ArticlePDF Available

Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Recent evidence suggests that certain features on the human face indicate hormonal levels during growth, and that women judge the attractiveness of potential partners based on the appearance of these features. One entrenched notion is male facial features that are affected by testosterone are used as direct cues in mate preference. Testosterone may be particularly revealing as it is purported to be an honest indicator of male fitness. Increased testosterone may impose an immunocompetence handicap on the bearer and only the best males can carry this handicap. To date, tests of this theory have been indirect, and have relied on digital manipulations that represent unrealistic continuums of masculine and feminine faces. We provide a much more direct test by manipulating digitally male faces to mimic known shape variation, caused by varying levels of testosterone through puberty. We produced a continuum of faces that ranged from low to high levels of testosterone in male faces and asked women to choose the points on the continuum that appeared most attractive and most physically dominant. Our data indicate that high testosterone faces reveal dominance. However, there is no evidence of directional selection for increased (or decreased) testosterone in terms of attractiveness to the opposite sex. We discuss the relevance and applicability of evolutionary interpretations of our data and, contrary to predictions, provide evidence of stabilizing selection acting on testosterone through mate preferences.
Content may be subject to copyright.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165
, 2285-2289269 2002 Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
John P. Swaddle and Gillian W. Reierson
attractiveness in human males
Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not
References
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/269/1507/2285#related-urls
Article cited in:
Email alerting service
hereright-hand corner of the article or click
Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions go to: Proc. R. Soc. Lond. BTo subscribe to
This journal is © 2002 The Royal Society
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Received
9 May 2002
Accepted
15 August 2002
Published online
9 October 2002
Testosterone increases perceived dominance but
not attractiveness in human males
John P. Swaddle
*
and Gillian W. Reierson
Biology Department, College of William & Ma ry, Williamsburg, VA 23197-879, USA
Recent evidence suggests that certain f eatures on the human face indicate hormonal levels during growth,
and that women judge the attractiveness of potential partners based on the appearance of these features.
One entrenched notion is male facial features that are affe cted by testosterone are used as direct cues in
mate preference. Testosterone may be particularly revealing as it is purported to be an honest indicator
of male tness. Increased testosterone may impose an immunocompetence handicap on the bearer and
only the best males can carry this hand icap. To date, tes ts of this theory have been indirect, and have
relied on digital manipulations that represent unrealistic continuums of masculine and feminine faces. We
provide a much more direct test by manipulating digitally male faces to mimic known shape variation,
caused by varying levels of testosterone through puberty. W e prod uced a continuum of faces that ranged
from low to high levels of testosterone in male faces and asked women to choose the points on the
continuum that appeared most attractive and most physically domin ant. Our data indicate that high testos -
terone faces reveal dominance. However, there is no evidence of directional selection for increased (or
decreased) testosterone in terms of attractiveness to the opposite sex. We discuss the relevance and appli-
cability of evolutionary interpretations of our data and, contrary to predictions, provide evidence of stabil-
izing selection acting on testosterone through mate preferences.
Keywords: sexual selection;
Homo sapiens
; immunocompetence; handicap; mate preference
1. IN TRODUCTION
Several hypotheses suggest that human faces display mar-
kers of hormones, and that these cues affect judgements
of facial attractiveness ( Johnston & Franklin 1993; Perrett
et al.
1998; Thornhill & Gangestad 1999; Johnston
et al.
2001) and dominance (Mazur
et al.
1994; Mazur & Booth
1998). In particular, evolutionary psychologists have gen-
erated theories to pred ict that levels of testosterone
re ected in male facial features are (or were) important in
generating sexual dimorphism in humans, as well as
directly affecting mate preferences (Perrett
et al.
1998;
Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Thornhill & Gangestad 1999;
Johnston
et al.
2001). It has been suggested that facial
testosterone may be an honest indicator of male tne ss
(Thornhill & Gangestad 1999), as increased testosterone
can impose an immunocompetence handicap on the
bearer (see discussions in Grossman 1985; Hasselquist
et
al.
1999; Duckworth
et al.
2001) and only the best males
can carry this handicap (Zahavi 1975; Hamilton & Zuk
1982).
Existing tests of testosterone-related hypotheses of
human attractiveness have focused on producing femi-
nine–masculine axes of facial shape in digital represen-
tations (by computer graphics manipulations) and
monitoring people’s responses to these faces (Perrett
et al.
1998; Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001). In
some cases , more masculine, male faces are preferred
(Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001), whereas
in others more feminine male faces are judged as attractive
(Perrett
et al.
1998). Part of this variation appears to be
correlated with the stage of ovarian cycle of women judg-
*
Author for correspondence (jpswad@wm.edu).
Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B (2002)
269
, 2285–2289 2285 Ó 2002 The Royal Society
DOI 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165
ing the faces (Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001). However, there is no evidence to indicate that
these uctuations in female preferences affect actual part-
ner choice.
Previous experiments have also provided fairly un natu-
ral, and rather indirect, tests of the testosterone-related
hypotheses. First, there are many differences between
masculine and feminine faces, which are not accounted
for by testosterone alone (Perrett
et al.
1998). Hence, the
results of these studies are dif cult to interpret in terms
of testosterone expression in the male face. Second, the
faces produc ed through such manipulations result in male
facial shapes outside a natural male distribution (Penton-
Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001), and are commonly
presented as faces without hairlines or necks (Perrett
et
al.
1998). Preferences for face shapes from such arti cial
distributions may not re ect preferences shown in society,
as the facial variation in normal male human populations
can be very different.
A more direct approach to testing testosterone-related
hypotheses is to produce facial variation that re ects
quanti ed variation in te stosterone levels. Therefore, we
manipulated the shape of male faces to mimic growth dif-
ferences resulting from varying levels of testosterone dur-
ing puberty. Women viewed these faces and made
judgements of facial attractiveness and dominance.
According to previou s reports, we predicted that women
should nd face s with higher testosterone expression most
attractive ( Thornhill & Gangestad 1999).
2. MATERIAL AND METHODS
We took digital photographs of 21 male Caucasians’ faces in
a (right-side) pro le and face-on orientation to the camera.
Models were 18–21 years old, had short hair, lacked beards or
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
2286 J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reie rson Testosterone and facial attractiveness
Table 1. Approximate relative trait size changes (i.e. distances between two points) resulting from w arp manipulations for the
100% high and 100% low testosterone treatments, for pro le and face-on photographs of male faces.
(Facial characters were determined from anthropometric de nitions (Farkas 1981) and their pos ition was estimated on each male
face. Points used were: nasion (N) midpoint of both nasal root and nasofrontal suture; pogonion (POG) anterior midpoint of
the chin; gonion (GO) most lateral point of the mandibular angle; menton (ME) lowest point on the mandible; ans (ANS)
approximated by the posterior extremity of the nose; articulare (ART) estimated by the posterior and ventral atta chment of the
ear; sella (S) estimated by the anterior and ventral attachment of the ear.)
pro le face-on
trait 100% high 100% low 100% high 100% low
N–ANS
1
0.01
2
0.01
1
0.01
2
0.01
N–ME
1
0.01
2
0.01
1
0.01
2
0.01
ART–ANS
1
0.01
2
0.01 0 0
ART–POG
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.02
2
0.02
ART–GO
1
0.04
2
0.04
1
0.04
2
0.04
S–ME
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.02
2
0.02
ART–ME
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.02
2
0.02
GO–POG
1
0.02
2
0.02
1
0.01
2
0.01
moustaches, were not wearing jewellery, and were told to adopt
a neutral facial expression while sitting for the photographs.
Photographs were taken in standardized lighting conditions
against a common background.
We compared how facial s tructure changes with natural levels
of testosterone during puberty, and in delayed puberty boys
treated with low-dose testosterone (Verdonck 1997; V erdonck
et al.
1999) to create vectors of facial trait changes that represent
realistic variation in plasma testosterone in uencing facial bone
growth elds (Enlow 1990; Silveira
et al.
1992). The relative
changes of the facial traits we manipulated are listed in table 1.
Brie y, face height increased and the lower jaw became larger
with increased testosterone. These manipulations were speci -
cally de signed to isolate the effects of te stosterone on facial skel-
etal characters, providing a more direct test of the effec ts of
testosterone on perception of the face.
We used the warp functi on in Gryphon Software’s Morph
program to alter the shape of male faces according to the trait
size c hanges listed in table 1. We also digitized 10 points around
the eyes, ve on the nose, and ve on the lips to ensure that this
part of the face did not change under the warp manipulation.
The warp function moves the reference points to a speci ed pos-
ition without altering colour or tone ( gure 1). In addition to
producing the high and low testosterone treatments, we pro-
duced nine, equally spaced, intermediate warps between the
control and high testosterone faces, and between the control and
low testosterone faces. This resul ted in 2 1 representations of a
single face that ran ged from the low treatment to the high treat-
ment with equal warp differences betwe en all images, and where
the median face was the control (i.e. non-manipulated face).
The sequence of 21 representations was arranged in ascending
or descending order in a PowerPoint presentation, with one
image on each slide. For half the faces the rst slide contained
the lowest testosterone treatment; for the remaining faces the
rst slide was the highest testosterone treatment. Each slide also
displayed a sequential number so that viewers could tell us
which slide they chose.
Thirty females, age 18–21 years old, viewed each of the 42
sequences on a computer screen. Subjects were asked to choose
the most sexually attractive, and most physically dominant look-
ing, face in each sequence. One of the experimenters advanced
(or went back through) the slides at a regular pace of one slide
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
approximately every 0.5 s. In ad dition, viewe rs instructed the
experimenter to advance, reverse, and stop the sequence when-
ever the viewer wished. The order of male faces viewed by
females was random ized, except that all the pro le faces were
presented in sequence, as were the face-on presentations. Half
of the viewers saw the pro le sequences rst, and vice versa.
Viewers were paid for their participation. If a viewer recognized
one of the male faces, she informed the researchers and the data
for that face were discarded.
3. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
The frames chosen by females as being most attractive,
for both face-on and pro le views, were faces with testos-
terone expression very similar to that of the non-manipu-
lated face (paired
t
-tests of the most attractive versus the
control face: face-on,
t
2 0
= 1.94,
p
= 0.067; pro le,
t
2 0
= 1.86,
p
= 0.078). The frequency diagram of most
attractive frame choice illustrates that women’s prefer-
ences centre on existing levels of testosterone in men’s
faces ( gure 2). S uch distributions are consistent with sta-
bilizing selection acting on testos terone expression in
men’s faces rather th an the previously proposed direc-
tional selection for reduced or increased testosterone
(Perrett
et al.
1998; Johnston
et al.
2001).
It is important to point out that individual female raters
used the entire range of faces for attractivenes s choices.
For face-on views, the mean ± s.e. (among-rater) range of
frame chosen was 18.63 ± 0.54 (out of a possible 21) for
face-on views, and 19.50 ± 2.54 for pro le views. This
indicates that females did not feel constrained to only
using the central portion of the animation sequence s, and
that our result is not an artefact of a rater central tendency
independent of attractiveness judgements.
In order to explore further the effects of testosterone on
facial attractivenes s, we calculated an index of testosterone
in non-manipulated male faces (the ratio of AR T–POG
length to N–S length, where a high value indicates higher
facial testosterone expression; see table 1 for de nitions)
and examined whether natural variation in testosterone
affected choice of frames. Linear measurements were
taken directly from the digital photographs of pro le faces.
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Testosterone and facial attractiveness J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reierson 2287
frame 1
low
frame 11
control
frame 21
high
(i)
(ii)
(iii)
(i)
(ii)
(iii)
Figure 1. The extremes of the manipulations p roduced for (
a
(i)–(iii)) face-on and (
b
(i)–(iii)) pro le views of the same face.
Low, low testosterone treatment; control, unmanipulated face; high, high testosterone treatment.
Natural levels of facial testosterone were negatively corre-
lated with choice of attractive frame (face-on: Pearson
r
=
2
0.636,
n
= 21,
p
= 0.0011; pro le:
r
=
2
0.710,
n
= 21,
p
= 0.000 15). Inspection of the numerical values
of these correlations indicated that the most preferred ver-
sions of naturally high testosterone faces were those that
reduced apparent testosterone. Whereas the most pre-
ferred representations of naturally low testosterone faces
were those that increased apparent testosterone. The net
effect of such pre ferences is a convergent selection press-
ure on average testosterone expression across the popu-
lation, which provides further evidence of stabilizing
selection on tes tosterone. Although these correlations are
strong, we need to emphasize that there is a possibility
that raters’ preferences could alter with their oestrus cycle
(cf. Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Johnston
et al.
2001). In
further studies, it would be interesting to investigate
whether females preferences for facial testosterone change
systematically according to the possibility of conception.
The choice of the most dominant frame was very differ-
ent to the choice of the most attractive frame. In agree-
ment with previous evidence (Perrett
et al.
1998; Johnston
et al.
2001), the most dominant frames were those with a
high degree of testosterone expression ( gure 2). This pat-
tern he lps to co n rm that our manipulation was realistic,
as links betwee n testosterone and male aggression and
dominance are generally well supported (Mazur &
Booth 1998).
It was also evident th at choices made by viewers when
judging males face-on were different to when males were
in pro le. Females chose faces signi cantly higher in tes-
tosterone as most attractive when viewing a face in pro le,
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
compared with the same male face-on (
t
2 0
= 3.16,
p
= 0.0049). Conversely, females chose faces signi cantly
lower in testosterone as most dominant for pro le, versus
face-on, views of males (
t
2 0
= 3.65,
p
= 0.0016). These dif-
ferences clearly demo nstrate that female judgement of a
male face changes with viewing angle. This implies that
further experiments need to account for three- dimensional
viewing of faces if re searchers are going to document
realistic relations between facial characters and socially (or
evolutionarily) important parameters. However, in the
context of our experiment, we observed the same general
relationships between attractiveness (or dominance) and
facial testosterone expression in pro le and face-on.
A recent resurgence in adaptive explanations of human
behaviour and morphology has stimulated interest in
whether researchers can demonstrate evolutionarily
important relations hips through preference and rating
experiments (Perrett
et al.
1998; Scheib
et al.
1999; John-
ston
et al.
2001). To demonstrate a current evolutionary
effect a research programme must meet certain criteria. In
the context of this study, we would need to demonstrate
evidence of selection pres sure acting on traits, and heri-
table variation in those traits. In terms of the former, o ur
data could indicate stabilizing selection if the re is a link
between differential reproductive success and rating of
facial attractiveness. There is evidence that facial attract-
iveness positively affects the ways in which others treat
them in many social contexts (Kalick 1988; Zebrowitz
1997). However, links between facial attractiveness and
health or reproductive success are equivocal or non-
existent (Kalick
et al.
1998; Shackelford & L arsen 1999;
Rhodes
et al.
2001). Women may place less importance
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
2288 J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reie rson Testosterone and facial attractiveness
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
1
3
5
7
9 11 13 15 17 19 21
frame chosen
frequency frequency
a
)
b
)
Figure 2. (
a
) Frequency histogram for frame choice of most
attractive (open bars) and most dominant ( lled bars) frames
for face-on views; (
b
) and for pro le v iews of male faces. In
both distributions, the choice of dominant frame represented
a much higher degree of testosterone expression than the
choice of attractive frame (face-on,
t
20
=
11.75,
p
,
0.001;
pro le,
t
20
=
3.05,
p
=
0.0064).
on physical attractiveness when choosing a mate than men
do (Waynforth 2001). This implies a low probability that
male facial attractiveness will relate to reproductive suc-
cess or tness. However, it does not preclude that attract-
iveness was related to reproductive success at some time
in our evolutionary history. There is some indication that
increased facial dominance is associated with increased
sexual activity in you ng men (Mazur
et al.
1994), but
increased sexual activity does not nec essarily result in
increased numbers of offspring (Mueller & Mazur 1998).
Additionally, extreme dominance can negatively in uence
reproductive success (Mueller & Mazur 1998).
In terms of the ‘heritability c riteria, there is some e vi-
dence of signi c ant heritability of the male facial traits we
assessed (upper estimates range from 10% to 36%
heritability) (Enlow 1990) and of circulating testosterone
levels in men (upper estimates of
ca
. 60% heritability)
(Harris
et al.
1998). Overall, interpreting our experiment
and those using similar methodologies (Perrett
et al.
1998;
Penton-Voak
et al.
1999; Scheib
et al.
1999; Johnston
et
al.
2001) in terms of evolutionary processes is problem-
atic, and generally unsupported by empirical evidence. It
is more likely that judgements of particular facial shapes
are mediated by cultural norms and individual decision-
making (Tooby & Cosmides 1989).
Even if some researchers insist on evolutionary
interpretations, we cannot nd evidence for directional
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
selection to red uce or increase testosterone through female
preferences. Previous studies that have documented
female preference s for masculine ( Johnston
et al.
2001) or
feminine (Perrett
et al.
1998) male faces cann ot be inter-
preted in terms of the effects of testosterone alone. Other
hormonal differences between the sexes must (in part)
explain previously d ocumented preferences. It may be that
selection acting on oestrogen and on female faces may
equally explain human evolutionary changes (Thornhill &
Grammer 1999). The comparative literature has demon-
strated that both sexes can be the target of directional sex-
ual selection and that both sexes change through
evolutionary time, resulting in dimorphism (Karubian &
Swaddle 2001). Our data also suggest t hat social domi-
nance may underlie the evolution of human sexual dimor-
phism, as increased testosterone is positively associated
with perceived dominance. However, increased perceived
dominance does not enhance male attractiveness.
The authors thank Adam Rubenstein and three anonymous
reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft, as well as
the 21 male and 30 female students of William & Mary who
agreed to take part in this study. J.P.S. was supported by the
NSF (IBN-0133795) and the Thomas F. and Kate Miller
Jeffress Memorial Trust. G.W.R. was supported by the
Howard Hughes Medical Institute.
REFERENCES
Duckworth, R. A., Mendonca, M. T. & Hill, G. E. 2001 A
condition dependent link between testosterone and disease
resistance in the house nch.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B
268
,
2467–2472. (DOI 10.1098/rspb.2001.1827.)
Enlow, D. H. 1990
Facial growth
. Philadelphia, PA: Har-
court Brace.
Farkas, L. 1981
Anthropometry of the head and face in medicine
.
New York: Elsevier.
Grossman, C. J. 1985 Interactions between the gonadal ster-
oids and the immune system.
Science
227
, 257–261.
Hamilton, W. D. & Zuk, M. 1982 Heritable true tness and
bright birds: a role for parasites?
Science
218
, 384–387.
Harris, J. A., Vernon, P. A. & Boomsma, D. I. 1998 The heri-
tability of testosterone: a study of Dutch adolescent twins
and their parents.
Behav. Genet.
28
, 165–171.
Hasselquist, D., Marsh, J. A., Sherman, P. W . & Wing eld,
J. C. 1999 Is avian humoral immunocompetence suppressed
by testosterone?
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
45
, 167–175.
Johnston, V. S. & Franklin, M. 1993 Is beauty in the eye of
the beholder?
Ethol. Sociobiol.
14
, 183–199.
Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B. & Grammer,
K. 2001 Male facial attractiveness: evidence for hormone-
mediated a daptive design.
Evol. Hum. Behav.
22
, 251–267.
Kalick, S. M. 1988 Physical attractiveness as a status cue.
J.
Exp. Soc. Psychol.
24
, 469–489.
Kalick, S. M., Zebrowitz, L. A., Langlois, J. H. & Johnson,
R. M. 1998 Does human facial attractiveness honestly adver-
tise health? Longitudinal data on an evolutionary question.
Psychol. Sci.
9
, 8–13.
Karubian, J. & Swaddle, J. P. 2001 Selection on females can
create ‘larger males’.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B
268
, 725–728.
(DOI 10.1098/rspb.2000.1407.)
Mazur, A. & Booth, A. 1998 Testosterone and dominance in
men.
Behav. Brain Sci.
21
, 353–397.
Mazur, A., Halpern, C. & Udry, J. R . 1994 Domina nt looking
male teenagers copulate earlier.
Ethol. Sociobiol.
15
, 87–94.
Mueller, U. & Mazur, A. 1998 Reproductive constraints on
dominance competition in male
Homo sapiens
.
Evol. Hum.
Behav.
19
, 387–396.
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Testosterone and facial attractiveness J. P. Swaddle and G. W. Reierson 2289
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi,
T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K. & Minamisawa, R. 1999
Menstrual cycle alters face preference.
Nature
399
, 741–742.
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshi-
kawa, S., Burt, D. M., Henzl, S. P., Castles, D. L. & Akam-
atsu, S. 1998 Effects of s exual dimorphism on facial
attractiveness.
Nature
394
, 884–887.
Rhodes, G., Zebrowitz, L. A., Clark, A., Kalick, S. M., High-
tower, A. & McKay, R. 2001 Do facial averageness and sym-
metry signal health ?
Evol. Hum. Behav.
22
, 31–4 6.
Scheib, J. E., Gangestad, S. W. & Thornhill, R. 1999 Facial
attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes.
Proc. R.
Soc. Lond.
B
266
, 1913–1917. (DOI 10.1098/rspb.1999.
0866.)
Shackelford, T. K. & La rsen, R. J. 1999 Facial attractiveness
and physical health.
Evol. Hum. Behav.
20
, 71–76.
Silveira, A. M., Fishman, L. S., Subteiny, J. D. & Kassebaum,
D. K. 1992 Facial growth during adolescence in early, aver-
age and late maturers.
Angle Orthod.
52
, 185–190.
Thornhill, R. & Gangestad, S. W. 1999 Facial attract iveness.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (2002)
Trends Cogn. Sci.
3
, 452–460.
Thornhill, R. & Grammer, K. 1999 The body and face of
woman: one ornament tha t signals quality ?
Evol. Hum.
Behav.
20
, 105–120.
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. 1989 Evolutionary psychology and
the generation of culture. Part I: theoretical considerations.
Ethol. Sociobiol.
10
, 29–4 9.
Verdonck, A. 1997
Effect of testosterone on craniofacial growth in
the human and the rat
. Leuven: Katholieke Universiteit te
Leuven.
Verdonck, A., Gaethofs, M., Carels, C. & de Zegher, F. 1999
Effect of low-dose testosterone treatment on craniofacial
growth in boys with delayed puberty.
Eur. J. Orthod.
21
,
137–143.
Waynforth, D. 2001 Mate choice trade-offs and women’s pref-
erence for physically attractive men.
Hum. Nat.
12
, 207–219.
Zahavi, A. 1975 Mate selection: a selection for a handicap.
J.
Theor. Biol.
53
, 205–214.
Zebrowitz, L. A. 1997
Reading faces: windows to the soul?
Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
on May 24, 2011rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
... The preference for more masculine male faces can also disappear when the female observer is in the luteal phase of menstruation (when conception is less likely) (Penton-Voak et al., 1999), or is using oral contraception (Little et al., 2002), or is in her third trimester of pregnancy (Limoncin et al., 2015). Furthermore, Glassenberg et al. (2010) and Swaddle and Reierson (2002) found no preference for highly masculine male faces amongst heterosexual females, suggesting preferences based on masculinity are evenly distributed throughout populations when moderator variables are not controlled for. Few facial attractiveness studies have focused on the preferences of homosexual males and females. ...
... For physical attractiveness, this pattern can only be attributed to a change in the featural configuration of the faces as they are depicted at differing pitches, because each face was rated on its' own merit at presentation. The fact that there was not a three-way interaction between sex of participant, sex of stimulus, and pitch challenges the findings of Glassenberg et al. (2010) and Swaddle and Reierson (2002) as this indicates that females actually do find feminine male faces more attractive in the same way they find feminine female faces more attractive. Admittedly, this can also be attributed to an overrepresentation of females categorised as not fertile in the present study. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Studies of facial dimorphism and its functions in humans have concluded that tilting a face at differing angles of pitch influences perceptions of dimorphism, attractiveness, and dominance. However, there are problems with the methodology of previous studies. The use of un-lifelike stimuli is of particular concern. The current study aims to improve the ecological validity of results from dimorphism studies through the use of stimuli that are more representative of real world faces – specifically moving faces. Predictions for the effects of moving faces expected results to correlate with the effects of the stationary faces, while also showing differences indicative of more lifelike stimulus. Participants were presented with stationary stimuli similar to types used previously, as well as a new type of dynamic stimuli which depicted faces moving through a range of pitch. Participants were asked to rate both types of stimulus on categories of dimorphism, attractiveness, and dominance. The results largely matched predictions with stationary images replicating previous findings, and results from dynamic stimuli reiterating those results. The argument is then made that dynamic images are a more ecologically valid stimulus, because they appear more realistic and also elicit more realistic behaviours.
... Women are therefore expected to show a preference for masculine facial appearance and muscular bodies in men. However, studies asking participants to choose between masculinized and feminized versions of men's faces typically nd either no preference for masculinity (Scott et al., 2010;Stephen et al., 2012;Swaddle & Reierson, 2002) or a preference for feminized faces (Mogilski & Welling, 2017;Perrett et al., 1998;Stephen et al., 2018), possibly because high testosterone levels and masculine facial shapes are associated with personality traits that are detrimental in long-term partnerships (e.g., aggression and in delity; Lee et al., 2017;Mazur & Booth, 1998;Perrett et al., 1998). This hypothesis suggests that women's mate selection mechanisms may be performing a trade-o between masculine men who may have "good genes" but personality traits that are undesirable in a long-term partner and father, and feminine men who may be less prone to aggressive behavior and are more suitable as husbands and fathers. ...
Chapter
The dominant evolutionary theory of sexual attraction posits that attraction serves as a psychobehavioral and motivational mechanism for identifying healthy, fertile, and appropriate mates. According to this theory, humans and animals display cues that reflect their mate quality and, if successful, are perceived as attractive by potential mates. There is evidence for such valid cues in human faces, bodies, and non-bodily traits, which include adornments and items that signal provisioning ability, creativity, artistic skills, or conspicuous consumption. In this chapter, we discuss the evidence for the existence of these facial, bodily, and non-bodily cues and for their role in communicating aspects of partner quality, including health, fertility, developmental stability, genetic quality, and potential for parental investment. We further discuss sex differences in the physical cues that men and women rely on in mate choice. We conclude by highlighting the centrality and evolutionary importance of physical cues in contemporary sexual selection, and how they manifest in evolutionarily novel inventions such as physical self-enhancements, “sexy selfies,” social media, and online dating.
... For example, when babies' genders are unknown, people typically assume that those with a higher degree of infantile features are girls (Zebrowitz, 1997(Zebrowitz, /2018. By contrast, men are more likely to develop larger facial bones, such as wider jaws, than women, because of higher testosterone levels, the dominant male sex hormone (Penton-Voak and Chen, 2004;Swaddle and Reierson, 2002). Men also have more prominent noses and skulls, owing to their larger lungs (Holton et al., 2014). ...
Article
Purpose This study aims to investigate the impact of a salesperson’s babyface in his/her profile picture on the number of online reviews the salesperson receives. In addition to testing the direct relationship, this study explores the moderating roles of salesperson gender and consumer involvement. Design/methodology/approach Responding to the call for field-based consumer research, the authors test their theory using an experimental design and a field study. Study 1 employs an experimental design in high and low involvement service settings to test the effect of a babyface on consumers’ intention to write online reviews. Study 2 uses field data, utilising real estate salespeople’s online profile pictures to test the effect of salespeople’s babyface on the number of online reviews they receive. It does so by using an artificial intelligence facial recognition application interface. Findings A salesperson’s babyface results in fewer online reviews in situations in which consumers are highly involved in the purchase process. By contrast, a salesperson’s babyface engenders more online reviews when consumers purchase low involvement services. The adverse effect of a babyface on the number of online reviews, however, attenuates when a salesperson is female. Research limitations/implications Limited information about salespeople, a skewed number of online reviews and blurry online profile pictures from a real-world data set constitute the study’s limitations. Practical implications When consumers are highly involved in the purchase process, salespeople should appear mature in their online profile photos to engender more online reviews. However, salespeople providing low involvement services should opt for online profile pictures reflecting babyish facial features to generate more online reviews. Originality/value Research has shown that salespeople’s physical appearance plays an important role in consumers’ perceptions of salespeople and their performance. Although abundant research and practice have shown the importance of online reviews, less is known about how online profile pictures affect online reviews. Thus, building on well-studied cases of an overgeneralization effect, this work examines the extent to which salespeople’s babyface features in their online profile picture affects the number of online reviews received in a real-world setting.
... More particularly the approach process related to the desire to possess resources, known as "wanting" is presented with only the sensitivity to reward and Impulsivity-RST-PQ scales [10]. This is of a particular importance in the context of studying the relation between testosterone and the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory, especially when taking into consideration that the "wanting" process is thought to be related to testosterone whether through the positive relation between testosterone and the desire to attain higher social status and dominance [23]. Not to mention that testosterone appears to be highly related to situational cues, rising in winning situations and falling in losing situations [24]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) is a neuropsychological theory of personality emphasizing approach and avoidance as the two core behavioral aspects. Approach is represented by the behavioral approach system (BAS). Avoidance is represented by the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) and the fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS). Although the influence of testosterone on human behavior has been demonstrated, few studies have investigated the relation between testosterone and the RST. The aim of this narrative review was to decipher the possible role of testosterone on the biological systems involved in the RST in humans. Methods Google scholar, PubMed, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, Scopus and Cochrane library databases were interrogated using keywords such as testosterone, BIS, BAS, FFFS, personality, reinforcement sensitivity theory. Results Seven original articles, published between 2009 and 2022, assessing the relation between testosterone and the systems implicated in the RST, were included. The results of these studies suggested the presence of a possible positive relation between testosterone and the BAS. However, the impact of testosterone on the BIS and/or FFFS seems to be less clear. Discussion The consistency in the results supporting the presence of a positive relation between testosterone and the BAS might lead to the consideration of testosterone as a potential correlate in the clinical assessment of several psychopathologies. The inconsistency in the conclusions regarding the impact of testosterone on the BIS and/or the FFFS might be attributed to the different questionnaires used as measurement tools. Additional research remains needed.
... Testosterone, for example, modulates morphological characteristics such as muscle mass and strength (Bhasin et al., 1996), 2D:4D digit ratio (Manning et al., 2000), indicators of facial masculinity (Lefevre et al., 2013;Penton-Voak & Chen, 2004;Roney et al., 2006;Verdonck et al., 1999;Welker et al., 2016; but see Hodges-Simeon et al., 2016;Peters et al., 2008) and vocal pitch (Dabbs & Mallinger, 1999;Evans et al., 2008;Puts et al., 2012), that are positively associated with female ratings of attractiveness (Penton-Voak & Chen, 2004; but see Swaddle & Reierson, 2002), particularly in short-term mating contexts (Roney et al., 2006;Valentine et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Life-history theory provides a framework for understanding the resource trade-offs that are inherent in the struggle to maximize reproductive fitness. Hormones, and testosterone in particular, play important roles in mediating some of the morphological, behavioral, and physiological traits that are implicated in these trade-offs—one of the most widely studied of which is mating versus parenting. In this chapter, we use a life history perspective to review literature examining hormones and infidelity or related proxies (e.g., interest in extrapair sex), and how these links may be understood as a function of the mating vs. parenting trade-off. This chapter focuses the review primarily on testosterone, but also reviews other hormones and hormone systems that have been implicated in infidelity or relevant behavioral and psychological proxies. Further, it touches on contextual considerations for understanding the link between hormones and infidelity and mating, such as the type of mating system (e.g., monogamy vs. polygamy) and the menstrual cycle. The chapter concludes with a discussion of some of the limitations of current, and potential avenues for future, research on hormones and infidelity.
... There are several ways in which sexual dimorphism in faces can be measured [38][39][40] and manipulated 41,42 . This is why the method of manipulation of sex-typical facial shape affects the results in studies on the perception of manipulated human facial sex-typicality, too 40,41 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Biosocial impact of facial dominance and sex-typicality is well-evidenced in various human groups. It remains unclear, though, whether perceived sex-typicality and dominance can be consistently predicted from sexually dimorphic facial features across populations. Using a combination of multidimensional Bayesian approach and geometric morphometrics, we explored associations between perceived dominance, perceived sex-typicality, measured sexual shape dimorphism, and skin colour in a European and an African population. Unlike previous studies, we investigated the effect of facial variation due to shape separately from variation due to visual cues not related to shape in natural nonmanipulated stimuli. In men, perceived masculinity was associated with perceived dominance in both populations. In European women higher perceived femininity was, surprisingly, likewise positively associated with perceived dominance. Both shape and non-shape components participate in the constitution of facial sex-typicality and dominance. Skin colour predicted perceived sex-typicality in Africans but not in Europeans. Members of each population probably use different cues to assess sex-typicality and dominance. Using our methods, we found no universal sexually dimorphic scale predicting human perception of sex-typicality and dominance. Unidimensional understanding of sex-typicality thus seems problematic and should be applied with cautions when studying perceived sex-typicality and its correlates.
... A broad literature has focused on testosterone's role in directing social behavior, extending particularly from theoretical frameworks focused on status seeking and dominance (Archer, 2006;Mazur & Booth, 1998;Wingfield et al., 1990). Empirical evidence indicates that testosterone is associated with a suite of psychological (e.g., implicit power motives; , physiological (e.g., reduced cardiovascular indices of fear; Hermans et al., 2006;Van Honk et al., 2001), and morphological characteristics (e.g., facial cues that signal dominance; Hodges-Simeon et al., 2016;Swaddle & Reierson, 2002;Welling et al., 2016;cf. Kordsmeyer et al., 2019) that support the pursuit and maintenance of status and dominance within social hierarchies. ...
Article
Testosterone has been theorized to direct status-seeking behaviors, including competitive behavior. However, most human studies to date have adopted correlational designs, and findings across studies are inconsistent. This experiment (n = 115) pharmacologically manipulated men's testosterone levels prior to a mixed-gender math competition and examined basal cortisol (a hormone implicated in stress and social avoidance) and context cues related to an opponent's perceived status (an opponent's gender or a win/loss in a prior competition) as factors that may moderate testosterone's impact on competitive behavior. We test and find support for the hypothesis that testosterone given to low-cortisol men evokes status-seeking behavior, whereas testosterone given to high-cortisol men evokes status-loss avoidance. In the initial rounds of competition, testosterone's influence on competitive decisions depended on basal cortisol and opponent gender. After providing opponent-specific win-lose feedback, testosterone's influence on decisions to reenter competitions depended on basal cortisol and this objective cue to status, not gender. Compared to placebo, men given exogenous testosterone who were low in basal cortisol showed an increased tendency to compete against male and high-status opponents relative to female and low-status opponents (status-seeking). Men given exogenous testosterone who were high in basal cortisol showed the opposite pattern-an increased tendency to compete against female and low-status opponents relative to male and high-status opponents (status-loss avoidance). These results provide support for a context-dependent dual-hormone hypothesis: Testosterone flexibly directs men's competitive behavior contingent on basal cortisol levels and cues that signal an opponent's status. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
... Dimorphic traits that are exaggerated in males are typically referred to as masculine. In humans, masculine faces are characterized by features such as a pronounced brow ridge, a longer lower face, and wider mandibles, cheekbones, and chins (Swaddle and Reierson, 2002). Men are, on average, in either females or males. ...
Article
Full-text available
Humans are sexually dimorphic: men and women differ in body build and composition, craniofacial structure, and voice pitch, likely mediated in part by developmental testosterone. Sexual selection hypotheses posit that, ancestrally, more 'masculine' men may have acquired more mates and/or sired more viable offspring. Thus far, however, evidence for either association is unclear. Here, we meta-analyze the relationships between six masculine traits and mating/reproductive outcomes (96 studies, 474 effects, N = 177,044). Voice pitch, height, and testosterone all predicted mating; however, strength/muscularity was the strongest and only consistent predictor of both mating and reproduction. Facial masculinity and digit ratios did not significantly predict either. There was no clear evidence for any effects of masculinity on offspring viability. Our findings support arguments that strength/muscularity may be sexually selected in humans, but cast doubt regarding selection for other forms of masculinity and highlight the need to increase tests of evolutionary hypotheses outside of industrialized populations.
Article
Full-text available
The face is an important source of information in social interactions. Prior studies exploring the mechanism of face perception were consistent with either dominance or integration theory. Studies have shown that both sexually dimorphic features and background cues play essential roles in the formation of impressions and the perception of facial attractiveness. In this study, we conducted two experiments to examine 539 participants’ appraisal of attractiveness, warmth, and competence of the target faces of masculine and feminine men and women dressed in red, blue, or white. The results showed that: (1) feminized male/female faces were considered to have a higher degree of attractiveness, warmth, and competence, (2) people rated feminine faces wearing red higher in terms of attractiveness perception, while there was no significant effect of red on attractiveness perception of masculine faces, (3) when evaluating the warmth of targets, the promotion effect of red was found for feminine faces but not for masculine faces. This study, conducted in a pathogen disgust environment, provides direct evidence to support the integration theory over the dominance theory. Feminized red preference found in this study matches Chinese collectivism and the red cultural heritage, which has an important value for people's daily impression management and consumption decisions.
Article
Facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) is associated with aggressive behavior and perceived aggression. This study examined the independent effects of the horizontal and vertical components of fWHR on general aggression perception and specific aggression in romantic relationship (intimate partner violence, IPV) perception. By increasing width, the horizontal component of fWHR significantly increases the IPV perception. By reducing height, the vertical component of fWHR significantly decreases the IPV perception (Study 1). Upper and lower facial heights were reduced simultaneously in the vertical manipulation, indicating mixed effects. In Study 2, we separated the upper and lower face effects. Reducing upper face height could facilitate aggression perception. However, reducing the lower face could suppress aggression perception. The results indicated the disjunction effect of horizontal and vertical components on aggression perception. Our findings highlighted the effect of the lower face on aggression perception, which was ignored in previous studies.
Article
Full-text available
Culture is the ongoing product of the evolved psyches of individual humans living in groups. Progress in our understanding of culture as a phenomenon depends on progress in uncovering the nature of the evolved mechanisms that comprise the human psyche, including but not limited to those responsible for learning. Actual attemps to specify information processing mechanisms that could, in fact, perform tasks humans routinely perform have demonstrated that the human psyche cannot, even in principle, be comprised only of a general purpose learning mechanism or any other general purpose mechanism, such as an inclusive fitness maximizer. Instead, the human psyche appears to consist of a large number of mechanisms, many or most of which are special purpose and domain-specific. The output of these mechanisms taken together constitutes the “private culture” of each individual, and the interactions of these private cultures lead to the cross-individual patterns of similarity that have led anthropologists to think typologically of social groups as having “a” culture. The construction of a scientific theory of culture requires as its building blocks specific models of these psychological mechanisms, and so evolutionary anthropology depends on the forging of an evolutionary psychology. The most productive application of evolutionary biology is, therefore, in the study of the psychological mechanisms that generate and shape culture, rather than in the attempt to impose on cultural change too close a parallel to population genetics and organic evolution.
Article
Full-text available
Why are some humans considered more beautiful than others? Theory suggests that sexually reproducing organisms should choose mates displaying characters indicative of high genotypic or phenotypic quality. Attraction to beautiful individuals may therefore be an adaptation for choosing high-quality mates. Culturally invariant standards of beauty in humans have been taken as evidence favouring such an adaptationist explanation of attraction; however, if standards of beauty are instead no more than artefacts of culture, they should vary across cultures. Here we show that male preference for women with a low waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) is not culturally universal, as had previously been assumed.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has documented that more facially attractive people are perceived by others to be physically healthier. Using self-reports, observer ratings, daily diary methodology, and psychophysiological assessments, this study provides limited empirical evidence that more facially attractive people (N = 100) may be physically healthier than unattractive people. Discussion suggests the value of an evolutionary psychological perspective for understanding the relationship between facial attractiveness and physical health.
Article
Craniofacial growth was investigated in boys treated with low-dose testosterone for delayed puberty (>14 years old; testicular volume <4 ml; n =7) and compared with controls (12-14 years; n =37). Cephalometric radiographs, statural height and pubertal stage were recorded at the start of the study and after 1 year. Craniofacial growth was assessed by nine linear measurements. At the beginning of the study, statural height, mandibular ramus length, upper anterior face height, and total cranial base length were significantly shorter in the delayed puberty boys than in the controls. After 1 year, the growth rate of the statural height, total mandibular length, ramus length, and upper and total anterior face height was significantly higher in the treated boys than in the untreated height-matched controls ( n =7). The craniofacial measurements were similar in the treated boys as compared with the controls. These results show that statural height and craniofacial dimensions are low in boys with delayed puberty. Low doses of testosterone accelerate statural and craniofacial growth, particularly in the delayed components, thus leading towards a normalization of facial dimensions.
Article
Evidence has accumulated in recent years supporting the hypothesis that both facial and bodily physical attractiveness in humans are certifications of developmental and hormonal health. Such evidence indicates that physical attractiveness is an honest or Zahavian signal of phenotypic and genetic quality. The hypothesis that physical beauty connotes health was first proposed by Westermarck and was discussed later by Ellis and Symons. It has been suggested that facial attractiveness in women is a deceptive signal of youth, unrelated to phenotypic and genetic quality. This sensory-bias or super-stimulus hypothesis is not supported by this study of men’s ratings of the attractiveness of photographs of 92 nude women. Independent ratings of photographs of faces, fronts with faces covered, and backs of the same women are significantly, positively correlated. The correlation between the ratings of different photos implies that women’s faces and external bodies comprise a single ornament of honest mate value, apparently constructed during puberty by estrogen and also probably by developmental adaptations for symmetry. Thus, women’s physical attractiveness in face and body honestly signal hormonal and perhaps developmental health.
Article
Evidence has accumulated in recent years supporting the hypothesis that both facial and bodily physical attractiveness in humans are certifications of developmental and hormonal health. Such evidence indicates that physical attractiveness is an honest or Zahavian signal of phenotypic and genetic quality. The hypothesis that physical beauty connotes health was first proposed by Westermarck and was discussed later by Ellis and Symons. It has been suggested that facial attractiveness in women is a deceptive signal of youth, unrelated to phenotypic and genetic quality. This sensory-bias or super-stimulus hypothesis is not supported by this study of men’s ratings of the attractiveness of photographs of 92 nude women. Independent ratings of photographs of faces, fronts with faces covered, and backs of the same women are significantly, positively correlated. The correlation between the ratings of different photos implies that women’s faces and external bodies comprise a single ornament of honest mate value, apparently constructed during puberty by estrogen and also probably by developmental adaptations for symmetry. Thus, women’s physical attractiveness in face and body honestly signal hormonal and perhaps developmental health.
Article
Dominance competition among males of the same group is characteristic for most primates including humans, with the outcome—rank—being positively associated with fitness. However, because we do not observe an evolutionary arms race toward hyperdominance in primate social systems, the evolution of ever more dominating behavior characteristics may be checked by increased costs in fitness to top rank. Empirical evidence for such costs, particularly in humans, however, is almost nonexistent. Here, for the first time, we can demonstrate for a cohort of male humans, military officers, all graduates of the class of 1950 of the U.S. Military Academy, constituting a closed and cohesive social group, that competitive qualities leading to top rank may have a negative effect on fitness.
Book
Do we read character in faces? What information do faces actually provide? Why do we associate certain facial qualities with particular character traits? What are the social and psychological consequences of reading character in faces? Zebrowitz unmasks the face and provides the first systematic, scientific account of our tendency to judge people by their appearance. Offering an in-depth analysis of two appearance qualities that influence our impressions of others, babyfaceness" and attractiveness", and an account of these impressions, Zebrowitz has written an accessible and valuable book for professionals and general readers alike.The assumption that people's faces provide a window to their inner nature has a long and distinguished history, eloquently expressed in the works of ancient philosophers, like Aristotle, and great writers, like Shakespeare. Zebrowitz examines this assumption, focusing on four central points. She shows that facial appearance, particularly babyfaceness and attractiveness, has a strong impact on how we perceive an individual's character traits and on social outcomes in the workplace, in the criminal justice system, and in other settings. She proposes that facial stereotypes derive from evolutionarily adaptive reactions to useful information that faces can provide. She assesses the accuracy of facial stereotypes in light of plausible links between appearance and character. Finally, Zebrowitz suggests ways to counteract the consequences of reading faces.
Article
Inspired by the evolutionary conjecture that sexually selected traits function as indicators of pathogen resistance in animals and humans, we examined the notion that human facial attractiveness provides evidence of health. Using photos of 164 males and 169 females in late adolescence and health data on these individuals in adolescence, middle adulthood, and later adulthood, we found that adolescent facial attractiveness was unrelated to adolescent health for either males or females, and was not predictive of health at the later times. We also asked raters to guess the health of each stimulus person from his or her photo. Relatively attractive stimulus persons were mistakenly rated as healthier than their peers. The correlation between perceived health and medically assessed health increased when attractiveness was statistically controlled, which implies that attractiveness suppressed the accurate recognition of health. These findings may have important implications for evolutionary models.