ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Why HID headlights bother older drivers

Authors:

Abstract

Driving requires effective coordination of visual, motor, and cognitive skills. Visual skills are pushed to their limit at night by decreased illumination and by disabling glare from oncoming headlights. High intensity discharge (HID) headlamps project light farther down roads, improving their owner's driving safety by increasing the time available for reaction to potential problems. Glare is proportional to headlamp brightness, however, so increasing headlamp brightness also increases potential glare for oncoming drivers, particularly on curving two lane roads. This problem is worse for older drivers because of their increased intraocular light scattering, glare sensitivity, and photostress recovery time. An analysis of automobile headlights, intraocular stray light, glare, and night driving shows that brightness rather than blueness is the primary reason for the visual problems that HID headlights can cause for older drivers who confront them. The increased light projected by HID headlights is potentially valuable, but serious questions remain regarding how and where it should be projected.
PERSPECTIVE
Why HID headlights bother older drivers
M A Mainster, G T Timberlake
.............................................................................................................................
Br J Ophthalmol
2003;87:113–117
Driving requires effective coordination of visual, motor,
and cognitive skills. Visual skills are pushed to their limit
at night by decreased illumination and by disabling
glare from oncoming headlights. High intensity
discharge (HID) headlamps project light farther down
roads, improving their owner’s driving safety by
increasing the time available for reaction to potential
problems. Glare is proportional to headlamp brightness,
however, so increasing headlamp brightness also
increases potential glare for oncoming drivers,
particularly on curving two lane roads. This problem is
worse for older drivers because of their increased
intraocular light scattering, glare sensitivity, and
photostress recovery time. An analysis of automobile
headlights, intraocular stray light, glare, and night
driving shows that brightness rather than blueness is the
primary reason for the visual problems that HID
headlights can cause for older drivers who confront
them. The increased light projected by HID headlights is
potentially valuable, but serious questions remain
regarding how and where it should be projected.
..........................................................................
H
igh intensity discharge (HID) headlights
are brighter, more energy efficient, and
longer lasting than conventional incandes-
cent headlights. They allow owners to detect road
hazards, signage, and pedestrians at greater
distances. They are standard equipment on grow-
ing numbers of more expensive automobiles.
HID headlights probably improve the safety of
night driving for their users. Unfortunately, they
can dazzle viewers on two lane highways, making
it more difficult for approaching drivers to
identify pedestrians, road hazards, and curves in
the road. Night-time driving is difficult for older
individuals. Confronting HID headlights makes it
even more difficult. To understand why older
drivers may complain about their encounters
with HID headlights, it is useful to understand
automotive headlight design and how ageing
affects intraocular light scattering, glare, and
automobile driving.
HEADLIGHTS
Conventional headlights use incandescent light
bulbs. An incandescent bulb consists of a tung-
sten filament mounted in a sealed glass container.
The bulb is evacuated or filled with gases to
prevent filament oxidation. The filament glows
when it is heated by an electrical current. Higher
filament temperatures produce brighter, bluer
light. They also shorten a filament’s lifetime.
A tungsten-halogen (halogen) bulb provides
brighter light at higher filament temperatures,
using a regeneration cycle to lengthen the
tungsten filament’s lifetime.
1
A halogen gas such
as iodine is added to the bulb’s atmosphere. When
tungsten evaporates from a heated filament, it
collects on relatively cool bulb surfaces where it
combines with iodine. Volatile tungsten-halide
diffuses back to the heated filament, dissociating
and redepositing tungsten on the filament.
Irregularities in the redeposition process eventu-
ally cause filament thinning and bulb failure.
Halogen bulbs are used widely in automotive
headlights and also in ophthalmic instruments.
The luminous power output of halogen headlights
increases with increasing wavelength across the
visible spectrum.
2–4
HID lamps overcome many of the limitations of
incandescent bulbs. A discharge lamp consists of
two electrodes in a quartz container filled with a
high pressure gas. An electronic starter initiates
an electric discharge between the electrodes, pro-
ducing an ionised gas (a plasma) that generates a
continuous spectrum of light as well as narrow
spectral lines. Mercury arc highway illumination
lamps are HID devices. Xenon HID headlights
produce some continuous spectrum white light,
but much of their luminous power is generated as
broadened spectral lines, including blue lines at
405, 435, and 475 nm.
4
HID headlamps produce
two to three times more luminous power (flux)
than halogen bulbs.
56
The colour temperature of a light source is the
temperature of a black body that emits radiation
of the same subjective colour as the light
source.
78
Higher colour temperature sources have
a bluer appearance. Conventional incandescent
bulbs, halogen incandescent bulbs, xenon HID
bulbs, and the sun’s disc at midday when viewed
from ground level have colour temperatures
around 2800, 3200, 4200, and 5600°K,
respectively.
18
Thus, HID bulbs are not as blue as
ordinary sunlight, but they are brighter and bluer
than conventional halogen bulbs.
A HID headlight system consists of a discharge
lamp, its electronics, and a reflector. Reflector
design determines the illumination pattern pro-
jected ahead and adjacent to a vehicle.
3
It is also a
key factor in determining how bright a headlight
appears to an observer (that is, the headlight’s
luminance in candles/m
2
, where a candle is lumi-
nous intensity in lumens/steradian) or how
brightly the headlight lights up a surface at the
observer’s location (that is, the surface illumi-
nance that the headlight produces in lumens/m
2
or lux).
39
Automobiles have high beam and low beam
illumination systems. High beam illumination is
See end of article for
authors’ affiliations
.......................
Martin A Mainster, PhD,
MD, Department of
Ophthalmology, University
of Kansas Medical Center,
3901 Rainbow Boulevard,
Kansas City, KS
66160-7379, USA;
mmainste@kumc.edu
Accepted for publication
1 September 2002
.......................
113
www.bjophthalmol.com
aimed parallel to the road surface and not intended for use
with oncoming traffic. Low beam headlights are aimed slightly
downward to reduce glare for oncoming drivers. The brightest
central area (“hot spot”) in HID headlight illumination
patterns is positioned away from the centre of the road so as
to reduce an oncoming driver’s light exposure. Compared to
halogen low beam headlights, HID headlights have a larger
hot spot, project light farther down the road, and project light
farther to the right and left of their optical axis.
Low beam HID headlight systems in Europe and the United
States have comparatively sharp and soft horizontal illumina-
tion cutoffs, respectively.
51011
Luminous intensity drops off
abruptly (sharp cutoff) above the height of European
headlights, but declines more slowly (soft cutoff) above the
height of American headlights. Each of these illumination
patterns has its own advantages and disadvantages. European
HID headlights provide more light close to driver and less light
farther down the road than American HID headlights. The
sharp cutoff of luminous intensity above the horizontal height
of European headlights protects oncoming drivers from glare,
but limits visibility distance. The sharp cutoff also causes
“flashing” on bumpy roads when the horizontal cutoff
bounces up and down, into and out of an oncoming driver’s
field of view. Conversely, the softer American horizontal cutoff
causes more glare for oncoming drivers but provides better
overhead sign visibility, less flashing, and longer visibility dis-
tances. Adaptive headlights that adjust to changing driving
conditions could offer the advantages of both American and
European illumination systems.
5
LIGHT SCATTERING
Scattered light from oncoming headlights makes night
driving more difficult, in part because the human eye is an
imperfect optical device. Light from the visual environment
enters the eye through the pupil and is imaged on the retina.
Additional light enters the eye by transillumination through
the iris and sclera. Some light is absorbed in photoreceptor
photopigments or other pigments such as melanin, haemo-
globin, xanthophyll, and lipofuscin. Some of it is deflected by
light scattering in ocular tissues.
Light that has been scattered in the eye is termed “stray”
light. Stray light reaching the fovea decreases the contrast of
foveal images, producing disability glare. Light scattering,
reflection, and absorption determine the spectrum of in-
traocular stray light.
12–16
The directionality and spectrum of scattered light depends
on the density and size of the particles that scatter the light.
Particle density determines the intensity of light scattering.
7
Particle size determines its directionality and wavelength
dependence.
17 18
Small particle or Rayleigh scattering has no preferential
direction, but small particles scatter shorter wavelengths more
efficiently than longer wavelengths.
71719
Daylight sky is blue
because light reaching an observer from any direction other
than directly out of the sun is scattered by atmospheric parti-
cles that are small in comparison to visible light wavelengths.
Conversely, light coming directly from the setting sun appears
red because shorter wavelength light is scattered out of the
sun’s image during atmospheric transit.
Large particle or Mie scattering is not wavelength depend-
ent, but light is preferentially scattered in the forward
direction.
717
Mie scattering by retinal pigment epithelial
melanin granules, roughly 1000 nm in diameter,
20 21
improves
retinal image contrast by suppressing side scattered light.
22
Fog doesn’t change the apparent colour of automobile
headlights because large fog droplets scatter all visible
wavelengths equally effectively.
Stray light from the cornea and lens decreases with
increasing wavelength,
16
showing the influence of small parti-
cle scattering. Stray light from fundus reflectance or transillu-
mination increases with increasing wavelength,
16 23
showing
the influence of the decreased optical absorption of melanin
and haemoglobin in the red end of the visible spectrum.
15
The
net effect is that stray light reaching the fovea has little wave-
length dependence.
16 24 25
Thus, blue-white HID headlights
should produce no more foveal stray light or disability glare
than white headlights of the same luminance.
GLARE AND PHOTOSTRESS
Glare can cause discomfort or disability.
26 27
Discomfort glare
does not impair vision, but it can be startling or distracting to
a driver, and cause blinking, squinting, ocular aversion, and
fatigue. The physiological and psychophysical origins of
discomfort glare remain uncertain.
28 29
Disability glare does impair visual performance. It has been
divided classically into “veiling, “dazzle, and “scotomatic”
glare based primarily on the type of glare source.
30
Veiling disability glare occurs when a diffuse light source
reduces the contrast of a visual target by “somewhat
uniformly” superimposing light on the visual target’s retinal
image.
30
Veiling glare makes outdoor reading more difficult in
bright sunshine.
30
It also obscures indoor visualisation of
material between two adjacent windows illuminated by
brilliant sunlight.
30
Dazzle disability glare occurs when a bright glare source is
imaged at an extrafoveal location.
30 31
Ocular transit scatters
some light from the glare source light onto the viewer’s fovea.
This stray light decreases the contrast between the lighter and
darker details of a visual target’s foveal image.
14 32–34
Dazzle
glare from an oncoming vehicle’s headlights makes it more
difficult at night for a driver to identify the edge of a curving
two lane highway.
Scotomatic disability glare occurs when a brilliant light
source decreases visual sensitivity (“puts a retinal area
temporarily out of business”).
30
Sensitivity is diminished
while the visual system rapidly light adapts during glare
exposure and then more slowly dark adapts after glare expo-
sure. This “photostress” may startle and disorient observers,
producing afterimages that interfere with vision.
35
Scotomatic
glare occurs during flash photography and momentary expo-
sure to an inept lecturer’s laser pointer beam.
36
It is caused
primarily by rapid bleaching and subsequent slower regenera-
tion of retinal photoreceptor photopigments.
30 31 37–39
Clinical glare terminology is influenced by its testing meth-
ods. In ophthalmic parlance, clinical disability glare generally
refers to classic veiling and classic dazzle glare
30
produced by
intraocular stray light.
14 34
Clinical photostress refers to classic
scotomatic glare
30 40 41
caused by photopigment bleaching,
regeneration, and associated psychophysical processes.
41–48
Clinical terminology will be used throughout the rest of this
perspective.
Strictly speaking, clinical disability glare (classic veiling and
classic dazzle glare) is an optical process which should resolve
immediately after glare exposure because visual adaptation is
unaffected. Conversely, photostress is a psychophysical process
which should persist after light exposure because dark adap-
tation takes time to restore visual sensitivity to its pre-
exposure level. In reality, most clinical glare tests don’t differ-
entiate between optical and psychophysical processes. Clinical
glare sources are often bright enough to produce photostress
as well as disability glare, accounting for the use of terms such
as “glare recovery time”
49 50
for “photostress recovery
time.
38 51 52
Light adaptation can be controlled in specialised
disability glare tests,
53
but photostress sources always produce
stray light and disability glare sources can alter visual adapta-
tion.
Further confusion arises because methods other than
disability glare and photostress testing can be used to study
visual perception in suboptimal viewing circumstances.
114 Mainster, Timberlake
www.bjophthalmol.com
Disability glare testing illuminates a patient’s eye with an off-
axis light source to produce stray intraocular light that
decreases the contrast of a foveal target. Visual performance
under adverse conditions can also be studied without a sepa-
rate glare source, however, by decreasing the contrast of an
extraocular target before it is imaged on the retina. For exam-
ple, reduced contrast optotypes and sine wave gratings are
used in variable contrast acuity and contrast sensitivity
testing.
54–57
These studies are not considered to be disability
glare or photostress tests, but they use light sources, so they do
produce intraocular stray light and disability glare.
TESTING GLARE AND PHOTOSTRESS
Discomfort glare can be quantified from numerical estimates
of the extent of discomfort that people experience when they
are exposed to a particular retinal illumination while
performing a specific visual task. Discomfort glare varies at
different times and in different individuals, and it is worse in
older than younger people.
426345859
Most disability glare tests measure visual thresholds for
optotype, grating, or acuity targets in the presence or absence
of a glare source.
27 53 60–66
Poor performance on glare sensitivity
testing is often referred to as increased “glare sensitivity” or
decreased “glare resistance.” Stray light that causes glare can
be measured without threshold testing using an annular
flickering glare source that scatters light into the fovea. The
intensity of a foveal target flickering in counterphase with the
glare source is adjusted until flickering from the peripheral
glare source is no longer visible.
67–69
Stray light and disability glare increase with increasing
glare source intensity because there is more light scattered
intraocularly to reduce retinal image contrast.
14 16 23 27 30
Light
scattering increases in people over 50 years of age.
14 16 25 33 70 71
Degenerative changes throughout the visual system may
reduce the efficiency of visual information processing.
38 59
Older individuals perform worse than young ones on standard
disability glare tests, and even worse if they have cataract
formation.
33 59 65 72 73
Disability glare is increased by cataract,
74
intraocular lens implantation,
75–77
and posterior capsule
opacification.
78
Transient glare as occurs in a headlight
encounter is worse than static glare from a light source of the
same luminance, and this effect increases with ageing and
media opacities.
66 79
Most clinical photostress tests measure how long it takes
for visual sensitivity to recover after bright light exposure.
Vision is the most common test end point,
39
but pupillary
80
and
visually evoked
81 82
recovery times can also be measured. Pho-
tostress luminances and exposure durations vary between
protocols. Photostress recovery times have been monitored
with visual acuity test letters, grating targets, and Landolt-C
dark adaptometer targets.
40 51 52
Photostress recovery time increases with photostress source
luminance.
31 35 43 48
It also increases with ageing, although dif-
ferent rates of increase are reported for different testing
methods.
38 49 50 83–85
Macular degeneration and other retinal dis-
orders markedly prolong recovery times.
39 52 86 87
AGEING AND NIGHT DRIVING
The older driver is visually disadvantaged. Almost all self
reported and clinical measures of visual function decline with
ageing.
73 88–91
Eye disorders increase this problem, and their
prevalence also increases with ageing.
88 92 93
Even when high
contrast visual acuity is normal, visual performance decreases
with increasing age on most other sensory tests, including
visual field, glare recovery time, stereopsis, contrast sensitivity,
and low contrast visual acuity with and without glare.
72 89–91 94
Successful driving is a symphony of visual, motor, and cog-
nitive abilities.
95–99
Declining vision increases non-sensory
demands, which also decline with ageing. Older drivers tend
to make less efficient eye movements
100
and slower
decisions
101
in driving situations. Cataract, visual field loss, and
glaucoma are all associated with increased accident risk, as are
reduced mesopic vision and increased glare sensitivity.
94 102–108
Night driving is a mesopic (intermediate brightness vision)
rather than a scotopic (night-time vision) or photopic
(daytime vision) task. It can push even normal visual systems
to their limits. Visual acuity decreases with decreasing target
illumination. This loss is greater in people over 65 years of
age.
109 110
The distance at which night-time highway signs can
be read decreases significantly in older individuals compared
to younger people of the same visual acuity.
111
Headlight glare
sensitivity also increases with ageing.
26
The increased brightness of HID headlights is an advantage
for the older drivers who use them. They enable owners to see
farther down straight roads, providing them with more time
to respond to potential problems. Conversely, disability glare
and photostress recovery time increase with glare source
luminance.
14 23 27 30 31 35 43
Thus, HID headlights can produce
more glare than halogen headlamps for drivers who confront
them on curving or hilly two lane highways.
Older individuals have increased intraocular stray
light, glare sensitivity, and photostress recovery
time.
26 33 38 49 50 72 73 83–85 112
Thus, confronting brilliant HID head-
lights is a greater potential problem for older than younger
drivers. HID headlights also cause more discomfort glare than
conventional halogen headlights of the same photopic or sco-
topic illuminance.
2 4 113
This potential distraction is greater in
61–77 than 20–31 year old drivers.
4
Windshield or spectacle filters decrease night-time visibil-
ity, so they are not a useful solution for headlight glare.
114–116
Some older drivers simply choose to limit their night-
time driving,
107 117 118
but there is still a need to develop
screening techniques to identify drivers at greatest
accident risk.
79 103 106 119 120
Promising techniques have been
examined,
91 104 120 121
but the selection of appropriate methods
for screening older drivers raises complex medical, social, and
legal issues that remain under investigation.
Practical solutions for the highway glare problem have been
studied for decades.
3526
Countermeasures have never been
implemented seriously because of consumer disinterest,
manufacturer resistance, and lack of legislative resolve. HID
headlight glare has now captured the attention of many con-
sumers. There have been improvements in divided highway
design such as wide medians and glare screens separating
opposing lanes. Unfortunately, these construction advances
can’t be implemented on two lane, undivided highways where
headlight glare is worst.
Glare countermeasures include adaptive headlights and
ultraviolet headlight systems. Adaptive headlight systems
monitor and compensate both optically and mechanically for
changing traffic, road, and meteorological conditions. Ultra-
violet headlight systems project invisible ultraviolet radiation,
improving the visibility of fluorescent highway markers, signs,
and objects without increasing glare for oncoming drivers.
Ultraviolet radiation is especially valuable in fog because
highway signs can fluoresce in the visible spectrum when
exposed to ultraviolet radiation, but backscattered ultraviolet
radiation is invisible so it can’t reduce the contrast and legibil-
ity of the signs. This situation is similar to retinal imaging in
patients with asteroid hyalosis. Ordinary reflectance images
have poor detail because back scattered flashlamp light
reduces the contrast and visibility of retinal features. In fluo-
rescein angiography, however, back scattered blue light from
the photographic flash is blocked from reaching the camera by
a barrier filter, and green fundus fluorescence provides good
retinal image contrast.
Polarising headlight systems are perhaps the best solution
to the highway glare problem.
5
A polarising filter is placed in
front of all automobile headlights, with its axis of polarisation
inclined at 45° to the vertical. Another polarising filter known
as an analyser is placed in front of the eyes of all drivers. Ana-
lysers have the same axis of polarisation as polarisers in front
Why HID headlights bother older drivers 115
www.bjophthalmol.com
of their own headlights, which is perpendicular to that of
polarisers in oncoming vehicles. Thus, analysers block light
from opposing traffic but transmit light scattered from
roadside objects by their own headlights. Even ideal analysers
block approximately 50% of the light reaching them, but HID
headlights provide adequate headlamp brightness for fixed
analyser systems, and there are methods for switching analys-
ers on and off when polarised light is detected. A major draw-
back in the adoption of polarising headlamp systems is the
fact that their cost would be borne by their owners, but the
systems would benefit only oncoming drivers until they
became widely used.
5
CONCLUSION
A HID headlamp manufacturer touting the fact that a “xenon
bulb produces more than twice the amount of light of a halo-
gen bulb,
6
answered the question “Why does xenon light
sometimes appear to irritate oncoming drivers?”
6
by explain-
ing that “due to the conspicuous colour of xenon light, drivers
are more inclined to look in the headlamps.
6
Older drivers
encountering HID headlights on a winding two lane highway
know that “photophilia” isn’t the answer.
Night driving can push any visual system to its limits. Con-
ventional clinical glare tests underestimate disability glare
from moving light sources. Disability glare increases with
increasing glare source brightness. HID headlights are
brighter than conventional tungsten-halogen headlights.
Thus, they cause more disability glare under identical viewing
circumstances.
Visual quality declines with ageing, even in individuals with
20/20 visual acuity. Glare due to intraocular light scattering
increases with ageing, prompting many older drivers to curtail
their night driving activities. Increased glare from HID head-
lights is one more visual hurdle for the older driver.
Aircraft landing lights would allow automobile drivers to see
farther down a road. They would also incapacitate oncoming
drivers. Brighter headlights do provide increased visibility for
older drivers who use them. They also cause more glare for older
drivers who confront them. The optimal balance in headlight
brightness between owner visibility and viewer disability
depends on the driving situation, but technologies are available
for more adaptable, less bothersome headlight systems.
Governmental regulations determine which headlights we
encounter. Acceptance or rejection of the current generation of
HID xenon headlights ultimately depends on their record in
traffic and litigation. Xenon bulbs make good headlights. They
also make good glare sources. The additional light they project is
valuable. Questions remain regarding how and where it should
be projected. Until these questions are resolved, for many older
drivers confronting stylish HID headlights on two lane roads,
glare rather than beauty will be in the eye of the beholder.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
Supported in part by the Kansas Lions Sight Foundation, Inc.
.....................
Authors’ affiliations
M A Mainster, G T Timberlake, Department of Ophthalmology,
University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas
City, KS 66160-7379, USA
REFERENCES
1 Levi L.
Applied optics: a guide to optical system design
. New York: John
Wiley, 1968.
2 Flannagan MJ, Sivak M, Gellatly AW,
et al
.
A field study of discomfort
glare from high-intensity discharge headlights.
Ann Arbor, MI:
Transportation Research Institute, The University of Michigan, 1992.
3 Sliney DH, Fast P, Ricksand A. Optical radiation hazards analysis of
ultraviolet headlights.
Appl Optics
1995;34:4912–22.
4 Flannagan MJ.
Subjective and objective aspects of headlight glare:
effects of size and spectral power distribution.
Ann Arbor, MI:
Transportation Research Institute, The University of Michigan, 1999.
5 Mace D, Garvey P, Porter RJ,
et al
W.
Countermeasures for reducing the
effects of headlight glare
. Washington, DC: The AAA Foundation for
Traffic Safety, 2001.
6 Phillips Electronics NV.
The ten most common questions about xenon
light.
http://www.eur.lighting.philips.com/automotive/html/press6.htm.
Amsterdam, Netherlands, 1999.
7 Meyer-Arendt JR.
Introduction to classical and modern optics
.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1984.
8 Wyszecki G, Stiles WS.
Color science
. New York: John Wiley, 1967.
9 Sliney DH, Wolbarsht ML.
Safety with lasers and other optical sources:
a comprehensive handbook
. New York: Plenum Press, 1980.
10 Rumar K.
Vehicle lighting and the aging population.
Ann Arbor, MI:
Traffic Research Institute, The University of Michigan, 1998.
11 Simanaitis D. Where the photons hit the road.
Road and Track
2000;52:136–41.
12 Geeraets WJ, Williams RC, Chan G,
et al
. The loss of light energy in
retina and choroid.
Arch Ophthalmol
1960;64:606–15.
13 Boettner EA, Wolter JR. Transmission of the ocular media.
Invest
Ophthalmol
1962;1:776–83.
14 Vos JJ. Disability glare—a state of the art report.
Commission
International de l’Eclairage Journal
1984;3:39–53.
15 Mainster MA. Wavelength selection in macular photocoagulation. Tissue
optics, thermal effects, and laser systems.
Ophthalmology
1986;93:952–8.
16 Van den Berg TJ. Light scattering by donor lenses as a function of depth
and wavelength.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1997;38:1321–32.
17 Minnaert M, Seymour L.
Light and color in the outdoors
. Berlin:
Springer-Verlag, 1993.
18 Van de Hulst HC.
Light scattering by small particles
. New York: Dover
Publications, 1981.
19 Nassau K.
The physics and chemistry of color: the fifteen causes of
color
. New York: John Wiley, 1983.
20 Fine BS, Yanoff M.
Ocular histology: a text and atlas
. 2nd ed.
Hagerstown, MD: Harper and Row, 1979.
21 Hogan MJ, Alvarado JA, Weddell JE.
Histology of the human eye: an
atlas and textbook
. Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1971.
22 Ham WT Jr. Remarks on fundus reflectance.
Vis Res
1975;15:1167–8.
23 Van den Berg TJ, JK IJ, de Waard PW. Dependence of intraocular
straylight on pigmentation and light transmission through the ocular wall.
Vis Res
1991;31:1361–7.
24 Wooten BR, Geri GA. Psychophysical determination of intraocular light
scatter as a function of wavelength.
Vis Res
1987;27:1291–8.
25 Whitaker D, Steen R, Elliott DB. Light scatter in the normal young,
elderly, and cataractous eye demonstrates little wavelength dependency.
Optom Vis Sci
1993;70:963–8.
26 Pulling NH, Wolf E, Sturgis SP,
et al
. Headlight glare resistance and
driver age.
Hum Factors
1980;22:103–12.
27 Abrahamsson M, Sjostrand J. Impairment of contrast sensitivity function
(CSF) as a measure of disability glare.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1986;27:1131–6.
28 Fugate JM, Fry GA. Relation of changes in pupil size to visual
discomfort.
Illuminating Engineering
1956;51:537–49.
29 Howarth PA, Heron G, Greenhouse DS,
et al
. Discomfort from glare: the
role of the pupillary hippus.
Int J Lighting Res Tech
1993;25-:37–44.
30 Bell L, Troland LT, Verhoeff FH. Report of the sub-committee on glare of
the research committee IES
Trans Illuminating Engineering Soc New York
1922;17:743–50.
31 Wolf E. Glare and age.
Arch Ophthalmol
1960;64:502–14.
32 Fry GA, Alpern M. The effect of a peripheral glare source upon the
apparent brightness of an object.
J Opt Soc Am
1953;43:189–95.
33 IJspeert JK, de Waard PW, van den Berg TJ,
et al
. The intraocular
straylight function in 129 healthy volunteers; dependence on angle, age
and pigmentation.
Vis Res
1990;30:699–707.
34 Van den Berg TJ. On the relation between glare and straylight.
Doc
Ophthalmol
1991;78:177–81.
35 Brindley GS. The discrimination of after-images.
J Physiol
1959;147:194–203.
36 Mainster MA. Blinded by the light—not!
Arch Ophthalmol
1999;117:1547–8.
37 Chilaris GA. Recovery time after macular illumination as a diagnostic
and prognostic test.
Am J Ophthalmol
1962;53:311–14.
38 Elliott DB, Whitaker D. Changes in macular function throughout
adulthood.
Doc Ophthalmol
1991;76:251–9.
39 Horiguchi M, Ito Y, Miyake Y. Extrafoveal photostress recovery test in
glaucoma and idiopathic central serous chorioretinopathy.
Br J
Ophthalmol
1998;82:1007–12.
40 Severin SL, Tour RL, Kershaw RH. Macular function and the photostress
test 1.
Arch Ophthalmol
1967;77:2–7.
41 Severin SL, Tour RL, Kershaw RH. Macular function and the photostress
test 2.
Arch Ophthalmol
1967;77:163–7.
42 Rushton WA, Gubisch RW. Glare: its measurement by cone thresholds
and by the bleaching of cone pigments.
J Opt Soc Am
1966;56:104–10.
43 Miller ND. Positive afterimage following brief high-intensity flashes.
J
Opt Soc Am
1966;56:802–6.
44 Chisum GT. Intraocular effects of flashblindness.
Aerosp Med
1968;39:861–8.
45 Mainster MA, White TJ. Photoproducts of retinal photopigments and
visual adaptation.
Vis Res
1972;12:805–23.
46 Mainster MA. Retinol transport and regeneration of human cone
photopigment.
Nat New Biol
1972;238:223–4.
47 Smith PA. A study of the transient effects of high energy laser light on
visual function. PhD Thesis: University of London, 1996.
116 Mainster, Timberlake
www.bjophthalmol.com
48 Stamper DA, Lund DJ, Molchany JW,
et al
. Laser-induced afterimages in
humans.
Percept Mot Skills
2000;91:15–33.
49 Burg A. Light sensitivity as related to age and sex.
Percept Mot Skills
1967;24:1279–88.
50 Collins M. The onset of prolonged glare recovery with age.
Ophthalmic
Physiol Opt
1989;9:368–71.
51 Glaser JS, Savino PJ, Sumers KD,
et al
. The photostress recovery test in
the clinical assessment of visual function.
Am J Ophthalmol
1977;83:255–60.
52 Sandberg MA, Gaudio AR. Slow photostress recovery and disease
severity in age-related macular degeneration.
Retina
1995;15:407–12.
53 Yuan R, Yager D, Guethlein M,
et al
. Controlling unwanted sources of
threshold change in disability glare studies: a prototype apparatus and
procedure.
Optom Vis Sci
1993;70:976–81.
54 Mainster MA. Contemporary optics and ocular pathology.
Surv
Ophthalmol
1978;23:135–42.
55 Mainster MA, Timberlake GT, Schepens CL. Automated variable contrast
acuity testing.
Ophthalmology
1981;88:1045–53.
56 Regan D, Neima D. Low-contrast letter charts as a test of visual function.
Ophthalmology
1983;90:1192–200.
57 Regan D, Neima D. Low-contrast letter charts in early diabetic
retinopathy, ocular hypertension, glaucoma, and Parkinson’s disease.
Br
J Ophthalmol
1984;68:885–9.
58 Bennett CA. The demographic variables of discomfort glare.
Lighting
Design Application
1977;7:22–4.
59 Kline DW. Light, ageing and visual performance. In: Marshall J, ed.
The
susceptible visual apparatus
. London: Macmillan Press, 1991:150–61.
60 Miller D, Jernigan ME, Molnar S,
et al
. Laboratory evaluation of a
clinical glare tester.
Arch Ophthalmol
1972;87:324–32.
61 Paulsson LE, Sjostrand J. Contrast sensitivity in the presence of a glare
light. Theoretical concepts and preliminary clinical studies.
Invest
Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1980;19:401–6.
62 LeClaire J, Nadler MP, Weiss S,
et al
. A new glare tester for clinical
testing. Results comparing normal subjects and variously corrected
aphakic patients.
Arch Ophthalmol
1982;100:153–8.
63 Pelli DG, Robson JG. The design of a new letter chart for measuring
contrast sensitivity.
Clin Vis Sci
1988;2:187–99.
64 American Academy of Ophthalmology. Contrast sensitivity and glare
testing in the evaluation of anterior segment disease.
Ophthalmology
1990;97:1233–7.
65 Elliott DB, Bullimore MA. Assessing the reliability, discriminative ability,
and validity of disability glare tests.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1993;34:108–19.
66 Bichao IC, Yager D, Meng J. Disability glare: effects of temporal
characteristics of the glare source and of the visual-field location of the
test stimulus.
J Opt Soc Am A
1995;12:2252–8.
67 Van den Berg TJ. Importance of pathological intraocular light scatter for
visual disability.
Doc Ophthalmol
1986;61:327–33.
68 Beckman C, Abrahamsson M, Sjostrand J,
et al
. Evaluation of a clinical
glare test based on estimation of intraocular light scatter.
Optom Vis Sci
1991;68:881–7.
69 Van den Berg TJ. Clinical assessment of intraocular stray light.
Applied
Optics
1992;31:3694–6.
70 Van den Berg TJ. Analysis of intraocular straylight, especially in relation
to age.
Optom Vis Sci
1995;72:52–9.
71 Hennelly ML, Barbur JL, Edgar DF,
et al
. The effect of age on the light
scattering characteristics of the eye.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
1998;18:197–203.
72 Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Brabyn JA. Seeing into old age:
vision function beyond acuity.
Optom Vis Sci
1999;76:141–58.
73 Ivers RQ, Mitchell P, Cumming RG. Visual function tests, eye disease
and symptoms of visual disability: a population-based assessment.
Clin
Exp Ophthalmol
2000;28:41–7.
74 Elliott DB, Gilchrist J, Whitaker D. Contrast sensitivity and glare
sensitivity changes with three types of cataract morphology: are these
techniques necessary in a clinical evaluation of cataract?
Ophthalmic
Physiol Opt
1989;9:25–30.
75 Dick HB, Krummenauer F, Schwenn O,
et al
. Objective and subjective
evaluation of photic phenomena after monofocal and multifocal
intraocular lens implantation.
Ophthalmology
1999;106:1878–86.
76 Weatherill J, Yap M. Contrast sensitivity in pseudophakia and aphakia.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
1986;6:297–301.
77 Schmitz S, Dick HB, Krummenauer F,
et al
. Contrast sensitivity and glare
disability by halogen light after monofocal and multifocal lens
implantation.
Br J Ophthalmol
2000;84:1109–12.
78 Tan JC, Spalton DJ, Arden GB. Comparison of methods to assess visual
impairment from glare and light scattering with posterior capsule
opacification.
J Cataract Refract Surg
1998;24:1626–31.
79 Anderson SJ, Holliday IE. Night driving: effects of glare from vehicle
headlights on motion perception.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
1995;15:545–51.
80 Zabriskie NA, Kardon RH. The pupil photostress test.
Ophthalmology
1994;101:1122–30.
81 Lovasik JV. An electrophysiological investigation of the macular
photostress test.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1983;24:437–41.
82 Parisi V. Electrophysiological evaluation of the macular cone adaptation:
VEP after photostress. A review.
Doc Ophthalmol
2001;102:251–62.
83 Wolf E. Studies on the scatter of light in the dioptric media of the eye as
a basis of visual glare.
Arch Ophthalmol
1965;74:338–45.
84 Reading VM. Disability glare and age.
Vis Res
1968;8:207–14.
85 Margrain TH, Thomson D. Sources of variability in the clinical
photostress test.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
2002;22:61–7.
86 Magder H. Test for central serous retinopathy based on clinical
observations and trial.
Am J Ophthalmol
1960;49:147–50.
87 Wu G, Weiter JJ, Santos S,
et al
. The macular photostress test in diabetic
retinopathy and age-related macular degeneration.
Arch Ophthalmol
1990;108:1556–8.
88 Tielsch JM, Sommer A, Witt K,
et al
. Blindness and visual impairment in
an American urban population. The Baltimore Eye Survey.
Arch
Ophthalmol
1990;108:286–90.
89 Rubin GS, West SK, Munoz B,
et al
. A comprehensive assessment of
visual impairment in a population of older Americans. The SEE Study.
Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1997;38:557–68.
90 Klein BE, Klein R, Lee KE,
et al
Associations of performance-based and
self-reported measures of visual function. The Beaver Dam Eye Study.
Ophthalmic Epidemiol
1999;6:49–60.
91 Brabyn J, Schneck M, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G,
et al
. The Smith-Kettlewell
Institute (SKI) longitudinal study of vision function and its impact among
the elderly: an overview.
Optom Vis Sci
2001;78:264–9.
92 Ivers RQ, Mitchell P, Cumming RG. Sensory impairment and driving: the
Blue Mountains Eye Study.
Am J Public Health
1999;89:85–7.
93 Weih LM, VanNewkirk MR, McCarty CA,
et al
. Age-specific causes of
bilateral visual impairment.
Arch Ophthalmol
2000;118:264–9.
94 Johnson CA, Keltner JL. Incidence of visual field loss in 20,000 eyes
and its relationship to driving performance.
Arch Ophthalmol
1983;101:371–5.
95 Sivak M, Soler J, Trankle U. Cross-cultural differences in driver
risk-taking.
Accid Anal Prev
1989;21:363–9.
96 Sivak M, Olson PL, Kewman DG,
et al
. Driving and perceptual/cognitive
skills: behavioral consequences of brain damage.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
1981;62:476–83.
97 Rumar K. The basic driver error: late detection.
Ergonomics
1990;33:1281–90.
98 Sivak M. The information that drivers use: is it indeed 90% visual?
Perception
1996;25:1081–9.
99 Stutts JC, Stewart JR, Martell C. Cognitive test performance and crash
risk in an older driver population.
Accid Anal Prev
1998;30:337–46.
100 Maltz M, Shinar D. Eye movements of younger and older drivers.
Hum
Factors
1999;41:15–25.
101 Walker N, Fain WB, Fisk AD,
et al
. Aging and decision making:
driving-related problem solving.
Hum Factors
1997;39:438–44.
102 Von Hebenstreit B. Visual acuity and traffic accidents.
Klin Monatsbl
Augenheilkd
1984;185:86–90.
103 Keltner JL, Johnson CA. Visual function and driving safety.
Arch
Ophthalmol
1992;110:1697–8.
104 Ball K, Owsley C, Sloane ME,
et al
. Visual attention problems as a
predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1993;34:3110–23.
105 Owsley C, McGwin G Jr, Ball K. Vision impairment, eye disease, and
injurious motor vehicle crashes in the elderly.
Ophthalmic Epidemiol
1998;5:101–13.
106 Lachenmayr B, Berger J, Buser A,
et al
. Reduced visual capacity
increases the risk of accidents in street traffic.
Ophthalmologe
1998;95:44–50.
107 Owsley C, Stalvey B, Wells J,
et al
. Older drivers and cataract: driving
habits and crash risk.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
1999;54:M203–11.
108 Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G Jr,
et al
. Visual processing impairment and
risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults.
JAMA
1998;279:1083–8.
109 Sturgis SP, Osgood DJ. Effects of glare and background luminance on
visual acuity and contrast sensitivity: implications for driver night vision
testing.
Hum Factors
1982;24:347–60.
110 Sturr JF, Kline GE, Taub HA. Performance of young and older drivers on
a static acuity test under photopic and mesopic luminance conditions.
Hum Factors
1990;32:1–8.
111 Sivak M, Olson PL, Pastalan LA. Effect of driver’s age on nighttime
legibility of highway signs.
Hum Factors
1981;23:59–64.
112 Scharwey K, Krzizok T, Herfurth M. Night driving capacity of
ophthalmologically healthy persons of various ages.
Ophthalmologe
1998;95:555–8.
113 Flannagan MJ, Sivak M, Battle DS,
et al
.
Discomfort glare from
high-intensity discharge headlamps: effects of context and experience.
Ann Arbor, MI: Transportation Research Institute, The University of
Michigan, 1993.
114 Steen R, Whitaker D, Elliott DB,
et al
. Effect of filters on disability glare.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
1993;13:371–6.
115 Marmor MF. Double fault! Ocular hazards of a tennis sunglass.
Arch
Ophthalmol
2001;119:1064–6.
116 Eperjesi F, Fowler CW, Evans BJ. Do tinted lenses or filters improve
visual performance in low vision? A review of the literature.
Ophthalmic
Physiol Opt
2002;22:68–77.
117 Stutts JC. Do older drivers with visual and cognitive impairments drive
less?
J Am Geriatr Soc
1998;46:854–61.
118 Ball K, Owsley C, Stalvey B,
et al
. Driving avoidance and functional
impairment in older drivers.
Accid Anal Prev
1998;30:313–22.
119 Grosskopf U, Wagner R, Jacobi FK,
et al
. Twilight vision and glare
sensitivity in monofocal and multifocal pseudophakia.
Ophthalmologe
1998;95:432–7.
120 Mantyjarvi M, Tuppurainen K. Cataract in traffic.
Graefes Arch Clin Exp
Ophthalmol
1999;237:278–82.
121 Charman WN. Vision and driving—a literature review and commentary.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
1997;17:371–91.
Why HID headlights bother older drivers 117
www.bjophthalmol.com
... 9,10 Visual challenges associated with nighttime driving is further worsened by the headlights of on-coming vehicles. [11][12][13][14] Straylight from headlights can reduce retinal image contrast which manifests as either dazzling glare or scotomatic glare. [13][14][15] Dazzling glare occurs when high illumination sweeps across the retina and induces light avoidance behavior such as squinting and looking away from the source of glare. ...
... [11][12][13][14] Straylight from headlights can reduce retinal image contrast which manifests as either dazzling glare or scotomatic glare. [13][14][15] Dazzling glare occurs when high illumination sweeps across the retina and induces light avoidance behavior such as squinting and looking away from the source of glare. 11,13,16 Scotomatic glare, also known as photostress, involves reduced visual sensitivity following exposure to high illumination which rapidly bleaches and delays the restoration of retinal photopigments. ...
... In addition to reduced visibility at night, exposure to glare caused by the headlight of approaching vehicles significantly impacts driving performance and also increases the risk for RTA occurrences. 14,31 This study showed that the difference between mesopic visual acuities measured with and without a glare source (disability glare index) was a significant predictor of nighttime driver performance following exposure to glare. This finding suggests that the measurement of disability glare index in addition to photopic visual acuity provides an additional indicator for determining one's fitness to drive safely at night. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Glare caused by the headlights of on-coming vehicles risk safe driving at night. The study aimed to determine the relationship between glare exposure and nighttime driving performance among commercial drivers in Ghana. Methods This cross-sectional study involved commercial drivers with complaints of nighttime driving difficulties (N = 80; mean age = 41.5 ± 11.1 years). A questionnaire was used to investigate nighttime driving performance following glare exposure. We measured contrast sensitivity and visual acuity under photopic conditions. With an experimental setup in a mesopic setting, we measured visual acuity with and without glare exposure. The difference between the two mesopic visual acuities was quantified as disability glare index. With the same setup, photostress recovery time was also measured. Regression analyses were used to determine the relationship between nighttime driving performance score and the measures taken in both photopic and mesopic settings. Results The average nighttime driving performance score was 47.8 ± 17.5. Driving performance was negatively correlated with all variables (R = –0.87 to –0.30, all p < .01), except contrast sensitivity (R = 0.74, p < .01). A multiple linear regression showed that the model with all variables explained 83.8% of the variance, but only disability glare index was a significant predictor of nighttime driving performance following glare exposure (standardized B = –0.61, p < .01). Conclusion Our results show that the change in mesopic visual acuities following glare can predict nighttime driving performance. This measure can be incorporated into the assessment of driving fitness by licensing departments to evaluate whether a person can drive safely at night amidst glare exposure.
... Xenon HID headlights, for example, produce two-to-three times more luminous flux than conventional Tungsten Halogen headlights, which makes them more energy-efficient and also allows them to project light further down the road. 47 However, the higher levels of brightness, which have the advantage of increasing illumination on the roadway, also have the disadvantage of increasing glare for oncoming drivers, 51 generating increased glare complaints, particularly from middle-aged and older drivers. 47 More recently, adaptive headlights, have been introduced in some vehicles and can change their aim and/or modify their output in response to traffic and roadway conditions. ...
... 47 However, the higher levels of brightness, which have the advantage of increasing illumination on the roadway, also have the disadvantage of increasing glare for oncoming drivers, 51 generating increased glare complaints, particularly from middle-aged and older drivers. 47 More recently, adaptive headlights, have been introduced in some vehicles and can change their aim and/or modify their output in response to traffic and roadway conditions. 48 However, while it has been suggested that adaptive headlights have the potential to significantly reduce nighttime crash rates, 49 the benefits have not been fully evaluated. ...
Article
Purpose: Nighttime driving is dangerous and is one of the most challenging driving situations for most drivers. Fatality rates are higher at night than in the day when adjusted for distances travelled, particularly for crashes involving pedestrians and cyclists. Although there are multiple contributory factors, the low light levels at night are believed to be the major cause of collisions with pedestrians and cyclists at night, most likely due to their reduced visibility. Understanding the visibility problems involved in nighttime driving is thus critical, given the increased risk to road safety. Recent findings: This review discusses research that highlights key differences in the nighttime road environment compared to the day and how this affects visual function and driving performance, together with an overview of studies investigating how driver age and visual status affect nighttime driving performance. Research that has focused on the visibility of vulnerable road users at nighttime (pedestrians and cyclists) is also included. Summary: Collectively, the research evidence suggests that visual function is reduced under the mesopic lighting conditions of night driving and that these effects are exacerbated by increasing age and visual impairment. Light and glare from road lighting and headlights have significant impacts on vision and night driving and these effects are likely to change with evolving technologies, such as LED streetlighting and headlights. Research also highlights the importance of the visibility of vulnerable road users at night and the role of retroreflective clothing in the 'biomotion' configuration for improving their conspicuity and hence safety.
... The dazzling effect impairs vision and potentially disturbs concentration, which translates in a number of accidents caused by it. The literature is abundant with studies regarding the dazzle effect on car drivers created by High-Intensity Discharge (HID) or Halogen headlights as well as the dazzle effect on airplane pilots created by lasers or sunlight and consequent disruptive effect on vehicle operation capability [3][4][5][6][7][8]. Dazzling is a real danger and not currently adequately addressed. ...
... Part of the problem of reducing, or even eliminating potential dazzling of vehicle operators lies in the current way to access dazzle. The bulk of studies currently available are relying on subjective measures of the dazzle effect [5][6][7][9][10][11]. However, subjective measures might not reflect real concentration and vision impairment [12]. ...
... Visual changes are strongly correlated with the safety of drivers and pedestrians. Glare slightly reduces cognitive ability for objects as the driver's contrast sensitivity decreases [15][16][17]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Retinal damage caused by blue light can result in glare, decreased visual acuity, and accelerated macular degeneration. In clinical practice, blue light-blocking glasses, such as driving glasses, are used to block blue light effectively. This study was aimed at measuring light transmittance to analyze the blue light-blocking efficiencies of blue light-blocking and driving spectacle lenses manufactured with tinting, coating, and only materials and at distinguishing the difference between the two spectacle lenses. Methods: Blue light-blocking and driving spectacle lenses used to measure light transmittance were manufactured with tinting (blue light blocking lenses by tinting or “BTL” and driving spectacle lenses by tinting or “DTL,” respectively), coating (blue light blocking lenses by coating or “BCL” and driving spectacle lenses by coating or “DCL,” respectively), and only materials (blue light blocking lenses by material or “BML” and driving spectacle lenses by material or “DML,” respectively). Results: Compared to BTL, DTL had a significantly greater decrease in the light transmission efficiency for visible and blue lights (P < 0.05). The blue light hazard function was lower for BML and DML than for conventional coating lenses in both visible and blue lights, although without significant differences between visible and blue lights (P > 0.05). Conclusions: The blue light-blocking spectacle lenses had the highest blue light-blocking efficiency when manufactured with tinting, coating, and only materials, in order. With DML, the blue light-blocking efficiency was lower compared to DTL but higher compared to DCL. Therefore, DML could provide a balanced glare control and clear retinal image overall.
... Patients may be handicapped by DG and a prolonged photostress recovery time [40], which was on average 4 s and nearly 0.5 s longer after the higher-intensity xenon glare than with the relatively lower-intensity halogen glare in intermediate DLS patients. These results are well below the average 13.14 s reported after a higher-glare illumination of 320 lx and longer (10-s) exposure [13], indicating that photostress recovery time is related directly to glare intensity and duration. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: There is a lack of evidence about the exact deterioration of visual function associated with the age-related natural changes in the lens, particularly in intermediate (stage-2) dysfunctional lens syndrome (DLS). Standard photopic visual acuity and contrast sensitivity tests may not show the visual worsening in daily life activities, such as oncoming vehicle headlights at night. The purpose of this study was to analyze visual function under different conditions and glare sources in stage-2 DLS. Methods: Forty patients over 49 years of age with initial bilateral lens opacification (Lens Opacities Classification System III [LOCS-III] scores up to 3), best-corrected visual acuity of 20/25 or better, and no ocular disease were evaluated. Binocular photopic and mesopic contrast sensitivity (CS) with/without halogen and xenon increasing glare sources were analyzed. Mesopic disability glare (MDG) was calculated as the difference between mesopic CS with/without the glare source. Results: The median logarithmic CS (logCS) values were lower under mesopic conditions (1.05) than under photopic illumination (1.65; P < 0.001). Halogen and xenon glare further decreased mesopic CS (both, median logCS 0.75, P < 0.001). The mean MDG was 0.31 ± 0.10 log units for halogen glare and 0.33 ± 0.09 log units for xenon glare. The mesopic CS and MDG were not associated with any photopic test. The mesopic CS with glare but not photopic CS or mesopic CS was correlated with the LOCS-III scores. The best association was provided by MDG, which showed a pooled correlation with LOCS-III nuclear opalescence (r = 0.411, P < 0.001) and cortical scores (r = 0.226, P = 0.04). Conclusion: The mesopic CS under a glare source is an independent early indicator of visual impairment in stage-2 DLS patients, and appears to be substantial. Furthermore, the MDG is more sensitive than photopic and mesopic CS for evaluating patients with initial phacosclerosis. Surgeons should consider this in the decision-making process of the correct timing for lens surgery.
... Glare can be categorized into Discomfort Glare and Disability Glare. Discomfort glare is defined as 'glare that causes discomfort without necessarily impairing the vision of objects.' Discomfort glare causes annoyance, fatigue, or pain without necessarily affecting visibility and can lead to distraction (Bullough, Fu, & Van Derlofske, 2002;Mainster & Timberlake, 2003). Disability Glare is defined as 'glare that impairs the vision of objects without necessarily causing discomfort.' Disability Glare is caused by the diffusion of bright light inside the eye (Miller & Benedek, 1973;van den Berg et al. (René) van Rijn, L. J., Kaper-Bongers, R., Vonhoff, D. J. J., Völker-Dieben, H. J. J., Grabner, G., Gamer, D. , 2009) creating a more or less important veil, or disk halo around the glare source, that reduces retinal contrast across the visual field. ...
Article
Introduction: The present study proposes to validate the Driver Ecological Glare Test (DEGT), a test developed to measure the benefit of a headlight glare Advanced Driver Assistance System (ADAS), by comparing it to a laboratory glare test. Method: Twenty-four participants, aged from 55 to 70 years, were recruited to complete a visual examination, including monocular halo size measurement for both eyes using Vision Monitor device (MonCv3; Metrovision). An on-field evaluation took place at night at the UTAC CERAM test track to obtain disability glare measures using the DEGT. Results: A significant correlation was found between the two glare tests and Bland-Altman analysis reveals a good agreement with a bias of 73.7 arcmin between the halo size measurements obtained from the DEGT and Vision Monitor. The results of the present study demonstrate that the DEGT is a valid method to test halo size and is adapted to evaluate the benefits of an antiglare device for drivers in an ecological situation.
Article
Full-text available
Background/Aims Driving especially at night is a visually demanding task. Long-time outcome of cataract surgery in drivers is important to study, as many patients live for decades after surgery. The purpose of this study is to longitudinally investigate visual function in active car drivers, 20 years after cataract surgery. Methods From a population-based, prospective, cohort of cataract surgery patients, initiated in 1997–98, 114 of the 133 surviving patients were included. Preoperatively, postoperatively 5, 10, 15 and 20 years after surgery, the patients answered a visual function questionnaire including driving status and difficulty. Habitual visual acuity, best corrected visual acuity (BCVA), and low contrast acuity (LCVA) 10% and 2.5% were measured. Results The driving difficulties in daylight were almost absent after surgery and did not change over 20 years. Nighttime driving was more difficult and declined longitudinally after surgery, p=0.013, but were at 20 years still less than before cataract surgery. Patients with better BCVA experienced less difficulties driving in darkness, p=0.005. Self-reported problems with glare were significantly associated with BCVA of the better-seeing eye, LCVA 10% and LCVA 2.5% (p=0.046, p=0.033, and 0.024 respectively). Self-reported difficulties with seeing in low-contrast conditions were also significantly associated with BCVA, p=0.004. Conclusion Twenty years after cataract surgery, most active drivers have no or minor visual functional problems during driving in daytime. Difficulties in nighttime driving are more common and increase significantly over time. Twenty years after surgery, all current drivers had still better subjective ability to drive, compared with before surgery.
Chapter
Full-text available
Lighting has the power to illuminate and enhance our experience within the built environment. The light that enables people to travel around their neighbourhood or their city; the light which they see themselves and their neighbourhood under. Research into the effects of urban lighting on behaviour, environmental psychology and social interaction is developing at a rapid rate. Yet, despite the affect it has on our daily lives, the practical application of this research is a relatively untapped resource. There has been a persistent trend to use lighting as a tool for urban regeneration and many major urban lighting projects around the country are underway but there is more that can be done on a variety of scales. This book explores the needs and experiences of people at night and how these can be addressed by public lighting. It will give readers the confidence to develop more sophisticated lighting plans and add value to their projects. Case studies provide in-depth analysis of real-life projects and will help the reader to understand lighting designers’ own experiences, including post-installation observations. Written in an accessible style by an array of experts, this is an essential book for practitioners, academics and students alike, that will enable you to put the research in to practice and develop better lighting for better places. Features contributions from Navaz Davoudian, Elettra Bordonaro, Joanne Entwistle, Don Slater, Karolina Zielinska-Dabkowska, Jemima Unwin, Isabel Kelly, Dan Lister and Emily Dufner. .................................................................................................................................................The book can be purchased here: https://ribabookshops.com/item/urban-lighting-for-people-evidence-based-lighting-design-for-the-built-environment/40315/ .................................................................................................................................................Chapter 2 Abstract: .................................................................................................................................................Awareness of the significance and benefits of properly designed urban lighting masterplans has been growing since the early 21 st century. There are many factors driving this notable change, such as developments in lighting technology, energy conservation, city branding design and economics, environmental impacts, human health and wellbeing, and people-oriented sociological aspects. As the profession of ‘independent urban lighting designer’ is relatively new and still not fully recognised in certain parts of the world, it is essential to establish clear definitions relating to urban lighting masterplans that describe their nature, scope and meaning. In this chapter, with the help of graphics and diagrams, all the necessary steps in the design process and the methodologies used will be introduced. This will make it easier to inform clients, urban planners and other designers about the established approach, and facilitate sharing the work of projects and continuing professional development by disseminating existing research and practical knowledge in this new field. However, none of the above can be achieved if there is no proper process of collaboration in place between stakeholders and no common aim to create a magnificent piece of the city for its users to gather in. Collaboration is necessary in order to achieve creative results as well as to help generate appropriate, original lighting solutions for urban areas outside daylight hours. The intention of this chapter is that more and more city representatives, developers, urban planners/designers, architects, engineers and other members of the design team responsible for designing city lighting will understand that creating appropriate night-time illumination is a complex task, bringing with it enormous environmental and social responsibility. In order to come up with an approach that can minimise any negative issues and take into consideration all aspects of this multifaceted branch of design, cities must devise and fully implement urban lighting masterplans.
Article
Mapping knowledge domain (MKD) is an important application of visualization technology in Bibliometrics, which has been extensively applied in psychology, medicine, and information science. In this paper we conduct a systematic analysis of the development trend on road safety studies based on the Science Citation Index Expanded (SCIE) and Social Sciences Citation Index (SSCI) articles published between 2000 and 2018 using the MKD software tools VOSviewer and Sci2 Tool. Based on our analysis, we first present the annual numbers of articles, origin countries, main research organizations and groups as well as the source journals on road safety studies. We then report the collaborations among the main research organizations and groups using co-authorship analysis. Furthermore, we adopt the document co-citation analysis, keywords co-occurrence analysis, and burst detection analysis to visually explore the knowledge bases, topic distribution, research fronts and research trends on road safety studies. The proposed approach based on the visualized analysis of MKD can be used to establish a reference information and research basis for the application and development of methods in the domain of road safety studies. In particular, our results show that the knowledge bases (classical documents) of road safety studies in the last two decades have focused on five major areas of "Crash Frequency Data Analysis", "Driver Behavior Questionnaire", "Safety in Numbers for Walkers and Bicyclists", "Road Traffic Injury and Prevention", and "Driving Speed and Road Crashes". Among the research topics, the five dominant clusters are "Causation and Injury Severity Analysis of Road Accidents", "Epidemiologic Study and Prevention of Road Traffic Injury", "Intelligent Transportation System and Active Safety", "Young drivers' driving behavior and psychology", and "Older drivers' psychological and physiological characteristics". Finally, the burst keywords in research trends include Cycling, Intelligent Transportation Systems, and Distraction.
Thesis
Full-text available
There have been many studies of the threshold energy required for laser induced retinal damage. However, exposure to a visible laser which does not produce irreversible retinal damage could still result in substantial visual losses, albeit temporary. If any such deficits were severe enough to disrupt visual performance significantly then both the safety and efficiency of personnel could be compromised. The aim of this research was to investigate the transient effects of high intensity laser light on visual function in healthy human volunteers. Particular issues chosen for study were: visual recovery following exposure to flashes of laser light; visual function during repetitive pulse light exposures; and the effect of intervening optical materials on laser induced glare. The extent of laser glare and subsequent visual recovery was shown to depend on a number of variables. These included the parameters of the laser exposure, the presence of intervening optical materials, and the visual task under consideration. The results of the experiments were linked in a conceptual framework which can be used to predict the ultimate effect of laser exposure on visual performance.
Article
All of science springs from the observation of nature. In this classic book, the late Marcel Minnaert accompanies the reader on a tour of nature's light and color and reveals the myriad phenomena that may be observed outdoors with no more than a pair of sharp eyes and an enquiring mind. From the intriguing shape of the dapples beneath a tree on a sunny day, to rainbows, mirages, and haloes, to the colors of liquid, ice, and the sky, to the appearance of the sun, moon, planets, and stars - Minnaert describes and explains them all in clear language accessible to the layman. This volume includes 80 new photographs, over half in color, illustrating many of the phenomena - ordinary and exotic - discussed in the book. Most of the new photos are by Pekka Parviainen, the renowned Finnish nature photographer.
Article
Drs. Fine and Yanoff have compiled a handy textbook and atlas on ocular histology to celebrate the 50th anniversary of the establishment of the Ophthalmic Pathology Laboratory at the Armed Forces Institute of Pathology.
Article
Preface to the Second Edition.Preface to the First Edition. Acknowledgments for the Second Edition. Acknowledgments for the First Edition. LIGHT AND COLOR. Some Fundamentals: Color, Light, and Interactions. COLOR INVOLVING VIBRATIONS AND SIMPLE EXCITATIONS. Color Produced by Incandescence. Color Produced by Gas Excitations. Color Produced by Vibrations and Rotations. COLOR INVOLVING LIGAND FIELD EFFECTS. Color Caused by Transition Metals in a Ligand Field. COLOR INVOLVING MOLECULAR ORBITALS. Color in Organic Molecules. Charge Transfer Color. COLOR INVOLVING BAND THEORY. Color in Metals and Semiconductors. Color Centers. COLOR INVOLVING GEOMETRICAL AND PHYSICAL OPTICS. Dispersive Refraction and Polarization. Scattering and Nonlinear Effects. Interference and Diffraction. COLOR--RELATED TOPICS. Colorants of Many Types. Vision, Luminescence, Lasers, and Related Topics. Appendix A: Units, Chromaticity, and Color Order Systems. Appendix B: Incandescence Equations. Appendix C: Atoms and Simple Molecules. Appendix D: Crystal Fields, Ligand Fields, and Molecular Orbitals. Appendix E: Band Theory. Appendix F: Prism, Thin Film, and Layer Diffraction Grating. Appendix G: Recommendations for Additional Reading. Index.
Article
Elderly drivers often experience disability glare at night from the headlights of oncoming vehicles. To assess the effect of glare from vehicle headlights on visual performance for seeing moving targets, experiments were performed at night on a dimly lit road with observers seated in a stationary motor car viewing a computer-generated stimulus display at a distance of 23 m (the stopping distance for 50 kph). The display was set 2 m to the side of a second stationary car whose position on the road was that of an oncoming vehicle with respect to the observer, The headlights of the observer's car were on low-beam while those of the opposing car were switched off (control condition), on low-beam or on high-beam. Experiments were performed using mean display luminances of 50 cd/m(2) and 0.5 cd/m(2). Spatial contrast sensitivity functions for the directional discrimination of drifting (8 Hz) sinusoidal gratings were measured using three different viewing conditions: normal vision (binocular visual acuity (BVA) = 6/6); blurred vision (BVA = 6/9-); and simulated intraocular lens opacities (BVA = 6/6-). The data were fitted with an exponential function, which was extrapolated to 100% contrast to estimate dynamic visual acuity. The results show that simulated lens opacities, which have little or no effect on standard day time measures of visual acuity, have a marked effect on night-time measures of contrast sensitivity for moving targents. Taking into account the average luminance of objects lit by road lighting, we estimate that high-beam glare reduces maximum contrast sensitivity by an order of magnitude in persons affected by mild lens opacities, giving a dynamic acuity of 1.0 c/deg (6/180 Snellen equivalent) or less. From this and other studies we argue that there is now a strong case for the introduction of vehicle-licensing sight re-testing at regular intervals in the UK. In addition, we suggest that vehicle-licensing authorities consider the feasibility of introducing sight tests under night-time driving conditions.
Article
The purpose of the Committee on Ophthalmic Procedures Assessment is to evaluate on a scientific basis new and existing ophthalmic tests, devices, and procedures for their safety, efficacy, clinical effectiveness and appropriate uses. Evaluations include examination of available literature, epidemiological analyses when appropriate, and complication of opinions from recognized experts and other interested parties. After appropriate reviews by all contributors, including legal counsel, assessments are submitted to the Academy's Board of Directors for consideration as official Academy policy.