uals with positive core self-evaluations interpret intrinsic job char-
acteristics more positively, even controlling for actual job com-
plexity (Judge, Bono, & Locke, 2000). Affectively, these traits
might influence job satisfaction through their effect on mood
(Costa & McCrae, 1980) or mood at work (see Brief, 1998).
Finally, behaviorally, employees who are emotionally stable, ex-
traverted, and conscientious may be happier at work because they
are more likely to achieve satisfying results at work. Part of this
effect may operate through job performance, such that conscien-
tious employees perform better and are more satisfied with their
jobs because of the intrinsic and extrinsic rewards that high per-
formance provides. In part, it may operate through situation selec-
tion, such that extraverted employees are more likely to spend time
in situations that make people happy, such as in social interactions
(Magnus et al., 1993). Given the links between personality and job
performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991) and personality and job
satisfaction presented herein, perhaps the time has come for a
framework that takes the linkages among personality, job perfor-
mance, and job satisfaction into account. Such models may involve
more proximal predictors, such as integrity, which is related to the
five-factor model (see Sackett & Wanek, 1996).
In summary, results of the current quantitative review indicate
that Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Conscientiousness display ap-
preciable correlations with job satisfaction, and that the five-factor
model is a fruitful basis to examine the dispositional source of job
satisfaction. In view of these results, future studies should attempt
to integrate alternative frameworks of the dispositional source of
job satisfaction and to model the psychological processes that may
explain the relationships of the traits with job satisfaction.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analysis.
*Ahmad, S., & Razzack, B. (1983). A study of mental health and job
satisfaction of industrial workers. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 10, 239244.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical
analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84,
888918.
*Allison, N. L. (1984). The relationship between personality characteris-
tics and job satisfaction of selected computer programmers. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri.
*Anand, S. P. (1977). School teachers: Job satisfaction vs. extraversion and
neuroticism. Indian Educational Review, 12, 6878.
Arvey, R. D., Bouchard, T. J., Segal, N. L., & Abraham, L. M. (1989). Job
satisfaction: Environmental and genetic components. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 74, 187192.
Figure 1. Comparison of validity of Big Five traits for job satisfaction and life satisfaction. (Life satisfaction
correlations are corrected for unreliability based on DeNeve and Coopers, 1998, uncorrected correlations: [see
Footnote 7 for explanation of corrections]. For ease of presentation, Neuroticism is labeled by its opposite pole,
Emotional Stability.) Open bars represent job satisfaction; solid bars represent life satisfaction.
536 JUDGE, HELLER, AND MOUNT
Arvey, R. D., Carter, G. W., & Buerkley, D. K. (1991). Job satisfaction:
Dispositional and situational influences. International Review of Indus-
trial and Organizational Psychology, 6, 359383.
*Atteberry, M. G. (1977). The relationship between emotional stability and
job satisfaction of elementary school principals. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Arizona State University.
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimen-
sions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44,
126.
*Beehr, T. A. (1975). Role ambiguity as a role stress: Some moderating
and intervening variables. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
of Michigan.
*Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2000). Responses to transformational
leadership: Are some followers immune? Unpublished working paper,
University of Iowa.
*Bose, S., Roy, K. P., & Das Gupta, S. C. (1969). An attitude study of
multi-accident omnibus drivers. Indian Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 6, 312.
*Boswell, W. R. Boudreau, J. W., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Effects of
personality on executive career success in the United States and Europe.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 5381.
*Brayfield, A. H., & Marsh, M. M. (1957). Aptitudes, interests, and
personality characteristics of farmers. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 41, 98103.
Brayfield, A. H., & Rothe, H. F. (1951). An index of job satisfaction.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, 307311.
Brief, A. P. (1998). Attitudes in and around organizations. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Brief, A. P., & Roberson, L. (1989). Job attitude organization: An explor-
atory study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 717727.
*Bristow, C. L. (1985). Achievement motivation, job characteristics, and
job satisfaction: An exploratory study. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, University of Iowa.
*Burroughs, S. M., & Eby, L. T. (1998). Psychological sense of commu-
nity at work: A measurement system and explanatory framework. Jour-
nal of Community Psychology, 26, 509532.
*Callahan, S. D., & Kidd, A. H. (1986). Relationship between job satis-
faction and self-esteem in women. Psychological Reports, 59, 663668.
*Calvi, C. J. (1993). Career implications of personal styles for job roles
and functions and job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Missouri.
*Capin, P. H. (1986). The effects of personality and type of job on
satisfaction with work. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Louisville.
Carroll, J. M., Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). On the
psychometric principles of affect. Review of General Psychology, 3,
1422.
*Cartwright, L. K. (1978). Career satisfaction and role harmony in a
sample of young women physicians. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 12,
184196.
*Chusmir, L. H. (1981). Sex differences in the motivation of managers: A
look at need achievement, need affiliation, and need power. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Miami.
*Chusmir, L. H., & Hood, J. A. (1986). Relationship between Type A
behavior pattern and motivational needs. Psychological Reports, 58,
783794.
*Coddington, T. M. (1999). The role of expectations, personality, and
spirituality in the tenure of Catholic campus ministers: A three year
study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas.
Connolly, J. J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2000). The role of affectivity in job
satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 29, 265281.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and
neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668678.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality
Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor (NEO-FFI) Inventory pro-
fessional manual. Odessa, FL: PAR.
*Cottington, E. M. (1983). Occupational stress, psychosocial modifiers,
and blood pressure in a blue-collar occupation. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.
*Day, D. V., Bedeian, A. G., & Conte, J. M. (1998). Personality as
predictor of work-related outcomes: Test of a mediated latent structural
model. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 20682088.
DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-
analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 124, 197229.
Digman, J. M. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability,
and utility. Journal of Personality, 57, 195214.
*Dreher, G. F. (1980). Individual needs as correlates of satisfaction and
involvement with a modified Scanlon Plan company. Journal of Voca-
tional Behavior, 17, 8994.
*Dwyer, D. J. (1990). The effect of dispositional influences on job percep-
tions, work attitudes, and nonattendance behavior. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation, University of Nebraska.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Forth
Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
*Elovainio, M., Kivimaeki, M., Steen, N., & Kalliomaeki-Levanto, T.
(2000). Organizational and individual factors affecting mental health and
job satisfaction: A multilevel analysis of job control and personality.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 269277.
Emmons, R. A., Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Choice of situations
and congruence models of interactionism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 6, 693702.
Ernst Kossek, E., & Ozeki, C. (1998). Work-family conflict, policies, and
the job-life satisfaction relationship: A review and directions for orga-
nizational behavior-human resources research. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 83, 139149.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Manual for the Eysenck
Personality Inventory. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Test-
ing Service.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic
creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2, 290309.
*Fellers, R. B. (1974). Relationships between career satisfaction and
personality type for employed dieticians. Unpublished doctoral disser-
tation, University of Florida.
Fisher, V. E., & Hanna, J. V. (1931). The dissatisfied worker. New York:
Macmillan.
*Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (1999). A model of work frustration-aggression.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 915931.
*Francis, L. J., & Robbins, M. (1999). The relationship between person-
ality and satisfaction/dissatisfaction with ministry among female stipen-
diary Anglican clergy in the UK. Pastoral Psychology, 47, 439444.
*Furnham, A., & Zacherl, M. (1986). Personality and job satisfaction.
Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 453459.
*Garskof, M. S. (1985). Motivating teachers with nonfinancial incentives:
The relationships of compensatory-time jobs and the need to achieve to
the job satisfaction of high school teachers in New York City. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, New York University.
*Gilleland, R. L. (1974). The relationship between the WAIS and MMPI
subscale scores and work adjustment outcomes in adult blind and
partially sighted persons. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State
University.
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative description of personality: The
Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 59, 12161229.
537
PERSONALITY AND JOB SATISFACTION
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five
factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 2642.
*Gorini, H. M. (1991). An individuals correspondence preference as
related to work-related complaints, neuroticism, and job satisfaction.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois.
Gough, H. G. (1988). Manual for the California Psychological Inventory.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
*Grise, J. (1973). Interactional aspects of workersachievement needs with
trait anxiety levels and their relationships to job complexity, job tension,
job performance, job satisfaction and workerspotential. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Western Ontario.
*Gross, R. H. (1978). Moderators of the job performance-job satisfaction
relationship for research scientists. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Tennessee.
*Guerrieri, S. I. (1980). Teacher personality characteristics in selected
open and non-open elementary schools. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, University of Arizona.
*Guha, T. N. (1965). Personality factors and job satisfaction among shoe
factory workers. Indian Psychological Review, 2, 5964.
*Harbin, J. L. (1980). Managerial motivation: Some correlates of job
performance and job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Arkansas.
*Hart, A. L. (1989). Operating room nursespersonality profiles as re-
lated to job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana
University.
*Hart, P. M. (1999). Predicting employee life satisfaction: A coherent
model of personality, work, and nonwork experiences, and domain
satisfactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 564584.
*Hartley, M. P. (1975). The relationship of locus of control and need
achievement to job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rut-
gers University.
*Heron, A. (1955). Personality and occupational adjustment: A cross-
validation study. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 9, 1520.
*Higashi, C. J. (1987). A correlational study of the characteristic values
and needs with work related factors among registered nurses. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, University of the Pacific.
*Hodapp, V., Neuser, K. W., & Weyer, G. (1988). Job stress, emotion, and
work environment: Toward a causal model. Personality and Individual
Differences, 9, 851859.
Holland, J. L. (1985). The self-directed search: Professional manual.
Odessa, FL: PAR.
*Hoppock, R. (1935). Job satisfaction. New York: Harper.
House, R. J., Shane, S. A., & Herold, D. M. (1996). Rumors of the death
of dispositional research are vastly exaggerated. Academy of Manage-
ment Review, 21, 203224.
Huffcutt, A. I., Roth, P. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996). A meta-analytic
investigation of cognitive ability in employment interview evaluations:
Moderating characteristics and implications for incremental validity.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 459473.
Hunter, J. E. (1992). REGRESS: A multiple regression program in
BASICA. Users manual, Department of Psychology, Michigan State
University.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
*Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (in press). Understanding the dynamic relation-
ship between personality, mood, and job satisfaction: A field experience
sampling study. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes.
*Indiresan, J. (1975). Multivariate analysis of factors affecting the job
satisfaction of engineering teachers. Indian Journal of Psychometry and
Education, 6, 1627.
*James, L. R., & Jones, A. P. (1980). Perceived job characteristics and job
satisfaction: An examination of reciprocal causation. Personnel Psychol-
ogy, 33, 97135.
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the Big
Five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. Journal of
Personality, 64, 577591.
*Janman, K., Jones, J. G., Payne, R. L., & Rick, J. T. (1988). Clustering
individuals as a way of dealing with multiple predictors in occupational
stress research. Behavioral Medicine, 14, 1729.
*Janssen, O., de Vries, T., & Cozijnsen, A. J. (1998). Voicing by adapting
and innovating employees: An empirical study on how personality and
environment interact to affect voice behavior. Human Relations, 51,
945967.
*Jenkins, S. R. (1987). Need for achievement and womens careers over 14
years: Evidence for occupational structure effects. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 53, 922932.
*Jex, S. M., Spector, P. E., Gudanowski, D. M., & Newman, R. A. (1991).
Relations between exercise and employee responses to work stressors: A
summary of two studies. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6,
425443.
*Joyce, W. F., Slocum, J. W., & von Glinow, M. A. (1982). Person
Situation interaction: Competing models of fit. Journal of Occupational
Behaviour, 3, 265280.
Judge, T. A. (1992). The dispositional perspective in human resources
research. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Manage-
ment, 10, 3172.
*Judge, T. A., & Bauer, T. N. (1997). Five-factor model of personality and
employee attitudes. Unpublished manuscript.
*Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (1999). Job attitudes of employees at Hawkeye
Foodsystems. Unpublished technical report, University of Iowa.
*Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., & Locke, E. A. (2000). Personality and job
satisfaction: The mediating role of job characteristics. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 85, 237249.
Judge, T. A., & Bretz, R. D. (1993). Report on an alternative measure of
affective disposition. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53,
10951104.
*Judge, T. A., Bretz, R. D., Jr., Kennedy, D. J., & Bloom, M. C. (1996).
People as sculptors versus sculpture: Test of a dispositional model of
career success. Unpublished manuscript.
*Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999).
The Big Five personality traits, general mental ability, and career suc-
cess across the life span. Personnel Psychology, 52, 621652.
Judge, T. A., & Locke, E. A. (1993). Effect of dysfunctional thought
processes on subjective well-being and job satisfaction. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 78, 475490.
*Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., Durham, C. C., & Kluger, A. N. (1998).
Dispositional effects on job and life satisfaction: The role of core
evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 1734.
*Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Pucik, V., & Welbourne, T. M. (1999).
Managerial coping with organizational change: A dispositional perspec-
tive. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 107122.
*Jyothi, P. (1983). A study of achievement motivation in relation to job
satisfaction among high and low achieving working women. Managerial
Psychology, 4, 8493.
*Keene, D. L. (1983). Personality factors related to career choice and job
satisfaction among female elementary and secondary teacher education
alumni. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas.
*Kemmerer, B. E. (1991). The moderating effect of personality differences
on job stress: A longitudinal investigation. Unpublished doctoral disser-
tation, University of Nebraska.
*Kennedy, D. J. (1999). On the road again: An investigation of the
situational and intentional antecedents of job relocation decisions in the
service sector. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.
*Khoury, R. M., & Khoury, D. C. (1981). Professionalization of police: A
question of altruism. Psychological Reports, 49, 896898.
*Kirkcaldy, B., Thome, E., & Thomas, W. (1989). Job satisfaction amongst
538 JUDGE, HELLER, AND MOUNT
psychosocial workers. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 191
196.
*Lair, A. J. (1986). Kansas special education administrators: Personality
factors and demographics as indicators of job satisfaction. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Kansas State University.
*Landsbergis, P. A., Schnall, P. L., Deitz, D., Friedman, R., & Pickering,
T. (1992). The patterning of psychological attributes and distress by job
strainand social support in a sample of working men. Journal of
Behavioral Medicine, 15, 379403.
*Larocque, E. R. (1976). A comparison of need strength, need satisfaction,
and personality factors as they relate to job satisfaction in selected
occupational levels. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgia State
University.
*LaRussa, G. W. (1981). A personality study predicting the effectiveness
and satisfaction of Catholic priests in pastoral ministry. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Levin, I., & Stokes, J. P. (1989). Dispositional approach to job satisfaction:
Role of negative affectivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 752
758.
*Little, W. G. (1972). Relationships between certain personality charac-
teristics of post-secondary distributive education personnel and job
satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.
*Long, B. C. (1993). Coping strategies of male managers: A prospective
analysis of predictors of psychosomatic symptoms and job satisfaction.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42, 184199.
*Lucas, R. T. (1991). Personality and situational differences between
more- and less-satisfied ministers in the American Baptist Churches of
Indiana, and those who have left pastoral ministry. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation, Indiana University.
*Lusch, R. F., & Serpkenci, R. R. (1990). Personal differences, job tension,
job outcomes, and store performance: A study of retail store managers.
Journal of Marketing, 54, 85101.
Magnus, K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion and
neuroticism as predictors of objective life events: A longitudinal anal-
ysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 10461053.
*Manlove, E. E. (1993). Multiple predictors of burnout in child care
workers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State
University.
*Mannheim, B., Baruch, Y., & Tal, J. (1997). Alternative models for
antecedents and outcomes of work centrality and job satisfaction of
high-tech personnel. Human Relations, 50, 15371562.
Matt, G. E., & Cook, T. D. (1994). Threats to the validity of research
syntheses. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of
research synthesis (pp. 503520). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
*Mauldin, P. B. (1973). Selected personality characteristics and job sat-
isfaction in experienced elementary and secondary teachers. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, University of Alabama.
McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness.
Psychological Bulletin, 120, 323337.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1991). Adding liebe und arbeit: The full
five-factor model and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 17, 227232.
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor
model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 2, 175215.
*Medcof, J. W., & Wegener, J. G. (1992). Work technology and the needs
for achievement and nurturance among nurses. Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 13, 413423.
*Melamed, S., Meir, E. I., & Samson, A. (1995). The benefits of
personality-leisure congruence: Evidence and implications. Journal of
Leisure Research, 27, 2540.
*Miller, R. L., Griffin, M. A., & Hart, P. M. (1999). Personality and
organizational health: The role of conscientiousness. Work & Stress, 13,
719.
*Mohan, J., & Bali, S. (1988). A study of job-satisfaction of doctors in
relation to their personality, values and self-esteem. Journal of Person-
ality and Clinical Studies, 4, 6368.
*Molitor, D. D. (1998). An examination of the effects of personality and job
satisfaction on multiple non-workrole organizational behaviors. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Iowa State University.
*Monsour, D. K. (1987). Student teacher effectiveness and job satisfaction:
An analysis of personality and cognitive variables in relation to perfor-
mance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University.
*Mount, M. K., Harter, J. K., Barrick, M. R., & Colbert, A. (2000, August).
Does job satisfaction moderate the relationship between conscientious-
ness and job performance? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Academy of Management, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
*Moyle, P., & Parkes, K. (1999). The effects of transition stress: A
relocation study. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 625646.
*Muthayya, B. C., & Gnanakannan, I. (1973). Developmental personnel: A
psycho-social study across three states in India. Hyderabad, India:
National Institute of Community Development.
*Nagarathnamma, B. (1988). Job satisfaction as a function of locus of
control and neuroticism. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied
Psychology, 14, 1215.
Necowitz, L. B., & Roznowski, M. (1994): Negative affectivity and job
satisfaction: Cognitive processes underlying the relationship and effects
on employee behaviors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45, 270294.
*Nelson, A., Cooper, C. L., & Jackson, P. R. (1995). Uncertainty amidst
change: The impact of privatization on employee job satisfaction and
well-being. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
ogy, 68, 5771.
*Newsome, E. T. (1976). A study of relationships between job satisfaction
and personality needs of college student volunteers at Indiana State
University. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana State University.
*Nonis, S. A. (1992). Factors associated with salespersonsuse of influ-
ence tactics and their outcomes: An exploratory study. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of North Texas.
*Olson, H., Jr. (1967). Relationships between certain personality charac-
teristics of distributive education teacher-coordinators and job satisfac-
tion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.
Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., & Reiss, A. D. (1996). Role of social
desirability in personality testing for personnel selection: The red her-
ring. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 660679.
Organ, D. W., & Lingl, A. (1995). Personality, satisfaction, and organiza-
tional citizenship behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 135, 339350.
*Orpen, C. (1985). The effects of need for achievement and need for
independence on the relationship between perceived job attributes and
managerial satisfaction and performance. International Journal of Psy-
chology, 20, 207219.
*Pandey, A. K., & Prakash, P. (1984). A study on relationship between
achievement motivation and satisfaction of industrial employees. Indian
Psychologist, 3, 104110.
*Parasuraman, S., & Alutto, J. A. (1984). Sources and outcomes of stress
in organizational settings: Toward the development of a structural
model. Academy of Management Journal, 27, 330350.
*Parker, B., & Chusmir, L. H. (1991). Motivation needs and their rela-
tionship to life success. Human Relations, 44, 13011312.
*Parker, L. J. (1993). The relationship between personality factors and job
satisfaction in public school band directors. Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation, University of Kansas.
*Pavia, E. S. (1985). Differences between new and established industrial
workers: An interactional model. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Texas Christian University.
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the Satisfaction With Life
Scale. Psychological Assessment, 5, 164172.
*Perone, M., DeWaard, R. J., & Baron, A. (1979). Satisfaction with real
and simulated jobs in relation to personality variables and drug use.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 660668.
539
PERSONALITY AND JOB SATISFACTION
*Peterson, M. F. (1983). Co-workers and hospital staffs work attitudes:
Individual difference moderators. Nursing Research, 32, 115121.
*Pinder, C. C. (1977). Multiple predictors of post-transfer satisfaction: The
role of urban factors. Personnel Psychology, 30, 543556.
*Porwal, N. K. (1987). A comparative study of the personality of the job
satisfied and the job dissatisfied teachers as measured by factors A, C, E,
H, L, and Q
4
of 16-PF. Asian Journal of Psychology & Education, 19,
2731.
*Porwal, N. K., & Sharma, S. C. (1985). Job satisfaction and emotional
stability. Indian Psychological Review, 28, 14.
*Richard, L. R. (1994). Psychological type and job satisfaction among
practicing lawyers in the United States. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, Temple University.
*Roberts, S. W. (1987). The effects of anxiety and selected personality
characteristics on job satisfaction among Utah paramedics. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Brigham Young University.
*Romero, J. D. (1975). Personality factors and job satisfaction among
rural health care employees. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer-
sity of New Mexico.
*Runco, M. A. (1995). The creativity and job satisfaction of artists in
organizations. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 13, 3945.
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999a). On the bipolarity of positive and
negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 330.
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999b). The phoenix of bipolarity: Reply
to Watson and Tellegen (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 611617.
Sackett, P. R., & Wanek, J. E. (1996). New developments in the use of
measures of honesty, integrity, conscientiousness, dependability, trust-
worthiness, and reliability for personnel selection. Personnel Psychol-
ogy, 49, 787829.
*Schaubroeck, J., Ganster, D. C., & Kemmerer, B. (1996). Does trait affect
promote job attitude stability? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17,
191196.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1996). Measurement error in psychological
research: Lessons from 26 research scenarios. Psychological Methods, 1,
199223.
Schneider, R. J., & Hough, L. M. (1995). Personality and industrial/
organizational psychology. International Review of Industrial and Or-
ganizational Psychology, 10, 77129.
*Schwartz, G. R. (1984). Determinants of job satisfaction for exempt and
non-exempt employees. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Illinois Insti-
tute of Technology.
*Sharma, S., & Moudgil, A. C. (1986). Job satisfaction vs. n-achievement.
Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13, 203208.
*Sheinfeld, D. Z. (1983). Civil liberties climate in work organizations and
its relationship with job and work attitudes. Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation, Stevens Institute of Technology.
*Shrivastava, G. P. (1978). A study of some determinants of job satisfac-
tion among underground and ground colliery workers. Indian Journal of
Industrial Relations, 14, 131140.
*Slocum, J. W., Miller, J. D., & Misshank, M. J. (1970). Needs, environ-
mental work satisfaction and job performance. Training and Develop-
ment Journal, 24, 1215.
*Smith, C. A. (1983). Job satisfaction, workplace environment, and per-
sonality traits as sources of influence on helping behaviors: A social
exchange perspective. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana
University.
*Smith, C. A., Organ, D. W., & Near, J. P. (1983). Organizational citi-
zenship behavior: Its nature and antecedents. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 68, 653663.
Smith, P. C. (1955). The prediction of individual differences in suscepti-
bility to industrial monotony. Journal of Applied Psychology, 39, 322
329.
Smith, P. C., Kendall, L. M., & Hulin, C. L. (1969). The measurement of
satisfaction in work and retirement. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Smith, T. W., Pope, M. K., Rhodewalt, F., & Poulton, J. L. (1989).
Optimism, neuroticism, coping, and symptom reports: An alternative
interpretation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 56, 640648.
*Sondhi, M., & Bhardwaj, G. (1987). The psychology of hierarchy: A
macro level analysis in large scale paper industry. Indian Psychological
Review, 32, 19.
*Soyer, R. B., Rovenpor, J. L., & Kopelman, R. E. (1999). Narcissism and
achievement motivation as related to three facets of the sales role:
Attraction, satisfaction and performance. Journal of Business & Psy-
chology, 14, 285304.
Spector, P. E. (1997). Job satisfaction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
*Srivastava, S. K. (1985). A comparative study of job satisfaction among
private and public sector employees with special reference to achieve-
ment motivation. Indian Journal of Applied Psychology, 22, 1015.
*Srivastava, S. K. (1986). To measure the level of job satisfaction in
technical and non-technical employees with special reference to differ-
ent personality characteristics in public sectors. Indian Journal of Ap-
plied Psychology, 23, 15.
Staw, B. M., Bell, N. E., & Clausen, J. A. (1986). The dispositional
approach to job attitudes: A lifetime longitudinal test. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 31, 5677.
Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1985). Stability in the midst of change: A
dispositional approach to job attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 70, 469480.
*Steers, R. M. (1975). Effects of need for achievement on the job perfor-
mance-job attitude relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60,
678682.
*Stein, J. A., Smith, G. M., Guy, S. M., & Bentler, P. M. (1993).
Consequences of adolescent drug use on young adult job behavior and
job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 463474.
*Sterns, L., Alexander, R. A., Barrett, G. V., & Dambrot, F. H. (1983). The
relationship of extraversion and neuroticism with job preferences and
job satisfaction for clerical employees. Journal of Occupational Psy-
chology, 56, 145153.
*Stone, E. F., Mowday, R. T., & Porter, L. W. (1977). Higher order need
strengths as moderators of the job scope-job satisfaction relationship.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 466471.
*Struempfer, D. J. W. (1997). The relation between religious motivation
and work-related variables amongst agricultural workers. South African
Journal of Psychology, 27, 134142.
Tait, M., Padgett, M. Y., & Baldwin, T. T. (1989). Job and life satisfaction:
A reevaluation of the strength of the relationship and gender effects as
a function of the date of the study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74,
502507.
*Takahashi, K. (1999). [Job satisfaction of medical sales representatives in
Japan]. Unpublished raw data.
*Tang, T. L. P., & Ibrahim, A. H. S. (1998). Antecedents of organizational
citizenship behavior revisited: Public personnel in the United States and
in the Middle East. Public Personnel Management, 27, 529550.
*Tejagupta, Y. (1987). The relationship between early childhood teacher
personality and job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Illi-
nois State University.
*Terry, D. J., Neilsen, M., & Perchard, L. (1993). Effects of work stress on
psychological well-being and job satisfaction: The stress-buffering role
of social support. Australian Journal of Psychology, 45, 168175.
*Thoresen, C. J. (2000). Antecedents and consequences of coping with
setbacks at work: A theory-driven framework. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Iowa.
Tokar, D. M., Fischer, A. R., & Subich, L. M. (1998). Personality and
vocational behavior: A selected review of the literature, 19931997.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 53, 115153.
*Tokar, D. M., & Subich, L. M. (1997). Relative contributions of congru-
540 JUDGE, HELLER, AND MOUNT
ence and personality dimensions to job satisfaction. Journal of Voca-
tional Behavior, 50, 482491.
*Tregaskis, W. F. (1987). Job satisfaction and health. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation, Ball State University.
Vinchur, A. J., Schippmann, J. S., Switzer, F. S. III, & Roth, P. L. (1998).
A meta-analytic review of predictors of job performance for salespeople.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 586597.
Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing: Combining psy-
chometric meta-analysis and structural equations modeling. Personnel
Psychology, 48, 865885.
Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (2000). Measurement error in Big Five
factorspersonality assessment: Reliability generalization across studies
and measures. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60, 224
235.
*Wallace-Barnhill, G. L. (1982). Comparison of intensive care unit nurses,
non-intensive care unit nurses and non-nursing personnel with regard to
personality variables and job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral disser-
tation, University of Maryland.
*Wanberg, C. R., Carmichael, H. D., & Downey, R. G. (1999). Satisfaction
at last job and unemployment: A new look. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 20, 121131.
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job
satisfaction: How good are single-item measures? Journal of Applied
Psychology, 82, 247252.
Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional
core. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of
personality psychology (pp. 767793). San Diego: Academic Press.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
10631070.
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1999). Issues in dimensional structure of
affectEffects of descriptors, measurement error, and response formats:
Comment on Russell and Carroll (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125,
601610.
*Watson, G. T. (1976). The relationship of selected personal characteris-
tics of counselors to job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Texas A&M University.
*Weiss, D. J. (1968). A study of the relationship of participation in
decision-making selected personality variables and job satisfaction of
the educational research and development council elementary school
principals. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.
Weiss, D. J. Dawis, R. V. England, G. W., & Lofquist, L. H. (1967).
Manual for the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire. Minneapolis:
Industrial Relations Center, University of Minnesota.
Weitz, J. (1952). A neglected concept in the study of job satisfaction.
Personnel Psychology, 5, 201205.
*Wong, C., Tam, K., Fung, M., & Wan, K. (1993). Differences between
odd and even number of response scale: Some empirical evidence.
Chinese Journal of Psychology, 35, 7586.
*Woodworth, D. G. (1964). Job-satisfaction and personality: A study of
research scientists. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
California, Berkeley.
*Yasin, M. M. (1996). Entrepreneurial effectiveness and achievement in
Arab culture: New evidence to rekindle interest in an old predictor.
Journal of Business Research, 35, 6977.
*Yasin, M. M. (1987). Assessing managerial, technical, and academic
motivation in the Arab culture: The relationships of needs for achieve-
ment, affiliation and power with effectiveness. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Clemson University.
Zuckerman, M., Joireman, J., Kraft, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (1999).
Where do motivational and emotional traits fit within three factor
models of personality? Personality and Individual Differences, 26,
487504.
Received November 14, 2000
Revision received September 25, 2001
Accepted September 28, 2001
Appendix
PersonalityJob Satisfaction Correlations by Measure of Job Satisfaction
Big Five trait
Brayfield &
Rothe measure
(1)
Hoppock Job
Satisfaction
Blank (2) Job Descriptive
Index (3)
Minnesota
Satisfaction
Questionnaire
(4)
Other
previously
validated
(5)
Ad hoc
measure
(6)
Neuroticism .36
4,5
.56 .30 .26
1
.26
1
.30
Extraversion .37
4,5,6
.33 .24 .23
1
.21
1
.25
1
Openness to Experience .01 .02 .06 .13
5
.01
4
.02
Agreeableness .31
2,5,6
.13
1
.22 .19 .15
1
.02
1
Conscientiousness .38
3,5,6
.45 .20
1
.30 .19
1
.23
1
Note. Table entries are meta-analytic estimates of the average true score correlation (
) between Big Five traits and job satisfaction, corrected for
measurement error. Subscripts indicate significant differences in correlations. Across the cells, the number of correlations ranged from k4(n441)
to k26 (n4,959).
541
PERSONALITY AND JOB SATISFACTION
... Support for the role of affect as an antecedent of work attitudes comes from studies that measure both the attitudes and the affective dispositions of individuals. Some meta-analysis studies (Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002;Thoresen, Kaplan, Barsky, Warren, & De-Chermont, 2003) found that job satisfaction and organizational commitment are correlated positively with extraversion and negatively correlated with neuroticism. In summary we can say that individuals high in positive affect (extraverts) tend to be more committed and to express more favourable attitudes to their work, and individuals high in negative affect (neurotics) tend to be less committed and to express less favourable attitudes to their work (Gelade, Dobson, & Gilbert, 2006). ...
... Our finding for the third hypothesis portrayed significant positive association of employees' job satisfaction and organizational commitment with extroversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness personality traits (see Table 2 & 3). Many previous researches also describe significant positive association of job satisfaction with extroversion (Judge et al., 2002;Lee, Trauth, & Farwell, 1995;Skibba, 2002;Thoresen, et al., 2003;Watson & Clark, 1997), with Openness to experience (Judge et al., 2002;Tziner, Brodman, Vardi, & Waismal-Manor, 2008), with agreeableness (Skibba, 2002;Tziner, et al., 2008), and with conscientiousness (Jiang, Klein, & Ballou, 2001;Judge et al., 2002;Skibba, 2002). Similarly Some previous researcher also found positive association of organizational commitment with extroversion (Erdheim et al., 2006;Gelade, Dobson, & Gilbert, 2006;Meyer & Allen, 1991;Thoresen, et al., 2003), with openness to experience (Erdheim et al., 2006;Tziner, et al., 2008), with agreeableness (Erdheim et al., 2006;Tziner, et al., 2008), and with conscientiousness (Gelade, Dobson, & Gilbert, 2006;Erdheim et al., 2006;Tziner, et al., 2008) personality traits. ...
... Our finding for the third hypothesis portrayed significant positive association of employees' job satisfaction and organizational commitment with extroversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness personality traits (see Table 2 & 3). Many previous researches also describe significant positive association of job satisfaction with extroversion (Judge et al., 2002;Lee, Trauth, & Farwell, 1995;Skibba, 2002;Thoresen, et al., 2003;Watson & Clark, 1997), with Openness to experience (Judge et al., 2002;Tziner, Brodman, Vardi, & Waismal-Manor, 2008), with agreeableness (Skibba, 2002;Tziner, et al., 2008), and with conscientiousness (Jiang, Klein, & Ballou, 2001;Judge et al., 2002;Skibba, 2002). Similarly Some previous researcher also found positive association of organizational commitment with extroversion (Erdheim et al., 2006;Gelade, Dobson, & Gilbert, 2006;Meyer & Allen, 1991;Thoresen, et al., 2003), with openness to experience (Erdheim et al., 2006;Tziner, et al., 2008), with agreeableness (Erdheim et al., 2006;Tziner, et al., 2008), and with conscientiousness (Gelade, Dobson, & Gilbert, 2006;Erdheim et al., 2006;Tziner, et al., 2008) personality traits. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study had two main objectives; first, to discover the relationships between job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and personality traits among employees of AW (Air Weapon Complex), Second, to find out the age, salary, marital status, and education differences on job satisfaction and organizational commitment of employees. In present study three instrument; Job Satisfaction Scale JSS (Macdonald & Maclntyre, 1997), Organizational Commitment Questionnaire (OCQ) (Mowday, Steer, & Porter, 197), and Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) (Rentfrow, Gosling & Swann, 2003); were administered to a sample of (N=100) along with demographic sheet. Results of the study revealed significant positive correlation between job satisfaction and organizational commitment of employees; significant negative association of neuroticism with job satisfaction and organizational commitment; extroversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness showed significant positive correlation with job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Findings of the study also showed significant age, education, salary, and marital status differences on job satisfaction and organizational commitment as all these demographic variables were positive correlates of job satisfaction and organizational commitment.
... Agreeableness, another key trait, is also important for leadership. Leaders who are empathetic, cooperative, and supportive are more likely to foster teamwork, trust, and collaboration within their teams, leading to better performance and job satisfaction among employees (Judge et al., 2002). However, leadership effectiveness is not determined solely by personality traits; it is also influenced by situational factors, such as the organizational culture, team dynamics, and the specific challenges a leader faces. ...
... Extraverted are generally more assertive, sociable, and people influencer and inspirer of their organizations. Hence, their leadership effectivity improves (Judge et al., 2002). A noted difference in the effective leadership of a group coupled with its large effect size focuses attention on an important aspect of extraversion in effective organizational leadership. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study deals with how personality traits, including conscientiousness, extraversion, and emotional stability, play a role in job performance, leadership effectiveness, and career success. The methodology has been the carefully designed survey of employees from various sectors in Punjab, Pakistan, including professionals at different stages of their careers and leaders. All types of professionals were well represented through drawing the population from education, healthcare business, and government sectors. The stratified random sampling method was employed to generate a sample size that would ensure enough representation from each of the subgroups, which are divided by the job role, industry, and experience. The gathered data were subjected to an analysis using various statistical tools, such as regression analysis, t-tests, and ANOVA. Conscientiousness in relation to job performance was examined via regression analysis. Leadership efficacy between extraverts and non-extraverts was measured using the t-tests. ANOVA was employed to examine the differences in career success among individuals exhibiting varying levels of emotional stability. Ethical considerations were meticulously adhered to, guaranteeing participant consent, privacy, and the right to withdraw at any stage of the study. The findings indicated that conscientiousness served as a positive predictor of job performance, while extraversion was correlated with enhanced leadership effectiveness, and emotional stability was significantly related to career success. These findings stress the relevance of personality traits in shaping professional results. The results indicate that personality assessments should be integrated into recruitment, leadership development, and career counseling practices.
... Recently the researchers are more interested to explore the relationship between personality traits and both job satisfaction and career satisfaction. In a meta-analysis conducted by Judge, Heller, and Mount (2002) found that Extraversion and Neuroticism were consistent predictors of job satisfaction and also explained that 17% variance in job satisfaction can be explained by personality traits. Scheider and Dachler (1978) found that satisfaction with a job unusually remains stable over time, which made them believe that people's personality which leads them to the satisfaction with their job as compared to other variables. ...
... Personality traits are an important construct that has been used to predict job performance. Judge et al., (2002) found that neurotic individuals show diminished level of job satisfaction; extroverts are likely to experience a high-level of job satisfaction; conscientiousness has positive correlation with job satisfaction; agreeable individuals have a close relationship with job satisfaction; while openness individuals have no correlation with job performance. Organ and Lingl (1995) also found that conscientiousness and Agreeable personality traits may lead to higher levels of job satisfaction. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of the current study is to discover the relationship between rating on annual progress reports and job satisfaction; between personality traits and job satisfaction among Banks employees; and to explore age, gender salary, marital status, and education differences on job satisfaction. Data were collected from 500 employees of five major banks of Pakistan (Allaied Bank, Alfalah Bank, Al-Habeeb Bank, Al-Islamic Bank, and National Bank, taking annual progress reports, personal information sheets, and scores on Job Satisfaction Scale (Macdonald & Maclntyre, 1997) and Ten Item Personality Inventory (Rentfrow, Gosling & Swann, 2003) of employees. Rating on annual progress reports has significant positive correlation with job satisfaction; Neuroticism has significant negative while extroversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness have significant positive correlation with job satisfaction; age, education, salary, and marital status have significant positive correlation with job satisfaction; and women are more satisfied with their jobs than men.
... In the social sphere, agreeableness correlates positively with partner satisfaction and prosocial behaviours (e.g., Malouff et al., 2010;Thielmann et al., 2020). In the workplace, personality predicts job performance (He et al., 2019;Judge et al., 2013), satisfaction (Judge et al., 2002), absenteeism (Løset & von Soest, 2022), and burnout (Alarcon et al., 2009). All these publications, and plenty others, evidence the growing interest in non-cognitive measurement over the years. ...
Article
Full-text available
Forced-choice (FC) questionnaires have gained scientific interest over the last decades. However, the inclusion of unequally keyed item pairs in FC questionnaires remains a subject of debate, as there is evidence supporting both their usage and avoidance. Designing unequally keyed pairs may be more difficult when considering social desirability, as they might allow the identification of ideal responses. Nevertheless, they may enhance the reliability and the potential for normative interpretation of scores. To empirically investigate this topic, data were collected from 1,125 undergraduate Psychology students who completed a personality item pool measuring the Big Five personality traits in Likert-type format and two FC questionnaires (with and without unequally keyed pairs). These questionnaires were compared in terms of reliability, convergent and criterion validity, and ipsativity of the scores, along with insights on the construction process. While constructing questionnaires with unequally keyed blocks presented challenges in matching items on their social desirability, the differences observed in terms of reliability, validity, or ipsativity were sporadic and lacked systematic patterns. This suggests that neither questionnaire format exhibited a clear superiority. Given these results, it is recommended using only equally keyed blocks to minimize potential validity issues associated with response biases.
... The Big Five Personality Model (Digman, 1989;McCrae & Costa, 1987) has gained widespread acceptance in the scientific community and has contributed to a resurgence of personality research in organizational behavior and I/O psychology. Indeed, this model has been used to study relationships between personality and variables of interest to organizations such as leadership (Bono & Judge, 2004), job satisfaction (Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002), job performance (Barrick, Mount, & Judge, 2001) and turnover (Zimmerman, 2008). Researchers have generally supported the notion that "personality is an important determinant of individual behavior in the workplace". ...
... Evaluation objectivity concerns the numerical and categorical evaluation of respondents' answering behavior according to established rules. 20 This is fully given in the BFI-10, since the allocation rules of the values on the items are clearly defined and do not allow any room for interpretation. ...
Article
Full-text available
In terms of management understanding, employee behavior and attitudes have a serious impact on the potential of human resources, in terms of activity and efficiency, of an organization. It is of utmost importance to recognize people before hand and determine a relationship between their personalities and organizational behavior in terms of organizational development. This paper is primarily concerned with the assumption that the findings of psychological studies and neuroscience on human motivation can have a positive impact on the change processes of a company. The aim is to highlight the psychological findings and methods used in personnel development measures, taking psychological studies and neuroscientific research into account.
... Individuals higher in Agreeableness may be more likely to evoke positive and favorable responses from patients and from the work environment. Furthermore, greater job satisfaction and accomplishment have been reported in individuals scoring higher on this trait (Divinakumar et al., 2019;Judge et al., 2002). Weak and not significant associations were found between Consciousness and Extraversion traits and burnout dimensions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research is limited on the relationship between accountants' personality traits and their work-related attitudes, behaviors, and self-perception. Therefore, this study examines the effects of accountants' personality traits on job performance and mental health. The study examined individual variations in general self-efficacy, self-esteem, and “the Big Five personality traits, including openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism.” 192 accountants working in the Sakarya and Kocaeli regions participated in the study through online surveys. The findings indicate that positive personality traits, such as agreeableness and extraversion, have beneficial effects on both job performance and mental health. Additionally, self-esteem and self-efficacy were positively correlated with these variables. Conversely, neuroticism was identified as having a negative impact on both mental health and job performance. These results highlight the importance of assessing personality traits before hiring accountants. Neuroticism may increase the likelihood of experiencing difficulties in the accounting profession. Therefore, it is crucial to consider personality traits in the recruitment processes of accounting professionals.
Article
Purpose Online employee reviews have emerged as a crucial information source for business managers to evaluate employee behavior and firm performance. The purpose of this paper is to test the relationship between employee personality traits, derived from online employee reviews and job satisfaction and turnover behavior at the individual level. Design/methodology/approach The authors apply text-mining techniques to extract personality traits from online employee reviews on Indeed.com based on the Big Five theory. They also apply a machine learning classification algorithm to demonstrate that incorporating personality traits can significantly enhance employee turnover prediction accuracy. Findings Personality traits such as agreeableness, conscientiousness and openness are positively associated with job satisfaction, while extraversion and neuroticism are negatively related to job satisfaction. Moreover, the impact of personality traits on overall job satisfaction is stronger for former employees than for current employees. Personality traits are significantly linked to employee turnover behavior, with a one-unit increase in the neuroticism score raising the probability of an employee becoming a former employee by 0.6%. Practical implications These findings have implications for firm managers looking to gain insights into employee online review behavior and improve firm performance. Online employee review websites are recommended to include the identified personality traits. Originality/value This study identifies employee personality traits from automated analysis of employee-generated data and verifies their relationship with employee satisfaction and employee turnover, providing new insights into the development of human resources in the era of big data.
Article
Full-text available
As research in psychology becomes more sophisticated and more oriented toward the development and testing of theory, it becomes more important to eliminate biases in data caused by measurement error. Both failure to correct for biases induced by measurement error and improper corrections can lead to erroneous conclusions that retard progress toward cumulative knowledge. Corrections for attenuation due to measurement error are common in the literature today and are becoming more common, yet errors are frequently made in this process. Technical psychometric presentations of abstract measurement theory principles have proved inadequte in improving the practices of working researchers. As an alternative, this article uses realistic research scenarios (cases) to illustrate and explain appropriate and inappropriate instances of correction for measurement error in commonly occurring research situations.
Article
The current study was designed to investigate the situational, dispositional, and affective antecedents of counterproductive work behaviors. A model based on the organizational frustration–aggression work of Spector and colleagues was tested using structural equation modeling and zero-order correlational analysis. As expected, a positive relationship was found between employees' experience of situational constraints (events frustrating their achievement of organizational and personal goals) and counterproductive behavioral responses to frustration (personal and organizational aggression), mediated by affective reactions to frustration. In addition, personality (trait anger and trait anxiety), control beliefs (Work Locus of Control), and estimation of likelihood of punishment were strongly associated with affective and behavioral responses. In particular, strong direct relationships were found between affective response variables and anxiety and locus of control, while direct relationships were found between behavioral response variables and anger and punishment. Finally, differentiated relationships between two facets of trait anger (angry temperament and angry reaction) and four categories of counterproductive behaviors (serious and minor deviance directed at organizational and personal targets) were explored. Copyright © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
This study examined a theoretical model that linked neuroticism, extraversion, daily hassles and uplifts in bath work and nonwork domains, job satisfaction, and nonwork satisfaction to overall life satisfaction. Structural equation analyses were conducted on 3 waves of data obtained from 479 police officers. It was found that job satisfaction and nonwork satisfaction made independent contributions to overall life satisfaction, but there was no significant relationship between work experiences and nonwork satisfaction, nor between nonwork experiences and job satisfaction. These findings support a segregation model rather than a spillover model of the links between the work and nonwork domains of employees' lives. Moreover, the total effects showed that life satisfaction was determined, in order of importance, by nonwork satisfaction, neuroticism, nonwork hassles, job satisfaction, nonwork uplifts, extraversion, work hassles, and work uplifts.
Article
Recently, several researchers have argued that previous work in organizational science has underestimated the contributions made by dispositional factors to the prediction of important job outcomes, and have proposed that an individual's behavior is co-determined by characteristics of the individual in a dynamic interplay with the situation.^ The present study examined the roles that dispositions play in the prediction of various salient organizational outcomes, such as facets of work satisfaction and nonattendance behaviors. The basic research question addressed the effects of particular dispositional factors (i.e., Negative and Positive Affectivity, Time Orientation and Self-monitoring) in their relationships with (1) perceptions of job demands, job complexity and control, (2) job satisfaction, and (3) tardiness and absence behaviors.^ Questionnaire responses were received from 115 blue-collar workers employed at a medium-sized, midwestern manufacturing plant. These data measured dispositions, perceptions and attitudes. Objective measures assessed job demands and attendance. Path analyses, using hierarchical multiple regressions, were used to assess the direct and indirect relationships.^ The findings showed that individuals who perceive that their jobs are high in psychological demands, who view themselves as having low control over those jobs, who have a diffused-point time orientation, and who are dissatisfied with their supervisors were more likely to have greater tardiness behavior. Persons who took voluntary absences, on the other hand, had less physically demanding jobs, perceived themselves to have greater control at work, and were generally satisfied with their pay, but dissatisfied with their supervisors and coworkers. Further, individuals scoring high in Negative Affectivity, but low in Self-monitoring, were likely to be absent due to ill health.^ Conclusions reached in this study included the following: (1) there may be health implications for high-Negative Affectivity and low Self-monitoring individuals when they do not have control and well-liked coworkers and supervisors at work, (2) a distinction between nonattendance behaviors is both practically and theoretically important for practicing managers, and (3) dispositional influences are an important component to understanding and predicting behavior in an organizational context.
Article
This meta-analysis evaluated predictors of both objective and subjective sales performance. Biodata measures and sales ability inventories were good predictors of the ratings criterion, with corrected rs of .52 and .45, respectively. Potency (a subdimension of the Big 5 personality dimension Extraversion) predicted supervisor ratings of performance (r =.28) and objective measures of sales (r =.26). Achievement (a component of the Conscientiousness dimension) predicted ratings (r =.25) and objective sales (r=.41). General cognitive ability showed a correlation of .40 with ratings but only .04 with objective sales. Similarly, age predicted ratings (r =.26) but not objective sales (r = -.06). On the basis of a small number of studies, interest appears to be a promising predictor of sales success.