ArticlePublisher preview available

When Do Stereotypes Come to Mind and When Do They Color Judgment? A Goal-Based Theoretical Framework for Stereotype Activation and Application

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The authors describe a theoretical framework for understanding when people interacting with a member of a stereotyped group activate that group's stereotype and apply it to that person. It is proposed that both stereotype activation and stereotype application during interaction depend on the strength of comprehension and self-enhancement goals that can be satisfied by stereotyping one's interaction partner and on the strength of one's motivation to avoid prejudice. The authors explain how these goals can promote and inhibit stereotype activation and application, and describe diverse chronic and situational factors that can influence the intensity of these goals during interaction and, thereby, influence stereotype activation and application. This approach permits integration of a broad range of findings on stereotype activation and application.
When Do Stereotypes Come to Mind and When Do They Color Judgment?
A Goal-Based Theoretical Framework for Stereotype Activation and
Application
Ziva Kunda and Steven J. Spencer
University of Waterloo
The authors describe a theoretical framework for understanding when people interacting with a member
of a stereotyped group activate that group’s stereotype and apply it to that person. It is proposed that both
stereotype activation and stereotype application during interaction depend on the strength of compre-
hension and self-enhancement goals that can be satisfied by stereotyping one’s interaction partner and on
the strength of one’s motivation to avoid prejudice. The authors explain how these goals can promote and
inhibit stereotype activation and application, and describe diverse chronic and situational factors that can
influence the intensity of these goals during interaction and, thereby, influence stereotype activation and
application. This approach permits integration of a broad range of findings on stereotype activation and
application.
As one interacts with a member of a stereotyped group—for
example, an African American, a woman, a doctor—the stereotype
of that group may or may not come to mind. When it does, it may
or may not color one’s impression of that person. These two
processes have been labeled stereotype activation, that is, the
extent to which a stereotype is accessible in one’s mind, and
stereotype application, that is, the extent to which one uses a
stereotype to judge a member of the stereotyped group. We ex-
amine what determines the activation and the application of ste-
reotypes and how these two processes relate to one another. We
propose that both the activation and the application of stereotypes
depend on three kinds of goals: comprehension goals, self-
enhancement goals, and the motivation to avoid prejudice.
1
We
explain how these goals can promote and inhibit the activation and
the application of stereotypes, and describe situational and chronic
factors that can trigger these goals and enable their execution.
Conceptualizing and Assessing Stereotype Activation and
Stereotype Application
There has always been a clear theoretical distinction between
the accessibility of knowledge structures such as stereotypes and
their use in judgment. Category accessibility was conceived as a
state of perceptual readiness that makes the category available for
use in judgments such as identification, categorization, and infer-
ence about category members (Bruner, 1957). Empirically, how-
ever, the distinction between the increased accessibility of catego-
ries and their increased use in judgment has sometimes been
muddied because categories manifest their accessibility through
their impact on judgment. Therefore, increases in the accessibility
of categories have typically been inferred from increases in their
use (e.g., Srull & Wyer, 1979; for a review, see Higgins, 1989).
However, the extent of stereotype activation cannot always be
inferred from stereotype application. When perceivers apply a
stereotype to a member of the stereotyped group, it can be assumed
that they have also activated the stereotype, because a stereotype
cannot be applied without first being activated. But when perceiv-
ers do not apply a stereotype, it cannot be assumed that they have
not activated it either, because people do not always apply their
activated stereotypes (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). For example, per-
ceivers who encounter an Asian American woman may activate
the Asian American stereotype, thereby experiencing heightened
accessibility of stereotypic traits such as shy and intelligent, but
they may refrain from applying this activated stereotype to this
individual, that is, they may not judge her as especially shy or
intelligent. People may avoid applying an activated stereotype
because they are motivated to avoid prejudice (cf. Devine, 1989;
Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). Nevertheless, its increased activation
may still be detectable through implicit measures that tap other
judgments that the perceivers cannot control or that they are not
motivated to control because they do not realize that their reactions
can reveal stereotype activation (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995).
Several implicit measures of stereotype activation have been
developed. Increased accessibility of stereotypic attributes can be
revealed by faster identification of stereotypic words (Kawakami,
1
We view the terms motivation to avoid prejudice and goal of avoiding
prejudice as interchangeable. We use motivation throughout because that is
the term most commonly used in other articles on this topic (e.g., Dunton
& Fazio, 1997; Plant & Devine, 1998).
This research was supported by Grants 410-2002-0964 and 410-99-0993
from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and
Grant 138483-98 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research
Council of Canada. We thank the stereotype discussion group at the
University of Waterloo for many helpful contributions to the development
of these ideas, and Christian Jordan and Mark Zanna for comments on an
earlier version of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to either
Ziva Kunda or Steven J. Spencer, Department of Psychology, Univer-
sity of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada. E-mail: zkunda@
watarts.uwaterloo.ca or sspencer@watarts.uwaterloo.ca
Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2003, Vol. 129, No. 4, 522–544 0033-2909/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.522
522
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
... Negative stereotypes about the outgroup justify discrimination and substitute accurate information with generalized, negative images . When stereotypes are activated, outgroup membership becomes more prevalent, enabling discriminatory decisions (Kunda and Spencer 2003;Bodenhausen and Richeson, 2010). Stereotypes about Roma people in Hungary are overwhelmingly negative, depicting them as non-competent, non-trustworthy, and prone to criminality (Kende et al. 2021). ...
... In Study 1, the fact that Roma applicants were writing emails in a language signifying higher educational level and higher socio-cultural status, may have disconfirmed stereotypes and thus, led to the lack of unequal treatment (Hewstone 1989). This effect was countered by activating a strong stereotype in Study 2. Results are in line with the literature on stereotype activation, suggesting that in case stereotypes about an outgroup member are activated, chances of expressing prejudice and negative treatment are higher (Kunda and Spencer 2003). Also, the stereotypical information itself may have drawn attention to the name, used as the ethnic trope, that otherwise might have been overlooked (Abramova 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we examine children’s ethnic discrimination in access to sport. We conducted two field experiments to test whether Roma applicants get greater rejection rates when applying to sport clubs and whether this rate is higher in case Roma applicants have a lower-class background. Basketball, volleyball, and soccer coaches were contacted by e-mail using Roma and non-Roma sounding names, requesting to participate in a trial practice. Previous findings show persistent discrimination of Roma people in Central and Eastern Europe in the labor and housing market and in access to various public services. Our findings in the field of sport in Hungary show that there is significant ethnic discrimination only when a stereotype about Roma people being lower class is activated. Further, gender was found to have a significant effect, with higher positive response rates for mothers applying for a trial session for a daughter, than for a son, regardless of their ethnicity. Our findings suggest a prevailing intersection of class and ethnicity, though our data does not allow for the separation of these two effects. These differences are discussed to better understand the role of intersectionality in discrimination within sport.
... For example, Wiese et al. (2017) found that services that humanoid robots provide convey competence and warmth. In addition, social cognitive activation affects individuals' perceptions and behaviors (Kunda and Spencer 2003). Thus, social cognitive theory has the potential to provide an especially nuanced perspective for understanding consumer responses to AI service quality. ...
Article
Full-text available
Using a social cognitive theory perspective, this study examines how AI service quality influences perceived competence, perceived warmth, and consumer well-being. We tested the hypothesized relationships using structural equation modelling with data collected from 580 consumers in China. The results showed that four dimensions of AI service quality (i.e., technical security, technical reliability, perceived personalization, and hyperconnectivity service capability) are important drivers of perceived competence and perceived warmth. Furthermore, perceived competence and perceived warmth significantly affect consumer well-being. Last, employee service quality commitment significantly moderates the relationship between AI service quality and perceived competence and perceived warmth.
... This study has several caveats that warrant acknowledgment. First, the participants were recruited through a non-probability sampling method which could introduce selection bias and provide inaccurate results [119,120]. Thus, there must be discretion in interpreting the data as a representative sample of the Filipino population. Future research may utilize probability sampling techniques with measures that reduce biases of all forms to improve the generalizability of the findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The WHO-5 well-being index is a brief rating scale extensively used to evaluate well-being symptoms. Despite the increasing number of studies validating this instrument across different samples from different countries, its psychometric properties remain unexplored in the Philippine context. Bridging this gap, the present study assessed the psychometric properties of the WHO-5 in Filipinos amid the pandemic. Methods In study one, exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) was conducted using a sample of Filipinos (N = 2,521) from the general population and a unidimensional model of well-being was extracted. In study two, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was employed to examine the one-factor model in 1,289 Filipino government workers. In study three, nomological validity was examined by performing a mediation analysis using 407 Filipino left-behind emerging adult children with dysfunctionality as mediator, pandemic-related adversities as independent variable, and well-being as the dependent variable. Results The results of ESEM and CFA provided support for the WHO-5 one-factor model. Moreover, the negative relationship of well-being to anxiety, depression, and distress lend evidence to the scale’s criterion validity. The results of the mediation analysis performed in study three implied that those who experienced pandemic-related adversities tended to have greater dysfunctionality, and in turn, had lower levels of well-being. Conclusions Overall, the findings suggest that the WHO-5 well-being index is a psychometrically sound tool for measuring Filipinos’ well-being.
... First, individuals may use general models of how people react to the world, which can then be used to make inferences about other people's mental states, personalities, and behaviors (Van Overwalle, 2009;Wimmer & Perner, 1983). Second, individuals may activate stereotype-specific knowledge when making social inferences and fill in gaps in knowledge, particularly when they possess minimal knowledge about a target or perceive them as dissimilar (Ames, 2004b;Kunda et al., 2002;Kunda & Spencer, 2003). Finally, people may also activate person-specific theories of mind (Welborn & Lieberman, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Simulating other people can shift one’s self-concept, an effect known as simulation-induced malleability. How does imagining others shift the self? We propose that the activation of self-knowledge is the key factor by which simulation of others alters one’s self-concept. We test this possibility across four studies that each manipulate self-knowledge activation indirectly during simulation and measure the impact on subsequent self-ratings. Results demonstrate that increasing activation of self-knowledge during simulation is associated with increased self-concept change. People experienced greater self-concept change when simulating similar others (Studies 1 and 2). People also generalized simulation-induced changes to aspects of the self-concept that were semantically similar to the simulated content (Study 3). Finally, people who are less likely to recruit self-knowledge (i.e., older adults) during simulation were less susceptible to self-concept change (Study 4). These studies highlight self-knowledge activation as an essential component of the effects of simulation on self-rated change.
... Research in interpersonal interactions reveals that we are especially likely to apply stereotypes if they serve a comprehension goal or if other attributes of the situation make the stereotype salient (Kunda & Spencer, 2003;. When a story is unfolding, we are motivated to comprehend the situations and characters that we encounter. ...
Article
How are societal stereotypes transmitted to individual-level group preferences? We propose that exposure to a stereotype, regardless of whether one agrees with it, can shape how one experiences and learns from interactions with members of the stereotyped group, such that it induces individual-level prejudice—a process involving the interplay of semantic knowledge and instrumental learning. In a series of experiments, participants interacted with players from two groups, described with either positive or negative stereotypes, in a reinforcement learning (RL) task presented as a money-sharing game. Although players’ actual sharing rates were equated between groups, participants formed more positive reward associations with players from positively stereotyped than negatively stereotyped groups. This effect persisted even when stereotypes were described as unreliable and participants were instructed to ignore them. Computational modeling revealed that this preference was due to stereotype effects on priors regarding group members’ behavior as well as the learning rates through which reward associations were updated in response to player feedback. We then show that these stereotype-induced preferences, once formed, spread unwittingly to others who observe these interactions, illustrating a pathway through which stereotypes may be transmitted and propagated between society and individuals. By identifying a mechanism through which stereotype knowledge can bypass explicit beliefs to induce prejudice, via the interplay of semantic and instrumental learning processes, these findings illuminate the impact of stereotype messages on the formation and propagation of individual-level prejudice.
Article
Social identity theory hypothesizes that ingroup positive distinctiveness serves as a source of self-esteem that in turn propels individuals to favor ingroups over outgroups. The current meta-analysis extends past reviews of this hypothesis by testing if (a) self-esteem is differentially related to ingroup versus outgroup evaluations, and (b) the self-esteem–outgroup evaluation relation is moderated by four theoretically driven factors. A total of 103 effect sizes measured the self-esteem and outgroup derogation relation ( N = 15,764) and the self-esteem and ingroup liking relation ( N = 15,741). High self-esteem was associated with strong ingroup liking and, to a lesser extent, strong outgroup liking. Regarding the second goal, moderator analyses of the relation between self-esteem and outgroup evaluations (up to 71 effect sizes) revealed four patterns: the relation emerged when personal self-esteem was measured, real groups were targeted, low-status groups judged high-status groups, and there was no domain match between self-esteem and the outgroup.
Article
From a glimpse of a face, people form trait impressions that operate as facial stereotypes, which are largely inaccurate yet nevertheless drive social behavior. Behavioral studies have long pointed to dimensions of trustworthiness and dominance that are thought to underlie face impressions due to their evolutionarily adaptive nature. Using human neuroimaging ( N = 26, 19 female, 7 male), we identify a two-dimensional representation of faces’ inferred traits in the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), a region involved in domain-general conceptual processing including the activation of social concepts. The similarity of neural-response patterns for any given pair of faces in the bilateral MTG was predicted by their proximity in trustworthiness–dominance space, an effect that could not be explained by mere visual similarity. This MTG trait-space representation occurred automatically, was relatively invariant across participants, and did not depend on the explicit endorsement of face impressions (i.e., beliefs that face impressions are valid and accurate). In contrast, regions involved in high-level social reasoning (the bilateral temporoparietal junction and posterior superior temporal sulcus; TPJ–pSTS) and entity-specific social knowledge (the left anterior temporal lobe; ATL) also exhibited this trait-space representation but only among participants who explicitly endorsed forming these impressions. Together, the findings identify a two-dimensional neural representation of face impressions and suggest that multiple implicit and explicit mechanisms give rise to biases based on facial appearance. While the MTG implicitly represents a multidimensional trait space for faces, the TPJ–pSTS and ATL are involved in the explicit application of this trait space for social evaluation and behavior.
Article
Full-text available
The authors studied social norms and prejudice using M. Sherif and C. W. Sherif's (1953) group norm theory of attitudes. In 7 studies (N = 1, 504), social norms were measured and manipulated to examine their effects on prejudice; both normatively proscribed and normatively prescribed forms of prejudice were included. The public expression of prejudice toward 105 social groups was very highly correlated with social approval of that expression. Participants closely adhere to social norms when expressing prejudice, evaluating scenarios of discrimination, and reacting to hostile jokes. The authors reconceptualized the source of motivation to suppress prejudice in terms of identifying with new reference groups and adapting oneself to fit new norms. Suppression scales seem to measure patterns of concern about group norms rather than personal commitments to reducing prejudice; high suppressors are strong norm followers. Compared with low suppressors, high suppressors follow normative rules more closely and are more strongly influenced by shifts in local social norms. There is much value in continuing the study of normative influence and self-adaptation to social norms, particularly in terms of the group norm theory of attitudes.
Article
Full-text available
When an attitude changes from A1 to A2, what happens to A1? Most theories assume, at least implicitly, that the new attitude replaces the former one. The authors argue that a new attitude can override, but not replace, the old one, resulting in dual attitudes. Dual attitudes are defined as different evaluations of the same attitude object an automatic, implicit attitude and an explicit attitude. The attitude that people endorse depends on whether they have the cognitive capacity to retrieve the explicit attitude and whether this overrides their implicit attitude. A number of literatures consistent with these hypotheses are reviewed, and the implications of the dual-attitude model for attitude theory and measurement are discussed. For example, by including only explicit measures, previous studies may have exaggerated the ease with which people change their attitudes. Even if an explicit attitude changes, an implicit attitude can remain the same.
Article
Full-text available
This research shows stereotype activation is controlled by chronic egalitarian goals. In the first 2 studies it was found that the stereotype of women is equally available to individuals with and without chronic goals, and the discriminant validity of the concept of egalitarian goals was established. In the next 2 experiments, differences in stereotype activation as a function of this individual difference were found. In Study 3, participants read attributes following stereotypical primes. Facilitated response times to stereotypical attributes were found for nonchronics but not for chronics. This lack of facilitation occurred at stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) where effortful correction processes could not operate, demonstrating preconscious control of stereotype activation due to chronic goals. In Study 4, inhibition of the stereotype was found at an SOA where effortful processes of stereotype suppression could not operate. The data reveal that goals are activated and used preconsciously to prevent stereotype activation, demonstrating both the controllability of stereotype activation and the implicit role of goals in cognitive control.
Article
Full-text available
The content of spontaneously activated racial stereotypes among White Americans and the relation of this to more explicit measures of stereotyping and prejudice were investigated. Using a semantic priming paradigm, a prime was presented outside of conscious awareness (BLACK or WHITE), followed by a target stimulus requiring a word–nonword decision. The target stimuli included attributes that varied in valence and stereotypicality for Whites and African Americans. Results showed reliable stereotyping and prejudice effects: Black primes resulted in substantially stronger facilitation to negative than positive stereotypic attributes, whereas White primes facilitated positive more than negative stereotypic traits. The magnitude of this implicit prejudice effect correlated reliably with participants’ scores on explicit racial attitude measures, indicating that people's spontaneous stereotypic associations are consistent with their more controlled responses.
Article
Full-text available
Social psychologists can benefit from exploring connectionist or parallel distributed processing models of mental representation and process and also can contribute much to connectionist theory in return. Connectionist models involve many simple processing units that send activation signals over connections. At an abstract level, the models can be described as representing concepts (as distributed patterns of activation), operating like schemas to fill in typical values for input information, reconstructing memories based on accessible knowledge rather than retrieving static representations, using flexible and context-sensitive concepts, and computing by satisfying numerous constraints in parallel. This article reviews open questions regarding connectionist models and concludes that social psychological contributions to such topics as cognition–motivation interactions may be important for the development of integrative connectionist models.
Article
Full-text available
People encountering deviants who violate a stereotype try to maintain the stereotype by subtyping the deviants. They use the deviants' additional attributes to justify subtyping them. Participants read about counterstereotypic targets. Participants who were given no additional information about targets, and so had no grounds for subtyping them, did generalize from them and changed their stereotypes. However, participants who were told that targets had an additional, neutral attribute appeared to use it as grounds for subtyping them; their stereotypes remained unchanged. Participants came to view the neutral attributes as atypical of the stereotype and as associated with deviance, that is, as good reasons for subtyping the deviant. Neutral attributes blocked generalization from truly counterstereotypic targets but not from overly stereotypic ones, suggesting that their effect was due to participants' attempts to explain away individuals who strongly challenge their stereotypes.
Article
Full-text available
Social behavior is ordinarily treated as being under conscious (if not always thoughtful) control. However, considerable evidence now supports the view that social behavior often operates in an implicit or unconscious fashion. The identifying feature of implicit cognition is that past experience influences judgment in a fashion not introspectively known by the actor. The present conclusion—that attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes have important implicit modes of operation—extends both the construct validity and predictive usefulness of these major theoretical constructs of social psychology. Methodologically, this review calls for increased use of indirect measures—which are imperative in studies of implicit cognition. The theorized ordinariness of implicit stereotyping is consistent with recent findings of discrimination by people who explicitly disavow prejudice. The finding that implicit cognitive effects are often reduced by focusing judges’ attention on their judgment task provides a basis for evaluating applications (such as affirmative action) aimed at reducing such unintended discrimination.
Article
Full-text available
Three experiments examine how the type of language used to describe in-group and out-group behaviors contributes to the transmission and persistence of social stereotypes. Two experiments tested the hypothesis that people encode and communicate desirable in-group and undesirable out-group behaviors more abstractly than undesirable in-group and desirable out-group behaviors. Experiment 1 provided strong support for this hypothesis using a fixed-response scale format controlling for the level of abstractness developed from Semin and Fiedler's (1988a) linguistic category model. Experiment 2 yielded the same results with a free-response format. Experiment 3 demonstrated the important role that abstract versus concrete communication plays in the perpetuation of stereotypes. The implications of these findings and the use of the linguistic category model are discussed for the examination of the self-perpetuating cycle of stereotypes in communication processes.
Article
Full-text available
This experiment tested predictions derived from a social contingency model of judgment and choice that identifies 3 distinctive strategies that people rely on in dealing with demands for accountability from important interpersonal or institutional audiences. The model predicts that (a) when people know the views of the audience and are unconstrained by past commitments, they will rely on the low-effort acceptability heuristic and simply shift their views toward those of the prospective audience, (b) when people do not know the views of the audience and are unconstrained by past commitments, they will he motivated to think in relatively flexible, multidimensional ways (preemptive self-criticism), and (c) when people are accountable for positions to which they feel committed, they will devote the majority of their mental effort to justifying those positions (defensive bolstering). The experiment yielded results supportive of these 3 predictions. The study also revealed some evidence of individual differences in social and cognitive strategies for coping with accountability.
Chapter
Full-text available
We develop a model of stereotyping that considers both facilitatory and inhibitory mechanisms underlying the activation and application of stereotypic beliefs in social perception. We first briefly introduce the two main overarching themes that comprise the major emphases of the model we are proposing. Then we introduce the key elements of the model, drawing upon a number of existing theories of social perception. The bulk of the chapter is then spent developing the application of these integrating themes to an understanding of stereotyping and its avoidance. In doing so, we endeavor to show how general-purpose mechanisms can produce domain-specific insights, thereby advancing our understanding of the particular dynamics of stereotyping while simultaneously contributing to the development of theoretical coherence across psychological domains.