ArticlePDF Available

Self-Report Bias and Underreporting of Depression on the BDI-II

Taylor & Francis
Journal of Personality Assessment
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

One problem in identifying and treating depression is underreporting of symptoms by individuals. Previous research suggests that there may be systematic sex differences in self-report bias,with men tending to minimize their depressive symptoms more than women. This study used an experimental design with a sample of 238 community members to test whether disguising the purpose of the Beck Depression Inventory-II would significantly reduce self-report bias, especially in men. We found a main effect of condition such that both men and women reported significantly more core depressive symptoms in the covert condition, suggesting that surveys of community samples may underestimate the prevalence of depression.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Self-Report Bias and Underreporting of Depression
on the BDI–II
HUNT, AURIEMMA, CASHAWSELF-REPORT BIAS ON THE BDI–II
Melissa Hunt, Joseph Auriemma, and Ashara C. A. Cashaw
Department of Psychology
University of Pennsylvania
One problem in identifying and treating depression is underreporting of symptoms by individu-
als. Previous research suggests that there may be systematic sex differences in self-report bias,
with men tending to minimize their depressive symptoms more than women. This study used an
experimental design with a sample of 238 community members to test whether disguising the
purpose of the Beck Depression Inventory–II would significantly reduce self-report bias, espe-
cially in men. We found a main effect of condition such that both men and women reported sig-
nificantly more core depressive symptoms in the covert condition, suggesting that surveys of
community samples may underestimate the prevalence of depression.
One of the major problems in the area of assessing and treat-
ing depression is that we must rely, to a large extent, on the
self-report of individuals. As a result, general prevalence
rates of unipolar depression based on community sampling
may significantly underestimate true rates of depression in
both men and women. Eaton, Neufeld, Chen, and Cai (2000)
examined data from the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment
Area follow-up and found a strong bias toward
underreporting depressive symptoms. This has profound im-
plications for access to treatment. Druss, Hoff, and
Rosenheck (2000), for example, found that a serious gap ex-
ists between the established efficacy of antidepressant medi-
cations and rates of treatment in the “real world. They
blamed this gap, in large part, on underreporting of depres-
sive symptoms. They also found that telling a primary care
provider about depressive symptoms predicted a 10-fold in-
crease in treatment.
Although underreporting is a problem for both sexes,
there is reason to believe that it is even more exaggerated in
men than in women. For example, Allen-Burge, Storandt,
Kinscherf, and Rubin (1994) found that the Beck Depression
Inventory (BDI) missed one fourth to one half of the index
cases of unipolar depression in male geriatric psychiatric in-
patients, depending on the cutoff score used. One of the most
widely cited epidemiological findings in mental health is the
2:1 sex difference in rates of unipolar depression (e.g.,
Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987; Weissman & Klerman, 1977).
Among the many explanations for this consistent finding is
that men may simply underreport depressive symptoms be-
cause it is socially undesirable for them to admit to problems
with mental health (e.g., Vredenburg, Krames, & Flett,
1986). The central assertion of this argument is that men ex-
perience depressive symptoms as frequently as women do,
but are less likely to admit it.
A number of studies have addressed the problem of
self-report bias on depression inventories, particularly with
respect to sex differences, but none have produced conclu-
sive results (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987). For example, Byrne
(1981) conducted a large-scale (n= 756) study of a commu-
nity sample. He found sex differences in the point prevalence
of depression. Although he discussed the possible impact of
social pressures and gender roles, he did not experimentally
manipulate those factors.
King and Buchwald (1982) conducted an experimental in-
vestigation of the role of self-report bias by comparing public
and private disclosure conditions in an undergraduate sam-
ple. They hypothesized that men in the public disclosure con-
dition (which consisted of a structured interview) would
have significantly lower BDI scores than women in a public
disclosure condition or men in a private disclosure condition
(which consisted of completing the BDI by themselves).
Their results did not support their hypothesis because they
did not find men to be any less willing to disclose symptoms
than women, even in the public condition. However, this re-
sult is hardly surprising in light of the fact that college stu-
dents are one of the few populations that rarely show the sex
difference in depression (Hammen & Padesky, 1977; Nolan
& Wilson, 1994; Stangler & Printz, 1980).
Also using an undergraduate sample, Page and Bennesch
(1993) attempted to experimentally manipulate social desir-
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT, 80(1), 26–30
Copyright © 2003, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
ability more subtly. Half of their participants completed the
BDI, accurately labeled as a “Depression” questionnaire.
The other half of their participants completed a BDI that was
disguised as a questionnaire addressing the “daily hassles of
normal living commonly experienced by all people.” To aid
the deception, they padded the BDI with items from the
Hopkins Symptom Checklist. They found that both men and
women reported more depressive symptoms in the masked
condition, but did not find evidence for a sex difference.
Again, this study used a college student sample, so the lack of
sex difference is not unexpected.
Although it is frequently assumed that self-report bias (i.e.,
the minimizing of actual symptoms) is due in part to the social
desirability of such reports for men versus women, very few
studies have examined the issue of social desirability directly.
For example, Clark, Crewsdon, and Purdon (1998) did find
that BDI scores were significantly negatively correlated with
scores on social desirability scales, but they did not address the
question of sex differences. Vredenburg et al. (1986), on the
other hand, investigated sex differences in the expression of
depressive symptoms in a sample of psychiatric patients. They
found that men were more likely to report
sex-role-appropriate symptoms such as work-related prob-
lems and somatic concerns, but they made no effort to experi-
mentallymanipulatethesocialdesirabilityofsuchreporting.
Finally, the BDI–II is a relatively new instrument. Al-
though its reliability and validity have been well established
(e.g., Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996; Steer, Ball, Ranieri, &
Beck, 1997), relatively few experimental studies with the
BDI–II exist. Moreover, although the original BDI was used
in numerous studies exploring sex differences in community
samples (e.g., Lips & Ng, 1986; Oliver & Simmons, 1985),
relatively few studies have examined sex differences in
self-report on this newer instrument and most have utilized
undergraduate samples (e.g., Dozois, Dobson, & Ahnberg,
1998).
This study is an effort to address self-report bias and
underreporting in a normative community sample using the
BDI–II. We propose to use a method similar to that of Page and
Bennesch (1993). That is, using the BDI–II as our core symp-
tom inventory, we devised two new questionnaires of equal
length. The first, accurately labeled “Depression Inventory,”
consisted of the BDI–II padded with other depression relevant
content items. The second, misleadingly labeled “Life Stress
Inventory,” consisted of the BDI–II padded with benign items
unlikely to trigger social desirability concerns.
We hypothesized that experimental condition would in-
fluence the number of depressive symptoms endorsed by
both men and women, but that men would show a greater ef-
fect. Specifically, we expected that both men and women
would score higher on the covert depression inventory than
on the overt depression inventory. Moreover, we expected
that men and women in the overt condition would show the
usual sex difference in depression, but that the sex difference
would be significantly reduced in the covert condition.
METHOD
Participants
There were a total of 238 participants (131 women and 107
men) in our sample. To obtain a broad community sample, par-
ticipants were recruited from varying venues including
churches, fitness centers, private companies, university per-
sonnel, and the jury selection pool for the City of Philadelphia.
The age of the group varied from 16 to 88 years of age, with a
mean age of 36.5 years. (For the participants who were under
the age of 18, parental consent was also obtained.) The women
in the sample had a mean age of 38, the men had a mean age of
34.5, ns. Approximately 60% of the sample was White and
37% were Black. All participants in the study were volunteers
and all returned fully completed questionnaires.
Procedure
This study used a2×2between-subjects design, comparing
men and women in both the control and experimental groups.
Participants were given a randomly selected packet contain-
ing a consent form, demographic/contact sheet, and ques-
tionnaire labeled either “Depression Inventory” or “Life
Stress Inventory. For all participants, the consent form indi-
cated that they were being invited to participate in a study of
“life stress.” On completing the study materials, participants
were debriefed and given a written copy of a debriefing letter.
All questions were answered at this point, and the partici-
pants were thanked for their participation.
Materials
The BDI–II is a 21-item self-report measure created to assess
the severity or intensity of depressive symptoms (Beck &
Steer, 1993, Beck et al., 1996). The BDI has been found to be
a reliable and valid instrument to measure depression in a va-
riety of normal and psychiatric populations (e.g., Beck, Steer,
& Garbin, 1988). According to the manual for the BDI–II,
scores ranging from 0 to 13 are considered not depressed,
scores from 14 to 19 mildly depressed, 20 to 28 moderately
depressed, and 29 to 36 severely depressed. Rather than man-
dating specific cutoff scores for research purposes, Beck et
al. recommended choosing cutoff scores carefully, depend-
ing on the need for either specificity or sensitivity. They
noted that a very conservative cutoff score of 17 yields a true
positive rate of 93% and a false positive rate of 18%. They
recommended that if the purpose of the study is to identify
the maximum number of possible cases of depression, cutoff
scores should be set somewhat lower, but still within the
range of scores (14 to 19) representing “mild depression.”
We chose to use a cutoff of 15 or greater for identifying prob-
able index cases of clinically significant or diagnosable ma-
jor depression as a sensitive indicator, but still conservatively
above the lowest score in the range.
SELF-REPORT BIAS ON THE BDI–II 27
In our study, the control group completed a questionnaire
clearly labeled “Depression Inventory.” This questionnaire
contained the 21 items from the BDI–II as well as 14 filler
items developed by the research team from sources such as
the Zung Self-Rating Scale for Depression (Zung, 1965). An
example of a depressed content filler item would be:
0 I find myself as attractive as usual
1 I feel less attractive than I used to be
2 I feel unattractive
3 I have never found myself attractive
The experimental group completed a questionnaire clearly
labeled “Life Stress Inventory.” This questionnaire contained
the 21-item BDI–II with 14 filler items that were developed by
the research team to reflect various examples of mild life
stressors.Anexampleofamildstressorfilleritemwouldbe:
0 Traffic does not bother me
1 Traffic is no more annoying to me than it is to anyone
else
2 Traffic often irritates me
3 Traffic is a major source of stress in my life
To score both sets of questionnaires, only the core BDI–II
items were counted.
RESULTS
The main finding of this study was that condition signifi-
cantly affected the number of depressive symptoms endorsed
by both men and women, whereas the effects of sex and the
interaction between sex and condition were not statistically
significant. A two-way analysis of variance yielded a signifi-
cant main effect of condition, F(1, 238) = 11.841, p=.001,
but showed no effect of either sex or the interaction of sex and
condition, both F(1, 238) < 1, both p> .30. Disguising the in-
tent of the depression inventory led both men and women to
report more depressive symptoms (see Figure 1).
A second test of the main effect of condition was con-
ducted using probable index cases of depression defined by a
cutoff score of 15 or higher on the BDI. Overall, 13% of par-
ticipants in the overt condition scored 15 or greater on the
BDI, whereas 34% of participants in the covert condition
scored at or above the cutoff (see Figure 2).
We also examined whether there were any sex differences
in the data using the index case method. Epidemiologically,
the finding is that twice as many women as men will experi-
ence clinically significant depressive episodes in their life-
time. The finding is not that women are “twice as depressed”
as men. Therefore, we reanalyzed the sex difference data us-
ing the cutoff score of 15. Looked at this way, we did find
some minimal suggestion of a sex difference in self-report
bias (see Figure 3).
In the overt condition, 16% of women were at or above the
cutoff, whereas only 10% of men were at or above the cutoff.
In the covert condition, 33% of men and 35% of women were
at or above the cutoff of 15 (see Table 1 for a summary of all
these findings). In other words, in the overt condition, we
found a sex difference in depression of approximately 3:2,
whereas in the covert condition, the ratio was approximately
1:1. However, the chi-square statistic for this difference was
not significant. Because we knew that there was a main effect
of condition for both men and women, we deemed it reason-
able to compare the effect sizes for each sex. For men, the in-
crease in depression was moderately strong, with an effect
size of d= .59. For women, the effect was somewhat less, at d
= .44. In comparing two effects sizes, it is also possible to
subtract one from the other. The resulting difference is itself
an effect size. For these data, the difference in effect size for
men and women is d= .15. Cohen would define this effect
28 HUNT, AURIEMMA, CASHAW
FIGURE 1 Mean Beck Depression Inventory score by condition.
FIGURE 2 Percentage depressed by condition.
FIGURE 3 Percentage depressed by condition and sex.
size as small. Although not compelling, these effect sizes
suggest that the manipulation had slightly more impact on
men than it did on women, but that this effect was minimal.
Finally, comparisons were also run for Whites versus
Blacks. There were no significant differences between these
two groups.
DISCUSSION
The main finding of this study was that masking the purpose
of the depression inventory led to greater endorsement of
symptoms by both men and women in a broad community
sample. Indeed, we found relatively high rates of depressive
symptoms, ranging from 10% to 35%. Because 1-month
prevalence rates for any affective disorder have been reported
to be 1.7% for men and 3.1% for women (Robins & Regier,
1991), our results raise questions about the effect of padding
the BDI with depression-congruent or benign items. It seems
possible that response set pulled our participant’s scores
higher than they “should” actually have been.
Other studies, however, have suggested that prevalence
rates of depression are, in fact, much higher. For example,
Brantley, Mehan, and Thomas (2000) reported that the prev-
alence of depressive disorders among the general populace is
approximately 17 to 24%. Moreover, approximately 21% of
patients being treated by family practice physicians describe
clinically significant depressive symptoms (Zung,
Broadhead, & Roth, 1993). Oliver and Simmons (1985)
compared current depression diagnoses based on either the
Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) or the BDI. In a sample
of 298 paid volunteers selected by random digit dialing, they
found that according to the DIS, 7.7% were diagnosed as cur-
rent affective disorder, whereas 19.8% scored depressed ac-
cording to the BDI. More recently, Eaton et al. (2000)
compared the DIS to the Schedule for Clinical Assessment in
Neuropsychiatry (or SCAN). Agreement between the two on
diagnoses of Major Depressive Disorder was only fair. In-
deed, the DIS, the instrument used most often in large
epidemiologic studies, actually missed many cases meeting
SCAN criteria. Many of these false negatives could be ex-
plained by individuals underreporting symptoms because
they attributed them to life stress. Interestingly, their results
suggested that the DIS was particularly likely to miss clini-
cally significant depression in men. Our results suggest that
prevalence rates of Major Depressive Disorder may indeed
be much higher than currently accepted prevalence rates.
However, our results are consistent with other recent studies
that have also found much higher rates of depressive disor-
ders and symptoms in the general population.
There were no significant sex differences in mean BDI
score for the entire sample or for either condition examined
separately. The lack of a significant sex difference in depres-
sive symptoms is somewhat surprising given the representa-
tive nature of the sample obtained. There was also no
significant interaction between sex and experimental condi-
tion on mean BDI score. That is, contrary to our predictions,
men’s mean BDI score was not affected by experimental
condition significantly more than women’s mean BDI score.
There were hints in these data, however, suggesting that
reducing self-report bias might mitigate the sex difference in
rates of index cases of depression. Specifically, in the overt
condition, probable index cases of depression occurred at a
ratio (women to men) of approximately 3:2, whereas in the
covert condition, the ratio dropped to 1:1. However, the re-
sults as they stand do not allow us to conclude that this differ-
ence was meaningful or reliable.
Because depression occurs at a relatively low rate, broad
community samples yield only a small percentage of de-
pressed cases. Power analyses showed that quadrupling our
total sample size (for a total sample size of approximately
1,000, but only 36 probable index cases of depressed men
across experimental conditions) would have provided suffi-
cient power to result in a significant chi-square. Unfortu-
nately, obtaining such a large community sample was
beyond the scope of this study.
One weakness of this study is the lack of a significant sex
difference in mean BDI or rates of depression, even in the
overt condition. This raises the possibility that our sample
was not comparable to the large epidemiological studies that
have found such a sex difference. Although we made every
effort to make our sample as broad and inclusive as possible,
we did not use methods such as random digit dialing. Clearly,
African Americans were overrepresented in our sample,
whereas Asian Americans and Hispanic Americans were
underrepresented. The possibility remains that our sample
was unusual, and this obviously limits the generalizability of
the findings. However, we believe the sample was more rep-
resentative of the population than the typical White under-
graduate sample of convenience. Although African
Americans may be overrepresented in our sample, they are
often not represented at all. Therefore, however limited our
results may be, they are clearly more generalizable than
much of the other research on this topic.
The second main weakness is the relatively low sample
size. Although an initial sample of 238 people may seem
large, our primary interest was in probable index cases of de-
SELF-REPORT BIAS ON THE BDI–II 29
TABLE 1
Summary of Results by Condition and Sex
Overt Covert
Male Female Male Female
N52 63 55 68
M(BDI–II) 7.08 8.32 11 11.71
SD 6.67 7.74 8.47 9.2
SEM 0.925 0.975 1.14 1.12
Ndepressed 5 10 18 24
% depressed 10 16 33 35
Note. BDI–II = Beck Depression Inventory–II.
pression. This is a problem that plagues any research relying
on normative samples to yield small percentages of index
cases of psychopathology. From that initial sample of 238,
we found only 57 individuals across both conditions who
were at or over the cutoff of 15 on the BDI–II. By the time the
sample was divided by sex and condition, there were as few
as 5 participants in some cells. It is our hope that this study
will be replicated using a larger sample to establish whether
the sex differences our data hint at actually exist.
Despite these drawbacks, this study suggests that our cur-
rent prevalence rates for unipolar depression (as estimated by
large cachement area studies) may be underestimates of true
rates of depression. Diagnostic practices must always find a
compromise between false negatives and false positives. The
covert administration of the BDI–II probably resulted in very
high sensitivity, but poor specificity. Perhaps primary care
providers would be well advised to couch their diagnostic que-
ries with both men and women in terms that are more socially
acceptable than “depression.” Asking an individual if he or she
is “under stress” might yield a more honest and fruitful admis-
sion of symptoms. Although this might result in some individ-
uals being prescribed antidepressants who do not need them,
research suggests that the alternative, in which depressed indi-
viduals will go untreated, is both much more common and
more serious. Despite significant progress in recent years,
mental health diagnoses still carry powerful stigma and are
likely to arouse strong self-presentational concerns and so-
cially desirable responding. Finding a way to help distressed
individuals acknowledge their symptoms should lead to better
diagnosis and treatment outcomes overall.
REFERENCES
Allen-Burge, R., Storandt, M., Kinscherf, D. A., & Rubin, E. H. (1994). Sex
differences in the sensitivity of two self-report depression scales in older
depressed inpatients. Psychology and Aging, 9, 443–445.
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Manual for the Beck Depression Inven-
tory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt, Brace.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck De-
pression Inventory (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora-
tion, Harcourt, Brace.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. (1988). Psychometric properties of
the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical
Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.
Brantley, P. J., Mehan, D. J., & Thomas, J. L. (2000). The Beck Depression In-
ventory and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. In M.
E. Maruish (Ed.), Handbook of psychological assessment in primary care
settings (pp. 391–421). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Byrne, D. (1981). Sex differences in reporting symptoms of depression in
the general population. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 83–92.
Clark, D., Crewsdon, N., & Purdon, C. (1998). No worries, no cares: An in-
vestigation into self-reported “nondistress” in college students. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 22, 209–224.
Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric
evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory–II. Psychological Assess-
ment, 10(2), 83–89.
Druss, B. G., Hoff, R. A., & Rosenheck, R. A. (2000). Underuse of anti-
depressants in major depression: Prevalence and correlates in a na-
tional sample of young adults. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61,
234–237.
Eaton, W. W., Neufeld, K., Chen, L. S., & Cai, G. (2000). A comparison of
self-report and clinical diagnostic interviews for depression: Diagnostic
Interview Schedule and Schedules for Clinical Assessment in
Neuropsychiatry in the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area fol-
low-up. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 217–222.
Hammen, C., & Padesky, C. (1977). Sex differences in expression of depres-
sive responses on the Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 86, 609–614.
King, D., & Buchwald, A. (1982). Sex differences in subclinical depression:
Administration of the Beck Depression Inventory in public and private
disclosure situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42,
963–969.
Lips, H. M., & Ng, M. (1986). Use of the Beck Depression Inventory with
three nonclinical populations. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science,
18(1), 62–74.
Nolan, R., & Wilson, V. (1994). Gender and depression in an undergraduate
population. Psychological Reports, 75, 1327–1330.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evi-
dence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259–277.
Oliver, J. M., & Simmons, M. E. (1985). Affective disorders and depression
as measured by the Diagnostic Interview Schedule and the Beck Depres-
sion Inventory in an unselected adult population. Journal of Clinical Psy-
chology, 41, 469–477.
Page, S., & Bennesch, S. (1993). Gender and reporting differences in mea-
sures of depression. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 25,
579–589.
Robins, L. N., & Regier, D. A. (Eds.). (1991). Psychiatric disorders in Amer-
ica: The ECA study. New York: Free Press.
Stangler, R., & Printz, A. (1980). DSM–III: Psychiatric diagnosis in univer-
sity populations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 937–940.
Steer, R. A., Ball, R., Ranieri, W. F., & Beck, A. T. (1997). Further evidence
for the construct validity of the BDI–II with psychiatric outpatients. Psy-
chological Reports,80, 443–446.
Vredenburg, K., Krames, L., & Flett, G. (1986). Sex differences in the clini-
cal expression of depression. Sex Roles, 14, 37–49.
Weissman, M., & Klerman, G. (1977). Sex differences and the epidemiology
of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 98–109.
Zung, W. W. (1965). A self-rating scale for depression. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 12(1), 63–70.
Zung, W. W., Broadhead, W. E., & Roth, M. E. (1993). Prevalence of de-
pressive symptoms in primary care. Journal of Family Practice, 37,
337–344.
Melissa Hunt
Department of Psychology
University of Pennsylvania
3815 Walnut Street
Philadelphia, PA 19104–6196
E-mail: mhunt@cattell.psych.upenn.edu
Received September 24, 2001
Revised June 23, 2002
30 HUNT, AURIEMMA, CASHAW
... In this study, depression was defined as a BDI-II score of 14 or higher. 22 The self-administered PSQI questionnaire assesses sleep quality over the past month. Scores range from 0 to 21, with lower scores indicating better sleep quality. ...
... It is important to acknowledge that our participant couples were aware of the study's purpose due to ethical constraints. Previous well-controlled and well-conducted research has identified a main effect of condition, indicating that both males and females reported significantly higher levels of insomnia severity, 48 depression, 22 and anxiety symptoms in the covert condition. 49 This suggests that we may have underestimated or underreported the frequency of insomnia, depression, and anxiety in male spouses, while potentially overestimating or overreporting these parameters in female spouses. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and objectives Oral microbiota dysbiosis and altered salivary cortisol levels have been linked to depression and anxiety. Given that bacterial transmission can occur between spouses, this study aimed to investigate whether the transmission of oral microbiota between newlywed couples mediates symptoms of depression and anxiety. Methods Validated Persian versions of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, Beck Depression Inventory-II, and Beck Anxiety Inventory were administered to 1,740 couples who had been married for six months. The researchers compared 268 healthy control spouses with 268 affected cases in a cross-sectional study. Data were analyzed using appropriate statistical methods. Results After six months, healthy spouses married to an insomniac with the depression-anxiety (DA) phenotype scored significantly higher on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, Beck Depression Inventory-II, and Beck Anxiety Inventory compared to their baseline scores. This indicates that their sleep quality, depression, and anxiety scores became more similar to those of their affected spouses. Additionally, the composition of their oral microbiota changed significantly, becoming increasingly similar to that of their spouses. Specifically, in couples where one partner had the DA phenotype, the oral microbiota of the healthy spouse mirrored that of the affected partner (p < 0.001). These microbial changes correlated with alterations in salivary cortisol levels as well as depression and anxiety scores. Linear discriminant analysis revealed that the relative abundances of Clostridia, Veillonella, Bacillus, and Lachnospiraceae were significantly higher in insomniacs with the DA phenotype compared to healthy controls (p < 0.001). Conclusions Oral microbiota transmission between individuals in close contact partially mediates symptoms of depression and anxiety.
... As the reasons for refusal are unavailable, we can only speculate with caution that some patients may have felt more uncomfortable in marking their suicidal symptoms on paper by themselves than in expressing them verbally to a nurse. A previous study reported that, when self-reporting, depressive symptoms may be underreported (Hunt et al. 2003), which could be related to a stigma of psychiatric illness, known to be prevalent among Korean elderly patients . Even though the nurse-interview mode was more likely to be accepted, it cannot be concluded that it is a better measure in terms of acceptance because the variables differed between the acceptors and refusers of the different screening modes (Table 3). ...
Article
Full-text available
Specific differences between interviewer‐rated and self‐report suicide risk assessments are unknown. This descriptive cross‐sectional study examined the acceptance of the Columbia‐Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C‐SSRS) screener, conducted by nurse‐interviewers versus self‐reporting, and identified the factors contributing to refusal in each mode. We reviewed the charts of inpatients at a general hospital that included the C‐SSRS screener at the time of admission. C‐SSRS screening by a nurse‐interviewer and self‐reported mode were included. Two modes and the acceptors and refusers were compared using descriptive statistics. Multi‐variate logistic regression was performed to determine associations between the studied factors and suicide risk. The study included 398 patients. The acceptance of the nurse‐interview and self‐report modes was 93.8% and 74.7%, respectively. Refusers for the former were more likely to be male ( p = 0.029) and from the surgical departments ( p < 0.001). Refusers for the latter tended to be older ( p < 0.001). Compared to the nurse‐interview group, the self‐report group was more likely to report a high risk of suicide (odds ratio = 6.508, p = 0.041). Each mode of the C‐SSRS screener, when used in general hospital inpatients, has its own advantages and disadvantages. Healthcare providers should select the mode optimal for their institutional environment after comprehensively considering the patients' characteristics and available hospital resources.
... The questionnaires used in this study contain highly intrusive inquiries, including questions on adverse personal experiences and depressive tendencies. Furthermore, a significant number of mothers in the maltreatment group experienced severe depressive symptoms, raising the possibility of response bias 34 . Second, no significant difference in salivary cortisol levels was observed between the maltreatment and non-maltreatment groups; p = 0.07. ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding the intergenerational transmission of childhood maltreatment (CM) is crucial to prevent its perpetuation to future generations. The literature has revealed parental empathy to be a pivotal factor in this process. Parental empathy is the ability to comprehend and empathetically respond to the emotions and mental states of children; therefore, its impairment may result in challenges in parenting, ultimately contributing to CM. In this study, we explored the factors that undermine empathy and how they alter parenting practices, to uncover the process of intergenerational transmission of CM. To investigate actual instances of CM, a comparative study was conducted between 13 mothers with a history of social interventions owing to CM and 42 mothers in the control group. Path analysis was performed to examine the trajectory from adverse childhood experiences to maltreatment and explore their correlations with variables including affective and cognitive empathy, depressive symptoms, and parenting styles. The results showed that experiences of CM specifically enhanced empathy in the emotional domain in the maltreatment group. Furthermore, heightened empathy in the maltreatment group influenced parenting style mediated by depressive symptoms. These results provide important insights into the intergenerational transmission process in the context of parental empathy.
... Second, self-reported data can be influenced by respondents' individual perceptions (Jylhä, 2009). While previous studies have supported the validity and reliability of self-reported health information (Halford et al., 2012;Pu et al., 2013;Santos et al., 2021), the stigma surrounding depression may contribute to self-report bias (Chan and Mak, 2017;Hunt et al., 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Previous studies have identified socioeconomic inequalities in the treatment of depression. However, these studies often take a narrow approach, focusing on a single treatment type and lacking a comprehensive theoretical framework. Moreover, income and education are frequently used interchangeably as indicators of disadvantage, without distinguishing their unique impacts. This study argues that relying solely on income to explain treatment inequalities is overly simplistic, suggesting instead that education influences treatment through two distinct pathways. The study’s objectives are twofold: first, to investigate the presence of a social gradient in depression treatment, and second, to examine how this gradient is manifested. Methods This study utilizes data from the Belgian Health Interview Survey (BHIS), covering four successive waves: 2004, 2008, 2013, and 2018. The weighted data represent a sample of the adult Belgian population. Multinomial regression models are used to address the research aims, and models are plotted to detect trends over time using marginal means post-estimation. Results Findings indicate that income is not significantly related to depression treatment, while persistent educational inequalities in treatment are observed over time. Individuals with longer educational attainment are more likely to use psychotherapy alone or a combination treatment, whereas individuals with shorter educational attainment are more likely to use pharmaceutical treatment alone. Discussion This study demonstrates that education plays a critical role in fostering health-related knowledge and reasoning, making individuals with longer education more likely to engage in rational health behaviors and choose more effective treatments, even when these treatments require more effort and competencies. The findings underscore the importance of considering education as a key determinant of depression treatment inequalities.
... It is important to acknowledge that our participant couples were aware of the purpose of the study, due to the ethical constraints. A main effect of condition has been found in previous well-controlled and wellconducted researches such that both males and females reported significantly more insomnia severity [56][57] , depression 21,58 and anxiety symptoms [59][60] in the covert condition, suggesting that we may have underestimated/under-reported the frequency of insomnia, depression and anxiety in males spouses while have overestimated/over-reported these parameter in female spouses. Despite controlling for the most important covariates, additional residual confounding may exist. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Oral microbiota dysbiosis and salivary cortisol are associated with depression and anxiety. Bacterial transmission can occur between spouses. Aims: We explored whether oral microbiota, salivary cortisol and a combined depression-anxiety (DA) phenotype affiliated in newly married couples. Methods: The researchers administered validated Persian versions of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory (PSQI), Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), and Beck Anxiety Inventory to 1740 couples, who had been married during the past six months. The investigators compared 296 healthy control spouses with 296 cases. Data analysis used appropriate statistical methods. Results: After six months, at the phyla level, we identified a significant decrease in Firmicutes and Actinomycetota abundance and an increase in Bacteroidetes, Proteobacteria, Fusobacteria and Patescibacteria abundance in healthy spouses married to an insomniac with DA-phenotype, showing that oral microbiota were significantly changed and became similar to that of participant's spouse, (i.e., if the spouse had DA phenotype, then composition of oral microbiota became similar to their spouse DA phenotype, p < 0.001). These changes paralleled alterations in salivary cortisol, depression and anxiety scores. Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) showed that relative abundances of Clostridia, Veillonella, Bacillus and Lachnospiraceae were significantly higher in insomniacs with DA-phenotype than healthy controls (p < 0.001). Results remained significant after controlling for confounders. The formal mediation analysis confirmed these outcomes. We observed distinct analogous gender differences for oral microbiota pattern, salivary cortisol level, and depression and anxiety scores. Conclusions: Microbiota transmission between two people in close contact with one another partially mediated depression and anxiety. Keywords: Oral microbiota, bacterial transmission, depression, anxiety, salivary cortisol
... Although the patient-reported WAI ratings are the best predictor of treatment outcome currently available in psychotherapy Flückiger, Rubel, et al., 2020), these ratings likely inherit any bias or noise in those patient-reported scores. Self-report measures are likely to be affected by the patient's ability, awareness, and motivation to report on the therapeutic process (Hunt et al., 2003), do not account for individual trait characteristics, and are vulnerable to (un) intentional reporting biases (Pampouchidou et al., 2019;Snowden, 2003). Arguably, assessment of patient self-reported working alliance might not provide valid data about the therapeutic relationship (McHorney & Tarlov, 1995) because patients are often hesitant to express any concerns or disappointment with the clinician or treatment (Rennie, 1994), as reflected by ceiling effects in alliance measurements (e.g., Meier & Feeley, 2022). ...
Article
Objective.Session-by-session tracking of the working alliance enables clinicians to detect alliance deterioration and intervene accordingly, which has shown to improve treatment outcome, and reduce dropout. Despite this, regular use of alliance self-report measures has failed to gain widespread implementation. We aimed to develop an automated alliance prediction using behavioral features obtained from video-recorded therapy sessions. Method: A naturalistic dataset of session recordings with patient-ratings of working alliance was available for 252 in-person and teletherapy sessions from 47 patients treated by 10 clinicians. Text and audio-based features were extracted from all 252 sessions. Additional video-based feature extraction was possible for a subsample of 80 sessions. We developed a modeling pipeline for audio and text and for audio, text and video to train machine learning regression models that fuse multimodal features. Results: Best results were achieved with a Gradient Boosting architecture, when using audio, text, and video features extracted from the patient (ICC = 0.66, Pearson r = 0.70, MAE = 0.33). Conclusion: Automated alliance prediction from video-recorded therapy sessions is feasible with high accuracy. A datadriven multimodal approach to feature extraction and selection enables powerful models, outperforming previous work.
... Another limitation involves the reliance on self-report measures, which may result in underreporting of symptoms, particularly among men (Hunt et al., 2003). This concern is compounded by individual differences in the perception of symptom severity, especially regarding depression in men (Papageorgiou et al., 2015). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Problem-gambling, an escalating global concern, disproportionately affects men. This study investigated the joint effects of social support and loneliness on the well-established link between depressive-vulnerability and problem-gambling. Utilising a large representative survey of Australian men (n = 5,204) ranging between 16 and 64 years (M = 44.25, SD = 12.08) who completed measures of problem-gambling, depressive-vulnerability, loneliness, and social support in 2020 and then again in 2022, a moderated-mediation model was tested. Even after controlling for several covariates, the results supported the hypothesis that the effects of depressive-vulnerability on problem-gambling are influenced by social support and loneliness, R2 = .262, F (12, 5191) = 153.55, p < .001. Notably, at high levels of loneliness, low social support correlated with higher problem-gambling (DE = -.005, LLCI/ULCI ̸= 0). These findings underscore the critical role of social support in mitigating problem-gambling among lonely individuals with depressive-vulnerability and offer potential implications for intervention strategies.
Article
Aim To synthesise existing evidence concerning the application of AI methods in detecting depression through behavioural cues among adults in healthcare and community settings. Design This is a diagnostic accuracy systematic review. Methods This review included studies examining different AI methods in detecting depression among adults. Two independent reviewers screened, appraised and extracted data. Data were analysed by meta‐analysis, narrative synthesis and subgroup analysis. Data Sources Published studies and grey literature were sought in 11 electronic databases. Hand search was conducted on reference lists and two journals. Results In total, 30 studies were included in this review. Twenty of which demonstrated that AI models had the potential to detect depression. Speech and facial expression showed better sensitivity, reflecting the ability to detect people with depression. Text and movement had better specificity, indicating the ability to rule out non‐depressed individuals. Heterogeneity was initially high. Less heterogeneity was observed within each modality subgroup. Conclusions This is the first systematic review examining AI models in detecting depression using all four behavioural cues: speech, texts, movement and facial expressions. Implications A collaborative effort among healthcare professionals can be initiated to develop an AI‐assisted depression detection system in general healthcare or community settings. Impact It is challenging for general healthcare professionals to detect depressive symptoms among people in non‐psychiatric settings. Our findings suggested the need for objective screening tools, such as an AI‐assisted system, for screening depression. Therefore, people could receive accurate diagnosis and proper treatments for depression. Reporting Method This review followed the PRISMA checklist. Patients or Public Contribution No patients or public contribution.
Article
Full-text available
Objective Parents of children with a chronic condition (CC) have a high prevalence of mental health (MH) difficulties. It is not known whether establishing screening programmes in paediatric clinics to identify parental MH difficulties increases detection or referrals to support services. We aimed to identify approaches to routine screening programmes for parents of children with a CC attending hospital outpatient clinics (aim 1); associated prevalence of MH symptoms (aim 2); and whether screening impacted referrals to, and uptake of, MH services (aim 3). Design Medline, Embase, PsycINFO, CINAHL and PubMed databases were searched between January 2000 and December 2023. Studies were selected if they conducted routine screening of MH of parents of children with CCs (aged <18 years). Study characteristics, population demographics and information on screening tools, MH symptoms and referral pathways were extracted. Results Eight articles met the inclusion criteria from 8673 screened. The prevalence of elevated parental MH symptoms ranged between 9.6% and 62.9% for anxiety and 7.7% and 57.0% for depression. Two studies using the Distress Thermometer for Parents found 3.3%–57.0% had elevated levels of ‘clinical distress’. There was limited detail on referral pathways, referrals made and uptake. Conclusions Elevated MH symptoms are common in parents of children with CCs, but there is wide variability in outcomes. More research is required to understand this and how best to identify and screen for and support parents with referrals to and uptake of services for their MH. PROSPERO registration number CRD42023438720.
Article
Full-text available
To test the idea that the preponderance of female depressives is due to a masculine tendency to avoid negative social consequences by reporting fewer depressive symptoms, the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) was administered to 106 male and 104 female undergraduates under conditions of public and private disclosure. It was hypothesized that males would score lower on the BDI than females in the public disclosure condition but not males in the private disclosure condition. Results of both studies fail to support the experimental hypothesis. However, ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between sex of S and sex of examiner. Results are discussed in terms of a willingness to admit more symptoms to a same-sex person due to fear of rejection by the opposite sex and in terms of gender-specific patterns of self-disclosure in first-encounter heterosexual situations. Results also suggest that BDI scores of college students can be interpreted without regard to type of administration. (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
This article provides psychometric information on the second edition of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI–II; A. T. Beck, R. A. Steer, & G. K. Brown, 1996), with respect to internal consistency, factorial validity, and gender differences. Both measures demonstrated high internal reliability in the full student sample. Significant differences between the mean BDI and BDI-II scores necessitated the development of new cutoffs for analogue research on the BDI–II. Results from exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses indicated that a 2-factor solution optimally summarized the data for both versions of the inventory and accounted for a cumulative 41% and 46% of the common variance in BDI and BDI–II responses, respectively. These factor solutions were reliably cross-validated, although the importance of each factor varied by gender. The authors conclude that the BDI–II is a stronger instrument than the BDI in terms of its factor structure. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
135 female and 75 male undergraduates (aged 18–30 yrs) responded to the Beck Depression Inventory and to 9 items assessing depression from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist. The hypothesis was explored that males particularly would endorse test items in a less "depressed" direction when presented explicitly as constituting a test of depression, but would endorse more depressive content when items were presented in a context not portrayed as measuring depression. Some support was obtained for the view that males may approach and respond differently to depression inventories compared to females. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Administered the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) to samples from 3 different nonclinical populations (402 introductory psychology students, 101 female [mean age 27.8 yrs] and 94 male [mean age 29.5 yrs] expectant parents, and 151 female [mean age 30.3 yrs] and 117 male [mean age 32.3 yrs] married adults). Responses to the BDI were subjected to principal components analysis. The factors extracted differed among the 3 samples, with the only factor common to all 3 groups being Negative Self-View. Analyses showed that the sex differences were largest for the expectant parents group and smallest for the adult couples group. Findings have implications for the choice of control or comparison groups in studies of depression. (French abstract) (31 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Background The field of psychiatric epidemiology continues to employ self-report instruments, but the low degree of agreement between diagnoses achieved using these instruments vs that achieved by psychiatrists in the clinical modality threatens the credibility of the results.Methods In the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area follow-up, 349 individuals who had a Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) interview were blindly examined by psychiatrists using the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN). Comparisons were made at the level of diagnosis, syndrome, and DSM-IV symptom group. Indexes of agreement were computed and characteristics of discrepant cases were identified.Results Agreement on diagnosis of major depressive disorder was only fair (κ = 0.20), with the DIS missing many cases judged to meet criteria for diagnosis using the SCAN (29% sensitivity). A major source of discrepancy was respondents with false-negative diagnoses who repeatedly failed to report DIS symptoms attributed to life crises or medical conditions. Older age, male sex, and lower impairment were associated with underdetection by the DIS, using logistic regression analysis. In spite of the diagnostic discrepancy, there was substantial correlation in numbers of symptom groups in the 2 modalities (r = 0.49). Agreement was highest (about 55% sensitivity and 90% specificity) when both the SCAN and DIS thresholds were set at the level of depression syndrome instead of diagnosis.Conclusions Weak agreement at the level of diagnosis continues to threaten the credibility of estimates of prevalence of specific disorders. A bias toward underreporting, as well as stronger agreement at the level of the depression syndrome and on ordinal measures of depressive symptoms, suggests that associations with risk factors are conservative.
Article
Background: Epidemiologic studies have reported disturbingly low rates of treatment for major depression in the United States. To better understand this phenomenon, we studied the prevalence and predictors of antidepressant treatment in a national sample of individuals with major depression. Method: Between 1988 and 1994, 7589 individuals, aged 17-39 years and drawn from a national probability sample, were administered the Diagnostic Interview Schedule as part of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Interviewers asked about prescription drug use and checked medication bottles to record the name and type of medications. Results: A total of 312 individuals, or 4.1% of the sample, met DSM-III criteria for current major depression. Only 7.4% of those with current major depression were being treated with an antidepressant. Among individuals with current major depression, being insured and having a primary care provider each predicted a 4-fold increase in odds of antidepressant treatment; telling the primary provider about depressive symptoms predicted a 10 fold increase in treatment. Conclusion: The study's findings support the notion that a serious gap exists between the established efficacy of antidepressant medications and rates of treatment for major depression in the "real world." Underreporting of depressive symptoms to providers and problems with access to general medical care appear to be 2 major contributors to this problem.
Article
An important methodological issue in depressionanalog research is whether individuals who scoreextremely low on self-report measures like the BeckDepression Inventory (BDI) should be included innondepressed control groups. Joiner, Schmidt, and Metalsky(1994) found that college students with BDI scores of 0or 1 evidenced a fake-good test taking style as measuredby the MMPI validity scales. The present study investigated whether very low BDI scores (BDI= 0 or 1; n = 21) might be associated with an elevatedpositive mood state, extreme optimism, positiveattributional style or social desirability. Resultsindicated that the very low scoring BDI subjects scoredhigher on social desirability than the low scoring group(BDI = 2 9, n = 63). Significant differences on mood,symptom and cognitive measures disappeared when social desirability was entered as a covariate.Findings support Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hammen, andIngram's (1987) recommendation that subjects who score0 or 1 on the BDI should be excluded from a nondepressed control group.
Article
Recent epidemiological studies have established that the lifetime prevalence rate of depression is greater in women than in men. It was the purpose of the present study to investigate the possibility that the true prevalence of male depression is underestimated because males have learned through social rejection that it is inappropriate for them to openly express depressive feelings. Consistent with this notion that men only express depressive symptoms consonant with their traditional male sex role, a discriminant function analysis performed on the self-reported symptomatology of depressed patients revealed that men were more likely to report sex role appropriate symptoms such as work-related problems and somatic concerns. Since other self-presentational concerns may contribute to the sex difference in depression, it is suggested that future research directly examine the ways in which men experience and express symptoms of depression.
Article
Research studies focusing on the psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) with psychiatric and nonpsychiatric samples were reviewed for the years 1961 through June, 1986. A meta-analysis of the BDI's internal consistency estimates yielded a mean coefficient alpha of 0.86 for psychiatric patients and 0.81 for nonpsychiatric subjects. The concurrent validitus of the BDI with respect to clinical ratings and the Hamilton Psychiatric Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) were also high. The mean correlations of the BDI samples with clinical ratings and the HRSD were 0. 72 and 0.73, respectively, for psychiatric patients. With nonpsychiatric subjects, the mean correlations of the BDI with clinical ratings and the HRSD were 0.60 and 0.74, respectively. Recent evidence indicates that the BDI discriminates subtypes of depression and differentiates depression from anxiety.