ArticlePDF Available

Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males

The Royal Society
Proceedings of the Royal Society B
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Recent evidence suggests that certain features on the human face indicate hormonal levels during growth, and that women judge the attractiveness of potential partners based on the appearance of these features. One entrenched notion is male facial features that are affected by testosterone are used as direct cues in mate preference. Testosterone may be particularly revealing as it is purported to be an honest indicator of male fitness. Increased testosterone may impose an immunocompetence handicap on the bearer and only the best males can carry this handicap. To date, tests of this theory have been indirect, and have relied on digital manipulations that represent unrealistic continuums of masculine and feminine faces. We provide a much more direct test by manipulating digitally male faces to mimic known shape variation, caused by varying levels of testosterone through puberty. We produced a continuum of faces that ranged from low to high levels of testosterone in male faces and asked women to choose the points on the continuum that appeared most attractive and most physically dominant. Our data indicate that high testosterone faces reveal dominance. However, there is no evidence of directional selection for increased (or decreased) testosterone in terms of attractiveness to the opposite sex. We discuss the relevance and applicability of evolutionary interpretations of our data and, contrary to predictions, provide evidence of stabilizing selection acting on testosterone through mate preferences.
Content may be subject to copyright.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165
, 2285-2289269 2002 Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
John P. Swaddle and Gillian W. Reierson
attractiveness in human males
Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not
References
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/269/1507/2285#related-urls
Article cited in:
Email alerting service
hereright-hand corner of the article or click
Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions go to: Proc. R. Soc. Lond. BTo subscribe to
This journal is © 2002 The Royal Society
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Received
9 May 2002
Accepted
15 August 2002
Published online
9 October 2002
Testosterone increases perceived dominance but
not attractiveness in human males
John P. Swaddle
*
and Gillian W. Reierson
Biology Department, College of William & Ma ry, Williamsburg, VA 23197-879, USA
Recent evidence suggests that certain f eatures on the human face indicate hormonal levels during growth,
and that women judge the attractiveness of potential partners based on the appearance of these features.
One entrenched notion is male facial features that are affe cted by testosterone are used as direct cues in
mate preference. Testosterone may be particularly revealing as it is purported to be an honest indicator
of male tness. Increased testosterone may impose an immunocompetence handicap on the bearer and
only the best males can carry this hand icap. To date, tes ts of this theory have been indirect, and have
relied on digital manipulations that represent unrealistic continuums of masculine and feminine faces. We
provide a much more direct test by manipulating digitally male faces to mimic known shape variation,
caused by varying levels of testosterone through puberty. W e prod uced a continuum of faces that ranged
from low to high levels of testosterone in male faces and asked women to choose the points on the
continuum that appeared most attractive and most physically domin ant. Our data indicate that high testos -
terone faces reveal dominance. However, there is no evidence of directional selection for increased (or
decreased) testosterone in terms of attractiveness to the opposite sex. We discuss the relevance and appli-
cability of evolutionary interpretations of our data and, contrary to predictions, provide evidence of stabil-
izing selection acting on testosterone through mate preferences.
Keywords: sexual selection;
Homo sapiens
; immunocompetence; handicap; mate preference
1. IN TRODUCTION
Several hypotheses suggest that human faces display mar-
kers of hormones, and that these cues affect judgements
of facial attractiveness ( Johnston & Franklin 1993; Perrett
et al.
1998; Thornhill & Gangestad 1999; Johnston
et al.
2001) and dominance (Mazur
et al.
1994; Mazur & Booth
1998). In particular, evolutionary psychologists have gen-
erated theories to pred ict that levels of testosterone
re ected in male facial features are (or were) important in
generating sexual dimorphism in humans, as well as
directly affecting mate preferences (Perrett
et al.
1998;
Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Thornhill & Gangestad 1999;
Johnston
et al.
2001). It has been suggested that facial
testosterone may be an honest indicator of male tne ss
(Thornhill & Gangestad 1999), as increased testosterone
can impose an immunocompetence handicap on the
bearer (see discussions in Grossman 1985; Hasselquist
et
al.
1999; Duckworth
et al.
2001) and only the best males
can carry this handicap (Zahavi 1975; Hamilton & Zuk
1982).
Existing tests of testosterone-related hypotheses of
human attractiveness have focused on producing femi-
nine–masculine axes of facial shape in digital represen-
tations (by computer graphics manipulations) and
monitoring people’s responses to these faces (Perrett
et al.
1998; Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001). In
some cases , more masculine, male faces are preferred
(Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001), whereas
in others more feminine male faces are judged as attractive
(Perrett
et al.
1998). Part of this variation appears to be
correlated with the stage of ovarian cycle of women judg-
*
Author for correspondence (jpswad@wm.edu).
Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B (2002)
269
, 2285–2289 2285 Ó 2002 The Royal Society
DOI 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165
ing the faces (Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001). However, there is no evidence to indicate that
these uctuations in female preferences affect actual part-
ner choice.
Previous experiments have also provided fairly un natu-
ral, and rather indirect, tests of the testosterone-related
hypotheses. First, there are many differences between
masculine and feminine faces, which are not accounted
for by testosterone alone (Perrett
et al.
1998). Hence, the
results of these studies are dif cult to interpret in terms
of testosterone expression in the male face. Second, the
faces produc ed through such manipulations result in male
facial shapes outside a natural male distribution (Penton-
Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001), and are commonly
presented as faces without hairlines or necks (Perrett
et
al.
1998). Preferences for face shapes from such arti cial
distributions may not re ect preferences shown in society,
as the facial variation in normal male human populations
can be very different.
A more direct approach to testing testosterone-related
hypotheses is to produce facial variation that re ects
quanti ed variation in te stosterone levels. Therefore, we
manipulated the shape of male faces to mimic growth dif-
ferences resulting from varying levels of testosterone dur-
ing puberty. Women viewed these faces and made
judgements of facial attractiveness and dominance.
According to previou s reports, we predicted that women
should nd face s with higher testosterone expression most
attractive ( Thornhill & Gangestad 1999).
2. MATERIAL AND METHODS
We took digital photographs of 21 male Caucasians’ faces in
a (right-side) pro le and face-on orientation to the camera.
Models were 18–21 years old, had short hair, lacked beards or
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
2286 J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reie rson Testosterone and facial attractiveness
Table 1. Approximate relative trait size changes (i.e. distances between two points) resulting from w arp manipulations for the
100% high and 100% low testosterone treatments, for pro le and face-on photographs of male faces.
(Facial characters were determined from anthropometric de nitions (Farkas 1981) and their pos ition was estimated on each male
face. Points used were: nasion (N) midpoint of both nasal root and nasofrontal suture; pogonion (POG) anterior midpoint of
the chin; gonion (GO) most lateral point of the mandibular angle; menton (ME) lowest point on the mandible; ans (ANS)
approximated by the posterior extremity of the nose; articulare (ART) estimated by the posterior and ventral atta chment of the
ear; sella (S) estimated by the anterior and ventral attachment of the ear.)
pro le face-on
trait 100% high 100% low 100% high 100% low
N–ANS
1
0.01
2
0.01
1
0.01
2
0.01
N–ME
1
0.01
2
0.01
1
0.01
2
0.01
ART–ANS
1
0.01
2
0.01 0 0
ART–POG
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.02
2
0.02
ART–GO
1
0.04
2
0.04
1
0.04
2
0.04
S–ME
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.02
2
0.02
ART–ME
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.02
2
0.02
GO–POG
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.01
2
0.01
moustaches, were not wearing jewellery, and were told to adopt
a neutral facial expression while sitting for the photographs.
Photographs were taken in standardized lighting conditions
against a common background.
We compared how facial s tructure changes with natural levels
of testosterone during puberty, and in delayed puberty boys
treated with low-dose testosterone (Verdonck 1997; V erdonck
et al.
1999) to create vectors of facial trait changes that represent
realistic variation in plasma testosterone in uencing facial bone
growth elds (Enlow 1990; Silveira
et al.
1992). The relative
changes of the facial traits we manipulated are listed in table 1.
Brie y, face height increased and the lower jaw became larger
with increased testosterone. These manipulations were speci -
cally de signed to isolate the effects of te stosterone on facial skel-
etal characters, providing a more direct test of the effec ts of
testosterone on perception of the face.
We used the warp functi on in Gryphon Software’s Morph
program to alter the shape of male faces according to the trait
size c hanges listed in table 1. We also digitized 10 points around
the eyes, ve on the nose, and ve on the lips to ensure that this
part of the face did not change under the warp manipulation.
The warp function moves the reference points to a speci ed pos-
ition without altering colour or tone ( gure 1). In addition to
producing the high and low testosterone treatments, we pro-
duced nine, equally spaced, intermediate warps between the
control and high testosterone faces, and between the control and
low testosterone faces. This resul ted in 2 1 representations of a
single face that ran ged from the low treatment to the high treat-
ment with equal warp differences betwe en all images, and where
the median face was the control (i.e. non-manipulated face).
The sequence of 21 representations was arranged in ascending
or descending order in a PowerPoint presentation, with one
image on each slide. For half the faces the rst slide contained
the lowest testosterone treatment; for the remaining faces the
rst slide was the highest testosterone treatment. Each slide also
displayed a sequential number so that viewers could tell us
which slide they chose.
Thirty females, age 18–21 years old, viewed each of the 42
sequences on a computer screen. Subjects were asked to choose
the most sexually attractive, and most physically dominant look-
ing, face in each sequence. One of the experimenters advanced
(or went back through) the slides at a regular pace of one slide
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
approximately every 0.5 s. In ad dition, viewe rs instructed the
experimenter to advance, reverse, and stop the sequence when-
ever the viewer wished. The order of male faces viewed by
females was random ized, except that all the pro le faces were
presented in sequence, as were the face-on presentations. Half
of the viewers saw the pro le sequences rst, and vice versa.
Viewers were paid for their participation. If a viewer recognized
one of the male faces, she informed the researchers and the data
for that face were discarded.
3. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
The frames chosen by females as being most attractive,
for both face-on and pro le views, were faces with testos-
terone expression very similar to that of the non-manipu-
lated face (paired
t
-tests of the most attractive versus the
control face: face-on,
t
2 0
= 1.94,
p
= 0.067; pro le,
t
2 0
= 1.86,
p
= 0.078). The frequency diagram of most
attractive frame choice illustrates that women’s prefer-
ences centre on existing levels of testosterone in men’s
faces ( gure 2). S uch distributions are consistent with sta-
bilizing selection acting on testos terone expression in
men’s faces rather th an the previously proposed direc-
tional selection for reduced or increased testosterone
(Perrett
et al.
1998; Johnston
et al.
2001).
It is important to point out that individual female raters
used the entire range of faces for attractivenes s choices.
For face-on views, the mean ± s.e. (among-rater) range of
frame chosen was 18.63 ± 0.54 (out of a possible 21) for
face-on views, and 19.50 ± 2.54 for pro le views. This
indicates that females did not feel constrained to only
using the central portion of the animation sequence s, and
that our result is not an artefact of a rater central tendency
independent of attractiveness judgements.
In order to explore further the effects of testosterone on
facial attractivenes s, we calculated an index of testosterone
in non-manipulated male faces (the ratio of AR T–POG
length to N–S length, where a high value indicates higher
facial testosterone expression; see table 1 for de nitions)
and examined whether natural variation in testosterone
affected choice of frames. Linear measurements were
taken directly from the digital photographs of pro le faces.
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Testosterone and facial attractiveness J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reierson 2287
frame 1
low
frame 11
control
frame 21
high
(i)
(ii)
(iii)
(i)
(ii)
(iii)
Figure 1. The extremes of the manipulations p roduced for (
a
(i)–(iii)) face-on and (
b
(i)–(iii)) pro le views of the same face.
Low, low testosterone treatment; control, unmanipulated face; high, high testosterone treatment.
Natural levels of facial testosterone were negatively corre-
lated with choice of attractive frame (face-on: Pearson
r
=
2
0.636,
n
= 21,
p
= 0.0011; pro le:
r
=
2
0.710,
n
= 21,
p
= 0.000 15). Inspection of the numerical values
of these correlations indicated that the most preferred ver-
sions of naturally high testosterone faces were those that
reduced apparent testosterone. Whereas the most pre-
ferred representations of naturally low testosterone faces
were those that increased apparent testosterone. The net
effect of such pre ferences is a convergent selection press-
ure on average testosterone expression across the popu-
lation, which provides further evidence of stabilizing
selection on tes tosterone. Although these correlations are
strong, we need to emphasize that there is a possibility
that raters’ preferences could alter with their oestrus cycle
(cf. Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001). In
further studies, it would be interesting to investigate
whether females preferences for facial testosterone change
systematically according to the possibility of conception.
The choice of the most dominant frame was very differ-
ent to the choice of the most attractive frame. In agree-
ment with previous evidence (Perrett
et al.
1998; Johnston
et al.
2001), the most dominant frames were those with a
high degree of testosterone expression ( gure 2). This pat-
tern he lps to co n rm that our manipulation was realistic,
as links betwee n testosterone and male aggression and
dominance are generally well supported (Mazur &
Booth 1998).
It was also evident th at choices made by viewers when
judging males face-on were different to when males were
in pro le. Females chose faces signi cantly higher in tes-
tosterone as most attractive when viewing a face in pro le,
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
compared with the same male face-on (
t
2 0
= 3.16,
p
= 0.0049). Conversely, females chose faces signi cantly
lower in testosterone as most dominant for pro le, versus
face-on, views of males (
t
2 0
= 3.65,
p
= 0.0016). These dif-
ferences clearly demo nstrate that female judgement of a
male face changes with viewing angle. This implies that
further experiments need to account for three- dimensional
viewing of faces if re searchers are going to document
realistic relations between facial characters and socially (or
evolutionarily) important parameters. However, in the
context of our experiment, we observed the same general
relationships between attractiveness (or dominance) and
facial testosterone expression in pro le and face-on.
A recent resurgence in adaptive explanations of human
behaviour and morphology has stimulated interest in
whether researchers can demonstrate evolutionarily
important relations hips through preference and rating
experiments (Perrett
et al.
1998; Scheib
et al.
1999; John-
ston
et al.
2001). To demonstrate a current evolutionary
effect a research programme must meet certain criteria. In
the context of this study, we would need to demonstrate
evidence of selection pres sure acting on traits, and heri-
table variation in those traits. In terms of the former, o ur
data could indicate stabilizing selection if the re is a link
between differential reproductive success and rating of
facial attractiveness. There is evidence that facial attract-
iveness positively affects the ways in which others treat
them in many social contexts (Kalick 1988; Zebrowitz
1997). However, links between facial attractiveness and
health or reproductive success are equivocal or non-
existent (Kalick
et al.
1998; Shackelford & L arsen 1999;
Rhodes
et al.
2001). Women may place less importance
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
2288 J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reie rson Testosterone and facial attractiveness
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
1
3
5
7
9 11 13 15 17 19 21
frame chosen
frequency frequency
a
)
b
)
Figure 2. (
a
) Frequency histogram for frame choice of most
attractive (open bars) and most dominant ( lled bars) frames
for face-on views; (
b
) and for pro le v iews of male faces. In
both distributions, the choice of dominant frame represented
a much higher degree of testosterone expression than the
choice of attractive frame (face-on,
t
20
=
11.75,
p
,
0.001;
pro le,
t
20
=
3.05,
p
=
0.0064).
on physical attractiveness when choosing a mate than men
do (Waynforth 2001). This implies a low probability that
male facial attractiveness will relate to reproductive suc-
cess or tness. However, it does not preclude that attract-
iveness was related to reproductive success at some time
in our evolutionary history. There is some indication that
increased facial dominance is associated with increased
sexual activity in you ng men (Mazur
et al.
1994), but
increased sexual activity does not nec essarily result in
increased numbers of offspring (Mueller & Mazur 1998).
Additionally, extreme dominance can negatively in uence
reproductive success (Mueller & Mazur 1998).
In terms of the ‘heritability c riteria, there is some e vi-
dence of signi c ant heritability of the male facial traits we
assessed (upper estimates range from 10% to 36%
heritability) (Enlow 1990) and of circulating testosterone
levels in men (upper estimates of
ca
. 60% heritability)
(Harris
et al.
1998). Overall, interpreting our experiment
and those using similar methodologies (Perrett
et al.
1998;
Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Scheib
et al.
1999; Johnston
et
al.
2001) in terms of evolutionary processes is problem-
atic, and generally unsupported by empirical evidence. It
is more likely that judgements of particular facial shapes
are mediated by cultural norms and individual decision-
making (Tooby & Cosmides 1989).
Even if some researchers insist on evolutionary
interpretations, we cannot nd evidence for directional
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
selection to red uce or increase testosterone through female
preferences. Previous studies that have documented
female preference s for masculine ( Johnston
et al.
2001) or
feminine (Perrett
et al.
1998) male faces cann ot be inter-
preted in terms of the effects of testosterone alone. Other
hormonal differences between the sexes must (in part)
explain previously d ocumented preferences. It may be that
selection acting on oestrogen and on female faces may
equally explain human evolutionary changes (Thornhill &
Grammer 1999). The comparative literature has demon-
strated that both sexes can be the target of directional sex-
ual selection and that both sexes change through
evolutionary time, resulting in dimorphism (Karubian &
Swaddle 2001). Our data also suggest t hat social domi-
nance may underlie the evolution of human sexual dimor-
phism, as increased testosterone is positively associated
with perceived dominance. However, increased perceived
dominance does not enhance male attractiveness.
The authors thank Adam Rubenstein and three anonymous
reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft, as well as
the 21 male and 30 female students of William & Mary who
agreed to take part in this study. J.P.S. was supported by the
NSF (IBN-0133795) and the Thomas F. and Kate Miller
Jeffress Memorial Trust. G.W.R. was supported by the
Howard Hughes Medical Institute.
REFERENCES
Duckworth, R. A., Mendonca, M. T. & Hill, G. E. 2001 A
condition dependent link between testosterone and disease
resistance in the house nch.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B
268
,
2467–2472. (DOI 10.1098/rspb.2001.1827.)
Enlow, D. H. 1990
Facial growth
. Philadelphia, PA: Har-
court Brace.
Farkas, L. 1981
Anthropometry of the head and face in medicine
.
New York: Elsevier.
Grossman, C. J. 1985 Interactions between the gonadal ster-
oids and the immune system.
Science
227
, 257–261.
Hamilton, W. D. & Zuk, M. 1982 Heritable true tness and
bright birds: a role for parasites?
Science
218
, 384–387.
Harris, J. A., Vernon, P. A. & Boomsma, D. I. 1998 The heri-
tability of testosterone: a study of Dutch adolescent twins
and their parents.
Behav. Genet.
28
, 165–171.
Hasselquist, D., Marsh, J. A., Sherman, P. W . & Wing eld,
J. C. 1999 Is avian humoral immunocompetence suppressed
by testosterone?
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
45
, 167–175.
Johnston, V. S. & Franklin, M. 1993 Is beauty in the eye of
the beholder?
Ethol. Sociobiol.
14
, 183–199.
Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B. & Grammer,
K. 2001 Male facial attractiveness: evidence for hormone-
mediated a daptive design.
Evol. Hum. Behav.
22
, 251–267.
Kalick, S. M. 1988 Physical attractiveness as a status cue.
J.
Exp. Soc. Psychol.
24
, 469–489.
Kalick, S. M., Zebrowitz, L. A., Langlois, J. H. & Johnson,
R. M. 1998 Does human facial attractiveness honestly adver-
tise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question.
Psychol. Sci.
9
, 8–13.
Karubian, J. & Swaddle, J. P. 2001 Selection on females can
create ‘larger males’.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B
268
, 725–728.
(DOI 10.1098/rspb.2000.1407.)
Mazur, A. & Booth, A. 1998 Testosterone and dominance in
men.
Behav. Brain Sci.
21
, 353–397.
Mazur, A., Halpern, C. & Udry, J. R . 1994 Domina nt looking
male teenagers copulate earlier.
Ethol. Sociobiol.
15
, 87–94.
Mueller, U. & Mazur, A. 1998 Reproductive constraints on
dominance competition in male
Homo sapiens
.
Evol. Hum.
Behav.
19
, 387–396.
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Testosterone and facial attractiveness J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reierson 2289
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi,
T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K. & Minamisawa, R. 1999
Menstrual cycle alters face preference.
Nature
399
, 741–742.
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshi-
kawa, S., Burt, D. M., Henzl, S. P., Castles, D. L. & Akam-
atsu, S. 1998 Effects of s exual dimorphism on facial
attractiveness.
Nature
394
, 884–887.
Rhodes, G., Zebrowitz, L. A., Clark, A., Kalick, S. M., High-
tower, A. & McKay, R. 2001 Do facial averageness and sym-
metry signal health ?
Evol. Hum. Behav.
22
, 31–4 6.
Scheib, J. E., Gangestad, S. W. & Thornhill, R. 1999 Facial
attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes.
Proc. R.
Soc. Lond.
B
266
, 1913–1917. (DOI 10.1098/rspb.1999.
0866.)
Shackelford, T. K. & La rsen, R. J. 1999 Facial attractiveness
and physical health.
Evol. Hum. Behav.
20
, 71–76.
Silveira, A. M., Fishman, L. S., Subteiny, J. D. & Kassebaum,
D. K. 1992 Facial growth during adolescence in early, aver-
age and late maturers.
Angle Orthod.
52
, 185–190.
Thornhill, R. & Gangestad, S. W. 1999 Facial attract iveness.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
Trends Cogn. Sci.
3
, 452–460.
Thornhill, R. & Grammer, K. 1999 The body and face of
woman: one ornament tha t signals quality ?
Evol. Hum.
Behav.
20
, 105–120.
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. 1989 Evolutionary psychology and
the generation of culture. Part I: theoretical considerations.
Ethol. Sociobiol.
10
, 29–4 9.
Verdonck, A. 1997
Effect of testosterone on craniofacial growth in
the human and the rat
. Leuven: Katholieke Universiteit te
Leuven.
Verdonck, A., Gaethofs, M., Carels, C. & de Zegher, F. 1999
Effect of low-dose testosterone treatment on craniofacial
growth in boys with delayed puberty.
Eur. J. Orthod.
21
,
137–143.
Waynforth, D. 2001 Mate choice trade-offs and women’s pref-
erence for physically attractive men.
Hum. Nat.
12
, 207–219.
Zahavi, A. 1975 Mate selection: a selection for a handicap.
J.
Theor. Biol.
53
, 205–214.
Zebrowitz, L. A. 1997
Reading faces: windows to the soul?
Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
... The 3D stimuli were imaged in two poses (a frontal and a half profile view). Two views were simultaneously presented to provide more 3D information (e.g., about chin shape and eyebrow structure) and because different views affect attributions differently [52]. Participants were able to transform the faces in increments of 10%, hence there were 30 increments for male faces and 40 for female. ...
... The levels of sexual dimorphism for optimal attractiveness found here are higher than commonly reported although they do align with measures taken with the same type of 3D face stimuli [28]. This may be due to the presentation of face and profile views which can elicit different judgements [52]. Alternatively, some of the textural details that contribute to gender recognition (e.g., a 5 o'clock shadow from a shaved beard may be lost in the formation of stimuli, hence shape cues need amplifying to compensate. ...
... The profile view of the head provides additional information about the neck, chin and cheek structure which may be important for attributions. Indeed, the profile pose of male faces decreases attractiveness and increases apparent dominance [52]. By including both face and half-profile views our results may have greater generality than studies using a single view. ...
Article
Full-text available
Thin and muscular have been characterised as body shape ideals for women and men, respectively, yet each sex misperceives what the other sex desires; women exaggerate the thinness that men like and men exaggerate the muscularity that women like. Body shape ideals align with stereotypic perceptions of femininity in women and masculinity in men. The present study investigates whether misperception of opposite-sex desires extends to femininity/masculinity in facial morphology. We used interactive 3D head models to represent faces varying in sexual dimorphism. White European heterosexual men and women were asked to choose their own and ideal face shape, the ideal shape of a short- and a long-term partner, and the face shape they thought the opposite sex would most like for a short- and a long-term partner. Women overestimated the facial femininity that men prefer in a partner and men overestimated the facial masculinity that women prefer in a partner. The discrepancy between own and ideal sexual dimorphism (an index of appearance dissatisfaction) covaried with by the misperception of what the opposite-sex desires. These results indicate misperception of opposite-sex facial preferences and that mistaken perceptions may contribute to dissatisfaction with own appearance.
... For example, T is positively associated with sIgA in men (Arnocky et al., 2018;Hodges-Simeon et al., 2020). Yet other studies have found null links between T and rated male facial attractiveness (e.g., Swaddle and Reierson, 2002;Neave et al., 2003;Penton-Voak and Chen, 2004), and others have found null links between T, C, and both facial attractiveness and other-rated perceptions of health (Kandrik et al., 2017). Some researchers have suggested that relying on baseline T or C levels may be insufficient, and that the dual hormone hypothesis involving an interaction between high T and low C might be complicit in phenotypic masculinization. ...
... However, it is noteworthy that this finding does conform to that of Rantala et al. (2012), who found the same effect. The overall weak association between hormones and facial attractiveness diverges from a study of women which showed a link between high C and lower facial attractiveness (Rantala et al., 2013), but corresponds with others of male facial attractiveness showing no links with either hormone (Swaddle and Reierson, 2002;Neave et al., 2003;Penton-Voak and Chen, 2004;Kandrik et al., 2017;Kordsmeyer et al., 2019). It has long been assumed in evolutionary psychology that male facial attractiveness is an honest cue of an individual's health and immunocompetence (see Jones et al., 2021 for discussion). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Facial attractiveness has recently been considered an indicator of underlying immunocompetence. However, studies examining this relationship have yielded mixed findings. Previous research suggested that these discrepant findings could be due to the common influence of lifestyle factors upon both rated facial attractiveness and health. Methods Young men (N = 162) provided standardized facial photos with a neutral expression subsequently rated by eight women for overall attractiveness. Saliva was assayed for immunoglobulin A, testosterone (T) and cortisol (C), and body fat was measured using a skinfold caliper. Self-reports of poor health, and lifestyle factors that could influence health status (age, sleep habits, smoking, drinking alcohol, family stress, and exercising) were collected. Results Results showed that symptoms of poor health and skinfold negatively predicted facial attractiveness. There was a modest but statistically non-significant T x C interaction where higher T lower C men trended toward having more attractive faces. A sequential mediation model examining the influence of lifestyle showed support for an indirect effect on facial attractiveness. Specifically, skinfold and poor health symptoms mediated the links between exercise, stress, and facial attractiveness. Discussion These findings suggest links between facial attractiveness and immunocompetence could be linked to some common lifestyle and hormonal variables, but that more comprehensive research involving lifestyle indicators (such as nutrition) are necessary.
... Compared to women, men have a more "robust" facial structure. This is seen in that the brow ridgethe bone underlying the eyebrowsis more prominent, and the lower face is more pronounced: it is longer, and the mandibles, the cheekbones, and the chin are wider (Koehler et al., 2004;Marečková et al., 2011;Swaddle & Reierson, 2002;Tanikawa et al., 2016). The eyebrows are bushier and set lower, and the eyes typically appear smaller due to being more deeply set. ...
... It should be noted that the degrees of masculinity and attractiveness in the base face of the stimuli may impact the pattern of preferences. Masculinized computer manipulations appear to be preferred if the base face is more attractive or less masculine, and feminization if the base face is less attractive or highly masculine (Swaddle & Reierson, 2002;Yang et al., 2015). Thus, the selection of stimuli has implications for the direction of observed preferences. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
... Medtem ko je večina raziskav pokazala, da moški preferirajo feminilne (ženstvene) obraze (Perrett idr., 1998), se preferenca maskuliniziranih obrazov med ženskami lahko precej razlikuje (Cunningham idr., 1990;Swaddle in Reierson, 2002). Ženske, ki si želijo kooperativnega in družinskega partnerja, pogosteje izberejo moške z bolj ženstvenim obrazom. ...
... Given that some studies have found a cross-cultural consensus on perceived attractiveness of faces, this may support the theory that certain preferences are part of our biological rather than cultural heritage [17,21]. A possible explanation has been established that some beauty traits, as for example symmetry of the face or feminine features in female faces and masculine features in male faces [22,23], are supposed to serve as indirect fitness indicators and point to good genes [21,24]. Since the characteristics of attractiveness in favour of sexual selection have evolved over thousands of years, it is difficult to identify the origin of body dysmorphic disorder. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article provides an overview of the development of the body dysmorphic disorder in the context of various prevailing ideals of beauty. The distinction from simple body dissatisfaction is presented, also in view of brain-organic as well as neurochemical differences. Individuals with body dysmorphic disorder have an increased sensitivity to the detection of detail-instead of a holistic view-, making them more likely to notice flaws or blemishes that are unnoticeable to others. This distorted perception can be corrected in the context of cognitive behavioural therapy, provided that the body dysmorphic disorder is recognised as such. The concealment by comorbidities as well as the consequences for affected persons and treatment options are described.
... Other explanations highlight the associations between facial masculinity and traits other than health or resistance to disease. One such association is the perception of dominance, which is positively associated with structural facial masculinity for judgments of both women (Johnston et al., 2001;Perrett et al., 1998;Puts, 2010;Swaddle & Reierson, 2002) and men (Muller & Mazur, 1997). In fact, the impact of masculine traits on the perception of dominance is more substantial and consistently positive compared to the ir effect on attractiveness (Puts et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
The impact of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness has been controversial due to contradictory results, particularly in studies on female preferences. Given that sexually dimorphic facial features, especially more masculine ones, have been previously related to the perception of anger, we investigated the bi-directional influence of emotional expressions and facial masculinity and explored their impact on women's preferences for facial masculinity. We confirmed the effect of facial sexual dimorphism on the perception of emotional cues (happiness and anger) and explored whether smiling or angry expressions influence women's perception of masculinity in male faces. Additionally, we examined women's preferences for emotionally expressive male faces altered along a continuum of masculinity. Results showed that masculinized faces are perceived as angrier, while feminized faces are perceived as happier (Experiment 1), and that angry faces are perceived as more masculine when compared to happy faces (Experiment 2). Noteworthy, our Experiment 3 uncovered a pivotal finding: women prefer reduced feminization in happy faces compared to neutral/angry faces. This suggests that the avoidance response observed towards masculinity is attenuated by a smiling expression. The current study introduces a new perspective to be considered when exploring the role of facial masculinity in women's attractiveness preferences.
... A high level of testosterone has been related to the increase in mateseeking behaviors in males [112,185], but data are inconclusive as to whether women can detect fluctuations in steroid hormones, like testosterone and cortisol, to drive their judgment of body odor attractiveness [144,189]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Humans produce odorous secretions from multiple body sites according to the microbiomic profile of each area and the types of secretory glands present. Because the axilla is an active, odor-producing region that mediates social communication via the sense of smell, this article focuses on the biological mechanisms underlying the creation of axillary odor, as well as the intrinsic and extrinsic factors likely to impact the odor and determine individual differences. The list of intrinsic factors discussed includes sex, age, ethnicity, emotions, and personality, and extrinsic factors include dietary choices, diseases, climate, and hygienic habits. In addition, we also draw attention to gaps in our understanding of each factor, including, for example, topical areas such as the effect of climate on body odor variation. Fundamental challenges and emerging research opportunities are further outlined in the discussion. Finally, we suggest guidelines and best practices based on the factors reviewed herein for preparatory protocols of sweat collection, data analysis, and interpretation.
Article
Mate choice, and sex differences in romantic behaviours, represented one of the first major applications of evolutionary biology to human behaviour. This paper reviews Darwinian approaches to heterosexual mate assessment based on physical characteristics, placing the literature in its historical context (1871-1979), before turning (predominantly) to psychological research on attractiveness judgements based on physical characteristics. Attractiveness is consistently inferred across multiple modalities, with biological theories explaining why we differentiate certain individuals, on average, from others. Simultaneously, it is a judgement that varies systematically in light of our own traits, environment, and experiences. Over 30 years of research has generated robust effects alongside reasons to be humble in our lack of understanding of the precise physiological mechanisms involved in mate assessment. This review concludes with three questions to focus attention in further research, and proposes that our romantic preferences still provide a critical window into the evolution of human sexuality.
Chapter
This chapter explores the multifaceted concept of “facial beauty and attractiveness,” a subject that has captivated scientists, artists, philosophers, and anthropologists alike. The quest for beauty, often manifesting in the pursuit of cosmetic enhancements, is not a modern phenomenon but a timeless human preoccupation, deeply rooted in the history and literature of every culture and ethnicity. Historical evidence underscores how societies across ages have endeavored to align their appearance with the prevailing beauty ideals of their time. Facial aesthetics and attractiveness represent a complex interplay of various disciplines including neuroscience, psychology, sociology, biology, and mathematics. This complexity is indicative of the cognitive intricacies involved in the perception of integrated beauty, which cannot be fully understood through a singular lens focusing solely on aesthetic appeal. The analysis of beauty standards, influenced by both intrinsic biological factors and extrinsic cultural and social norms, reveals a dynamic interplay between the objective and subjective determinants of beauty. Cross-cultural studies highlight the variability in beauty standards, yet also point to an underlying unity, suggesting a collective human inclination towards certain aesthetic qualities. Understanding facial beauty extends beyond the superficial, engaging with the neurological responses elicited by aesthetically pleasing features, such as the activation of the medial orbitofrontal cortex in response to attractive faces and expressions. This chapter seeks to provide aesthetic practitioners with a deeper insight into the complex dimensions of beauty, illuminating the integrated nature of facial aesthetics as it intersects with human psychology, societal influences, and biological imperatives.
Article
Full-text available
Culture is the ongoing product of the evolved psyches of individual humans living in groups. Progress in our understanding of culture as a phenomenon depends on progress in uncovering the nature of the evolved mechanisms that comprise the human psyche, including but not limited to those responsible for learning. Actual attemps to specify information processing mechanisms that could, in fact, perform tasks humans routinely perform have demonstrated that the human psyche cannot, even in principle, be comprised only of a general purpose learning mechanism or any other general purpose mechanism, such as an inclusive fitness maximizer. Instead, the human psyche appears to consist of a large number of mechanisms, many or most of which are special purpose and domain-specific. The output of these mechanisms taken together constitutes the “private culture” of each individual, and the interactions of these private cultures lead to the cross-individual patterns of similarity that have led anthropologists to think typologically of social groups as having “a” culture. The construction of a scientific theory of culture requires as its building blocks specific models of these psychological mechanisms, and so evolutionary anthropology depends on the forging of an evolutionary psychology. The most productive application of evolutionary biology is, therefore, in the study of the psychological mechanisms that generate and shape culture, rather than in the attempt to impose on cultural change too close a parallel to population genetics and organic evolution.
Article
Full-text available
Why are some humans considered more beautiful than others? Theory suggests that sexually reproducing organisms should choose mates displaying characters indicative of high genotypic or phenotypic quality. Attraction to beautiful individuals may therefore be an adaptation for choosing high-quality mates. Culturally invariant standards of beauty in humans have been taken as evidence favouring such an adaptationist explanation of attraction; however, if standards of beauty are instead no more than artefacts of culture, they should vary across cultures. Here we show that male preference for women with a low waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) is not culturally universal, as had previously been assumed.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has documented that more facially attractive people are perceived by others to be physically healthier. Using self-reports, observer ratings, daily diary methodology, and psychophysiological assessments, this study provides limited empirical evidence that more facially attractive people (N = 100) may be physically healthier than unattractive people. Discussion suggests the value of an evolutionary psychological perspective for understanding the relationship between facial attractiveness and physical health.
Article
Craniofacial growth was investigated in boys treated with low-dose testosterone for delayed puberty (>14 years old; testicular volume <4 ml; n =7) and compared with controls (12-14 years; n =37). Cephalometric radiographs, statural height and pubertal stage were recorded at the start of the study and after 1 year. Craniofacial growth was assessed by nine linear measurements. At the beginning of the study, statural height, mandibular ramus length, upper anterior face height, and total cranial base length were significantly shorter in the delayed puberty boys than in the controls. After 1 year, the growth rate of the statural height, total mandibular length, ramus length, and upper and total anterior face height was significantly higher in the treated boys than in the untreated height-matched controls ( n =7). The craniofacial measurements were similar in the treated boys as compared with the controls. These results show that statural height and craniofacial dimensions are low in boys with delayed puberty. Low doses of testosterone accelerate statural and craniofacial growth, particularly in the delayed components, thus leading towards a normalization of facial dimensions.
Article
Evidence has accumulated in recent years supporting the hypothesis that both facial and bodily physical attractiveness in humans are certifications of developmental and hormonal health. Such evidence indicates that physical attractiveness is an honest or Zahavian signal of phenotypic and genetic quality. The hypothesis that physical beauty connotes health was first proposed by Westermarck and was discussed later by Ellis and Symons. It has been suggested that facial attractiveness in women is a deceptive signal of youth, unrelated to phenotypic and genetic quality. This sensory-bias or super-stimulus hypothesis is not supported by this study of men’s ratings of the attractiveness of photographs of 92 nude women. Independent ratings of photographs of faces, fronts with faces covered, and backs of the same women are significantly, positively correlated. The correlation between the ratings of different photos implies that women’s faces and external bodies comprise a single ornament of honest mate value, apparently constructed during puberty by estrogen and also probably by developmental adaptations for symmetry. Thus, women’s physical attractiveness in face and body honestly signal hormonal and perhaps developmental health.
Article
Evidence has accumulated in recent years supporting the hypothesis that both facial and bodily physical attractiveness in humans are certifications of developmental and hormonal health. Such evidence indicates that physical attractiveness is an honest or Zahavian signal of phenotypic and genetic quality. The hypothesis that physical beauty connotes health was first proposed by Westermarck and was discussed later by Ellis and Symons. It has been suggested that facial attractiveness in women is a deceptive signal of youth, unrelated to phenotypic and genetic quality. This sensory-bias or super-stimulus hypothesis is not supported by this study of men’s ratings of the attractiveness of photographs of 92 nude women. Independent ratings of photographs of faces, fronts with faces covered, and backs of the same women are significantly, positively correlated. The correlation between the ratings of different photos implies that women’s faces and external bodies comprise a single ornament of honest mate value, apparently constructed during puberty by estrogen and also probably by developmental adaptations for symmetry. Thus, women’s physical attractiveness in face and body honestly signal hormonal and perhaps developmental health.
Article
Dominance competition among males of the same group is characteristic for most primates including humans, with the outcome—rank—being positively associated with fitness. However, because we do not observe an evolutionary arms race toward hyperdominance in primate social systems, the evolution of ever more dominating behavior characteristics may be checked by increased costs in fitness to top rank. Empirical evidence for such costs, particularly in humans, however, is almost nonexistent. Here, for the first time, we can demonstrate for a cohort of male humans, military officers, all graduates of the class of 1950 of the U.S. Military Academy, constituting a closed and cohesive social group, that competitive qualities leading to top rank may have a negative effect on fitness.
Book
Do we read character in faces? What information do faces actually provide? Why do we associate certain facial qualities with particular character traits? What are the social and psychological consequences of reading character in faces? Zebrowitz unmasks the face and provides the first systematic, scientific account of our tendency to judge people by their appearance. Offering an in-depth analysis of two appearance qualities that influence our impressions of others, babyfaceness" and attractiveness", and an account of these impressions, Zebrowitz has written an accessible and valuable book for professionals and general readers alike.The assumption that people's faces provide a window to their inner nature has a long and distinguished history, eloquently expressed in the works of ancient philosophers, like Aristotle, and great writers, like Shakespeare. Zebrowitz examines this assumption, focusing on four central points. She shows that facial appearance, particularly babyfaceness and attractiveness, has a strong impact on how we perceive an individual's character traits and on social outcomes in the workplace, in the criminal justice system, and in other settings. She proposes that facial stereotypes derive from evolutionarily adaptive reactions to useful information that faces can provide. She assesses the accuracy of facial stereotypes in light of plausible links between appearance and character. Finally, Zebrowitz suggests ways to counteract the consequences of reading faces.
Article
Inspired by the evolutionary conjecture that sexually selected traits function as indicators of pathogen resistance in animals and humans, we examined the notion that human facial attractiveness provides evidence of health. Using photos of 164 males and 169 females in late adolescence and health data on these individuals in adolescence, middle adulthood, and later adulthood, we found that adolescent facial attractiveness was unrelated to adolescent health for either males or females, and was not predictive of health at the later times. We also asked raters to guess the health of each stimulus person from his or her photo. Relatively attractive stimulus persons were mistakenly rated as healthier than their peers. The correlation between perceived health and medically assessed health increased when attractiveness was statistically controlled, which implies that attractiveness suppressed the accurate recognition of health. These findings may have important implications for evolutionary models.