ArticlePDF Available

Learning to overeat: Maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls' eating in the absence of hunger

Authors:

Abstract

Experimental findings causally link restrictive child-feeding practices to overeating in children. However, longitudinal data are needed to determine the extent to which restrictive feeding practices promote overeating. Our objectives were to determine whether restrictive feeding practices foster girls' eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) and whether girls' weight status moderates the effects of restrictive feeding practices. Longitudinal data were used to create a study design featuring 2 maternal restriction factors (low and high), 2 weight-status factors (nonoverweight and overweight), and 3 time factors (ages 5, 7, and 9 y). Mean EAH increased significantly (P < 0.0001) from 5 to 9 y of age. Higher levels of restriction at 5 y of age predicted higher EAH at 7 y of age (P < 0.001) and at 9 y of age (P < 0.01). Girls who were already overweight at 5 y of age and who received higher levels of restriction had the highest EAH scores at 9 y of age (P < 0.05) and the greatest increases in EAH from 5 to 9 y of age (P < 0.01). The developmental increase in EAH from 5 to 9 y of age may be especially problematic in obesigenic environments. These longitudinal data provide evidence that maternal restriction can promote overeating. Girls who are already overweight at 5 y of age may be genetically predisposed to be especially responsive to environmental cues. These findings are not expected to be generalized to boys or to other racial and ethnic groups.
Am J Clin Nutr 2003;78:215–20. Printed in USA. © 2003 American Society for Clinical Nutrition 215
Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices
promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger
1–3
Leann L Birch, Jennifer Orlet Fisher, and Kirsten Krahnstoever Davison
ABSTRACT
Background: Experimental findings causally link restrictive
child-feeding practices to overeating in children. However, longi-
tudinal data are needed to determine the extent to which restrictive
feeding practices promote overeating.
Objectives: Our objectives were to determine whether restrictive
feeding practices foster girls’ eating in the absence of hunger
(EAH) and whether girls’ weight status moderates the effects of
restrictive feeding practices.
Design: Longitudinal data were used to create a study design fea-
turing 2 maternal restriction factors (low and high), 2 weight-
status factors (nonoverweight and overweight), and 3 time factors
(ages 5, 7, and 9 y).
Results: Mean EAH increased significantly (P < 0.0001) from 5
to 9 y of age. Higher levels of restriction at 5 y of age predicted
higher EAH at 7 y of age (P < 0.001) and at 9 y of age (P < 0.01).
Girls who were already overweight at 5 y of age and who received
higher levels of restriction had the highest EAH scores at 9 y of
age (P < 0.05) and the greatest increases in EAH from 5 to 9 y of
age (P < 0.01).
Conclusions: The developmental increase in EAH from 5 to 9 y
of age may be especially problematic in obesigenic environments.
These longitudinal data provide evidence that maternal restriction
can promote overeating. Girls who are already overweight at 5 y
of age may be genetically predisposed to be especially responsive
to environmental cues. These findings are not expected to be gen-
eralized to boys or to other racial and ethnic groups. Am J
Clin Nutr 2003;78:215–20.
KEY WORDS Eating in the absence of hunger, overeating,
restrictive feeding practices, girls
INTRODUCTION
Parents use a variety of practices to achieve day-to-day goals
involving when, what, and how much children eat. Some practices,
although successful in their immediate effects on children’s eating,
may have unintended consequences for children’s food selection,
preferences, and the behavioral control of food intake. For exam-
ple, the current obesigenic food environment is characterized by
large amounts of inexpensive, readily available, palatable, energy-
dense foods (1). In response to this environment, parents may
attempt to limit children’s consumption of “junk” or “unhealthy”
foods by keeping foods out of reach or by placing constraints on
when and how much food may be consumed. Experimental stud-
ies have shown, however, that restrictive feeding practices increase
1
From the Department of Human Development and Family Studies, The
Pennsylvania State University, University Park (LLB and KKD), and the
Department of Pediatrics, US Department of Agriculture Children’s Nutrition
Research Center, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston (JOF).
2
Supported by NIH grants RO1 HD32973 and M01 RR10732 and by the
National Dairy Council.
3
Address reprint requests to LL Birch, 105N White Building, The Pennsyl-
vania State University, University Park, PA 16802. E-mail: llb15@psu.edu.
Received August 29, 2002.
Accepted for publication December 16, 2002.
children’s preferences for restricted foods (2), heighten respon-
siveness to the presence of palatable foods, and promote overeat-
ing when restricted foods are freely available (3).
Although children can self-regulate energy intake by respond-
ing to internal signals about the energy content of the foods that
they consume (4), they are also susceptible to environmental cues
such as portion size (5) and the presence of palatable foods (6).
Of particular concern, given our current obesigenic eating envi-
ronment, is whether restrictive feeding practices may inadver-
tently teach children to ignore their own hunger and fullness
when placed in eating environments where palatable, previously
restricted foods are readily available. To date, the association
between restrictive child-feeding practices and the behavioral
controls of eating has been largely limited to cross-sectional stud-
ies of middle-class white children; the findings are particularly
clear for young girls. Highly restrictive feeding practices are
associated with eating in the absence of hunger (EAH, 7), nega-
tive self-evaluations (8), restrained eating (9), and overweight
among young girls (7, 10). These eating behaviors are similar to
those that are overrepresented among overweight adults, which
include external and emotional overeating and symptoms of binge
eating disorder (11).
Although experimental research documents that restriction pro-
motes children’s intake in a laboratory setting (3, 12), whether
parental restriction is observed as a cause or as the effect of girls’
overeating and overweight within a family context has not been
established. Current research on child development indicates that
parenting both influences children and is elicited in response to
children’s characteristics (3, 12). The present study used a proto-
col designed to provide an index of individual differences in
responsiveness to food cues in the environment by measuring chil-
dren’s EAH. The main objective of the analyses was to determine
whether restrictions that mothers place on their daugthers’ eating
at 5 y of age promotes the daughters’ overeating in the absence of
by guest on June 4, 2013ajcn.nutrition.orgDownloaded from
216 BIRCH ET AL
hunger from 5 to 9 y of age and to assess whether girls’ weight
status moderates the effects of restriction.
SUBJECTS AND METHODS
Subjects
Participants were from central Pennsylvania and were part
of a longitudinal study of the health and development of young
girls. At entry into the study, the participants consisted of 197
girls with a mean (± SD) age of 5.4 ± 0.4 y and their parents.
Of those participants, 192 girls and their parents were
reassessed 2 y later when the girls had a mean age of 7.3 ± 0.3 y.
A third assessment, which consisted of 182 families, was per-
formed 2 y later, ie, 4 y after the initial assessment, when the
girls had a mean age of 9.34 ± 0.3 y. The eligibility criteria for
girls’ participation at the time of recruitment were as follows:
living with both biological parents, the absence of severe food
allergies or chronic medical problems affecting food intake,
and the absence of dietary restrictions involving animal prod-
ucts. Families were not recruited on the basis of weight status
or concern about weight. This study was conducted in accord
with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 1983. All
procedures were approved by The Pennsylvania State Univer-
sity Institutional Review Board, and the mothers provided writ-
ten informed consent for their own and their daughters’ partic-
ipation before data collection.
Families were recruited for participation in the study with
the use of flyers and newspaper advertisements. In addition,
families with age-eligible female children within a 5-county
radius received mailings and follow-up phone calls (Metromail
Inc, Chicago). On average, the parents were in their mid-30s
at the time of recruitment (mothers’ mean age: 35 ± 5 y;
fathers’ mean age: 37 ± 5 y). Ninety-seven percent of the
fathers and 63% of the mothers were employed when the girls
were 5 y old; the employment percentages increased to 99%
and 76%, respectively, when the girls were 7 y old and
remained at 99% for the fathers and increased to 78% for the
mothers when the girls were 9 y old. When the girls were 5 y
old, approximately equal numbers of families reported incomes
in the following ranges: $20 000–35 000, $35 000–50 000, and
> $50 000. The percentage of families reporting incomes
> $50 000 increased to 47% when the girls were 7 y old and
then to 57% when the girls were 9 y old. The parents were well
educated; the mothers’ mean amount of education was 15 ± 2 y
(range: 12–20 y), and the fathers’ mean amount of education
was 15 ± 3 y (range: 12–20 y). Parents were, on average,
slightly overweight at the first measurement, with a mean body
mass index (BMI; in kg/m
2
) of 25.6 ± 5.3 for the mothers and
28.1 ± 4.5 for the fathers. Weight status increased slightly
from time 1 to time 2, with mean BMI values at time 2 of
26.9 ± 6.2 for the mothers and 28.4 ± 4.3 for the fathers. There
was also a slight increase from time 2 to time 3, with mean
BMI values increasing to 27.5 ± 6.3 and 28.9 ± 4.4 for the
mothers and the fathers, respectively.
Measures
Eating in the absence of hunger: free-access protocol
To minimize the influence of hunger on intake in the free-
access session, each girl participated in a standard ad libitum
lunch before the free-access session with 3–6 other girls of the
same age. Each girl was provided with generous portions of food
at lunch, and portion sizes were increased for some items as the
girls increased in age. The portion sizes, including changes with
age where applicable, were as follows: bread (56 g), sandwich
meat (4 slices at 5 y of age and 6 slices at 7 and 9 y of age),
carrots (20 g), applesauce [120 mL (4 oz)], cheese (1 slice at 5 y
of age and 2 slices at 7 and 9 y of age; 21 g/slice), cookies
(2 medium, 16 g each), and milk [300 mL (10 oz)]. In addition,
a subjective measure of hunger was obtained from each girl
immediately after lunch with the use of 3 figures depicting “hun-
gry,” “half-full,” and “full.” Girls who ate little before the free-
access snack session or who indicated that they were “hungry”
after lunch were not included in the analyses.
Immediately after lunch, during the free-access session, the
girls’ preferences for the snack foods (data not shown) were
assessed. Each girl was asked to rate small (2-bite) samples of the
following 10 sweet and savory snack foods varying in fat, energy
content, and sensory properties: popcorn (15 g), potato chips
(58 g), pretzels (39 g), nuts (44 g), fig bars (51 g), chocolate chip
cookies (66 g), fruit-chew candy (66 g), chocolate bars (66 g), ice
cream (168 g), and frozen yogurt (168 g). Next, the girl was shown
various toys and containers holding generous, preweighed por-
tions of the same 10 snack foods. The girl was instructed that she
could play with the toys or eat any of the foods while the experi-
menter did some work in the adjacent room. The experimenter
then left the room for 10 min. When the experimenter returned,
the girl was interviewed about whether her parents let her have
the foods provided and how she felt about her eating during the
session. To determine energy intake for each child, each of the
food items that were served was weighed before and after the ses-
sion. Manufacturers’ data were used in conjunction with gram-
consumption data to calculate each child’s total energy intake dur-
ing the 10-min EAH period.
Weight status
The girls’ height and weight were measured in triplicate at each
age by a trained research assistant, and the girls’ BMI was calcu-
lated from the height and weight measures. Because BMI during
childhood is age and sex specific, BMI percentiles were calculated
by using recent growth charts from the Centers for Disease Con-
trol and Prevention (13).
Mothers’ child-feeding practices
The mothers’ child-feeding practices and their perception of
their daughter’s risk for overweight were assessed at each age by
using the Child Feeding Questionnaire (14). The questionnaire’s
restriction subscale, which assesses the extent to which mothers
control how much, when, and what girls eat, was the primary
child-feeding measure used in these analyses. Two other aspects
of control in feeding were assessed to evaluate the extent to which
girls who received high levels of restriction differed in other
aspects of control in child feeding. These 2 aspects were moni-
toring, which assesses the extent to which mothers keep track of
what their daughters eat, and pressure to eat, or mothers’ tendency
to pressure girls to eat more food, particularly at meal times. In
addition, the following 2 aspects of maternal perceptions of the
child were assessed: perceived child’s weight, or mothers’ per-
ceptions of their daughter’s weight history, and concern about
child’s weight, which assesses mothers’ concern about their
daughter’s risk of becoming overweight. Previous analyses of this
sample provided evidence of acceptable internal consistency and
by guest on June 4, 2013ajcn.nutrition.orgDownloaded from
EATING IN THE ABSENCE OF HUNGER 217
criterion validity for the Child Feeding Questionnaire subscales
included in this analysis (14).
Background characteristics
Several background characteristics were examined in this study
as potential confounding variables for the associations between
weight status, maternal restriction, and girls’ EAH. These charac-
teristics were family income (< $20 000, $20 000–35 000,
$35 000–50 000, > $50 000), mothers’ years of education, and
mothers’ BMI, which was based on 3 measures of the mothers’
height and weight when the girls were 5 y old.
Statistical analysis
Data for 140 girls were used in the analyses outlined below.
Only girls with complete data were considered for the analyses
(n = 182). Of the 182 girls who participated from 5 to 9 y of age,
data for 29 girls at 5 y of age were excluded, data for 6 girls at 7 y
of age were excluded, and data for 7 girls at 9 y of age were
excluded. These data were excluded for the following reasons:
interviewer ratings indicated general behavioral difficulties
throughout the interview day; interviewer ratings indicated that
the girl did not seem comfortable or understand instructions dur-
ing the 10-min period in which she was told she could play or eat;
the girl ate less than a total of 1680 kJ (400 kcal) at breakfast, a
midmorning snack, and the lunch preceding the free-access ses-
sion; or the girl indicated that she was hungry before the free-
access session. Thus, excluding all subjects with either missing
data at a given age or excluded data, 140 subjects with complete
EAH data were retained. The 140 girls who were included in the
analyses did not differ significantly from the excluded girls in
terms of family income (F = 0.82, P = 0.37), mean maternal
restriction (F = 0.16, P = 0.70), or mean BMI across the 5-y period
(F = 0.92, P = 0.34).
In this study, we were primarily interested in how maternal
restriction at 5 y of age affected girls’ subsequent intake regula-
tion, or EAH, from 5 to 9 y of age. We were also interested in
whether the effects of restriction differed significantly between
girls who were overweight and those who were not overweight at
entry into the study. Therefore, we used a 2 2 factorial design
based on maternal reports of restriction and girls’ weight status at
5 y of age, which resulted in the following 4 groups: girls who
were overweight and exposed to high restriction, girls who were
overweight and exposed to low restriction, girls who were not
overweight and were exposed to high restriction, and girls who
were not overweight and were exposed to low restriction.
Median splits were used to categorize girls as receiving either
low or high levels of maternal restriction in feeding at 5 y of age.
The girls’ BMI percentile scores were used to categorize the girls
as being nonoverweight or overweight. At 5 y of age, only 28 of
the 140 girls were at or above the overweight cutoffs from the
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (ie, 85th BMI per-
centile), and only 8 of those girls fell into the overweight, low
restriction group. Therefore, a less restrictive cutoff of the 75th
BMI percentile was used to ensure that the numbers of girls in the
4 groups were more equal (ie, approximately the same number of
girls in each of the 4 groups).
To identify potential covariates for the relation between mater-
nal restriction and girls’ EAH, differences between the 4 groups in
background characteristics (family income, mothers’ years of edu-
cation, and mothers’ BMI) were examined by using analysis of
variance. In addition, the association between each background
characteristic and the dependent variable of interest (ie, EAH) was
examined by using correlation analysis. In instances in which
significant differences in the background characteristic were iden-
tified for the independent variable (ie, the restriction–weight-status
groups) and the dependent variable (ie, EAH), the background vari-
able was entered as a covariate in the analyses outlined below.
Initial analyses examined differences in maternal child-feed-
ing practices at 5 y of age, including monitoring and pressure,
and in reaction to overweight in children by using analysis of
variance featuring 2 restriction categories (low and high) and
2 weight-status categories (nonoverweight and overweight). A
2 (weight status) 2 (restriction) 3 (time) repeated-measures
analysis of variance was then used to examine the following:
1) changes in the girls’ mean EAH scores from 5 to 9 y of age;
2) differences in the girls’ EAH at each age as a function of mater-
nal restriction, girls’ weight status, and the interaction between
restriction and weight status; and 3) changes in the girls’ EAH from
5 to 9 y of age as a function of restriction, weight status, and the
interaction between restriction and weight status. All analyses were
performed with the use of SAS (version 8.2; SAS Institute Inc,
Cary, NC), and P < 0.05 was used to indicate significant effects.
RESULTS
Of the 140 girls who had complete data at 5, 7, and 9 y of age,
41 were categorized as nonoverweight with low levels of maternal
restriction at 5 y of age, 45 were nonoverweight with high restric-
tion at 5 y of age, 25 were overweight with low restriction at 5 y
of age, and 29 were overweight with high restriction at 5 y of age.
With the use of reference data from 2000 from the Centers for Dis-
ease Control and Prevention, mean (± SD) BMI percentile scores
for the nonoverweight and overweight groups were 46 ± 2 and
86 ± 1, respectively. On a scale of 1 to 5, mean maternal restric-
tion scores at 5 y of age in the low-restriction and high-restriction
groups were 2.2 ± 0.1 and 3.7 ± 0.0, respectively.
Maternal perceptions of child’s weight status and maternal
aspects of control in child feeding at 5 y of age according to
child’s weight status and level of maternal restriction in feeding at
5 y of age are shown in Tabl e 1 . For perception of child’s weight,
there were significant main effects of weight status (P < 0.0001)
and restriction (P < 0.05). The mothers with overweight daugh-
ters and the mothers reporting high levels of restriction perceived
their daughters as being more overweight than did the mothers
with nonoverweight daughters or the mothers reporting low lev-
els of restriction. For concern about child’s overweight, there
were main effects of weight status (P < 0.001) and restriction
(P < 0.05), but these effects were modified by a significant inter-
action (P < 0.01). Post hoc follow-up analyses indicated that the
mothers who had overweight daughters and reported high levels of
restriction reported significantly higher concern about child’s
overweight than did the other 3 groups. For monitoring of child’s
eating, there was a significant main effect of restriction (P < 0.01).
The mothers who reported high levels of restriction also reported
high levels of monitoring. Finally, for pressure to eat in feeding,
there was a significant main effect of weight status (P < 0.01). The
mothers of nonoverweight daughters pressured their daughters to
eat more than did the mothers of overweight daughters.
The EAH scores of the girls in the 4 groups at 5, 7, and 9 y of
age are shown in Figure 1. A time-related increase in the girls’
EAH over the 5-y period was observed (P < 0.0001). The mean
energy intakes from snack foods consumed during the free-access
by guest on June 4, 2013ajcn.nutrition.orgDownloaded from
218 BIRCH ET AL
TABLE 1
Maternal perceptions of child’s weight status and maternal aspects of control in child feeding at 5 y of age according to child’s weight status and level of
maternal restriction in feeding at 5 y of age
1
Nonoverweight Overweight
Low restriction (n = 41) High restriction (n = 45) Low restriction (n = 25) High restriction (n = 29)
Perception of child’s weight
2,3
2.6 ± 0.1 2.8 ± 0.1 3.0 ± 0.1 3.1 ± 0.1
Concern about child’s overweight
2–4
2.0 ± 0.2 1.9 ± 0.2 2.2 ± 0.2 3.1 ± 0.1
Monitoring of child’s eating
3
3.4 ± 0.1 3.9 ± 0.1 3.5 ± 0.2 4.1 ± 0.2
Pressure to eat in feeding
2
2.2 ± 0.1 2.6 ± 0.1 1.9 ± 0.1 1.9 ± 0.1
1
Values are the mean (± SD) item response on the Child Feeding Questionnaire: higher scores indicate higher values of the construct. The high and low
restriction groups were classified on the basis of a median split. Nonoverweight and overweight were classified on the basis of the 75th BMI percentile.
2
Significant main effect of weight status, P < 0.05 (2 2 ANOVA).
3
Significant main effect of restriction, P < 0.05 (2 2 ANOVA).
4
Significant interaction between weight status and restriction, P < 0.05 (2 2 ANOVA).
FIGURE 1. Mean (± SEM) energy consumed during periods of eat-
ing in the absence of hunger (EAH) by girls at 5, 7, and 9 y of age
according to membership in 1 of the 4 following groups categorized by
child’s weight status and maternal restriction in child feeding: normal
weight and low restriction (; n = 41), normal weight and high restric-
tion (; n = 45), overweight and low restriction (; n = 25), and over-
weight and high restriction (; n = 29). Changes over time in energy
consumed (ie, EAH score) and differences in EAH scores between the
groups were assessed by using 2 2 3 repeated-measures ANOVA.
There were no significant effects at 5 y of age. At 7 y of age, there was
a significant main effect of restriction (P < 0.0002). At 9 y of age, there
was a significant main effect of restriction (P < 0.0018), which was mod-
ified by a significant interaction between weight status and restriction
(P < 0.053). There was also a significant time effect (P < 0.00001) and
a significant time weight status restriction interaction (P < 0.007).
periods at 5, 7, and 9 y of age were 523 ± 32 kJ (125 ± 8 kcal),
728 ± 39 kJ (174 ± 9 kcal), and 944 ± 48 kJ (225 ± 11 kcal),
respectively, which constituted 6.9%, 8.7%, and 11.2%, respec-
tively, of the age- and sex-specific recommended daily energy
intakes. At 5 y of age, there were no significant effects of weight
status or restriction on the girls’ EAH. At 7 y of age, there was a
significant main effect of restriction (P < 0.01). The girls who
were exposed to high levels of maternal restriction had higher
EAH scores than did the girls who were exposed to low levels of
restriction. At 9 y of age, there was also a main effect of restric-
tion (P < 0.01), but it was modified by an interaction between
weight status and restriction (P < 0.05). Overweight girls exposed
to higher levels of restriction had the highest EAH scores. For the
girls’ EAH scores from 5 to 9 y of age, there was a significant
time restriction interaction (P < 0.05). This effect, however,
was modified by a time weight status restriction interaction
(P < 0.01). Post hoc follow-up analyses indicated that girls who
were overweight and who were exposed to high levels of mater-
nal restriction showed the greatest increases in EAH across time.
DISCUSSION
Research has shown links between parents’ feeding practices
and children’s eating and weight status, but causality has remained
at issue; experimental research indicates that restriction can cause
overeating and is associated with overweight among children (3,
12), but other findings indicate that overweight girls tend to elicit
more restrictive feeding practices by mothers. The findings of this
study provide new longitudinal evidence that restrictive feeding
practices can promote overeating in response to the presence of
palatable foods among young girls during middle childhood, per-
haps increasing their risk for subsequent problems with eating and
energy balance. Regardless of the level of maternal restriction, the
girls’ EAH increased from 5 to 9 y of age in both absolute and rel-
ative terms and constituted an increasing percentage of the age-
and sex-specific recommended energy intake. This developmental
trend, which is consistent with a heightened responsiveness to the
presence of food, is consistent with other findings indicating that
as children develop, eating becomes increasingly responsive to
environmental cues, including portion size (5). This trend may be
especially problematic in our current environment, which is char-
acterized by the ready accessibility and availability of inexpen-
sive, palatable foods. The girls whose mothers reported using
higher levels of restriction when their daughters were 5 y old ate
more in the absence of hunger at 7 and 9 y of age than did those
whose mothers used lower levels of restriction. Furthermore, the
girls’ overweight was not a necessary precondition for the impo-
sition of maternal restriction, and restriction increased the girls’
EAH over time for both overweight and nonoverweight girls.
Five-year-old girls who were already overweight and who were
subject to higher levels of restriction showed the greatest overeat-
ing at 9 y of age. This group also showed the largest increases in
overeating from 5 to 9 y of age. These findings, which show that
the effects of early restrictive feeding practices were greatest
among the girls who were already overweight by 5 y of age, may
reflect a gene-by-environment interaction, with the child’s
genetic predisposition for overweight status moderating the effect
of maternal restriction. It is possible that girls who are already
overweight by 5 y of age may also be genetically predisposed to
be highly responsive to environmental factors, including both
by guest on June 4, 2013ajcn.nutrition.orgDownloaded from
EATING IN THE ABSENCE OF HUNGER 219
maternal restrictive feeding practices and the presence of palat-
able food. This pattern is consistent with recent epidemiologic
data charting population increases in weight status, which show
that the most overweight persons may be the most susceptible to
obesigenic environments (1, 15). During a time period in which
the eating environment has been characterized as increasingly
obesigenic, the greatest increases in weight status are at the upper
end of the weight-status distribution.
The results of the present study corroborate previous experi-
mental findings (3, 7, 8, 10) showing that feeding practices can
foster individual differences in children’s eating within the family
context, which indicates that parenting influences child outcomes.
In particular, these findings provide the first longitudinal evidence
indicating that maternal use of restrictive feeding practices pro-
motes daughters’ EAH. With respect to possible effects of the
child on parenting, the mothers of overweight girls in the present
study were more concerned about their daughters’ weight and eat-
ing and were less likely to pressure their daughters to eat. Paths of
influence in parent-child interactions flow in both directions, and
these bidirectional paths of influence create feedback cycles in
which both maternal feeding styles and the child’s overeating and
weight status persist across time, leading to higher levels of
restriction and greater degrees of overweight (10).
Among adults, dietary disinhibition and binge eating are char-
acterized by consuming relatively large amounts of food in the
absence of hunger, and both can be elicited by self-restrictions on
eating. Although we know little about the causes of these eating
problems, the present research indicates that EAH, a critical fea-
ture of both disinhibited eating and binge eating, is apparent
among girls during middle childhood (9, 16) and is fostered by
maternal restriction. In a retrospective study of overweight female
binge eaters (17), some women reported that dieting preceded the
onset of binge eating, but others indicated that binging preceded
the initiation of dieting. The present findings, which show that
girls’ overeating in the absence of hunger is promoted by higher
levels of maternal restriction, raise the possibility that maternal
restriction of girls’ intake might serve as one trigger for initial
overeating or binge eating episodes, which in turn, may initiate
self-imposed restrictive dieting attempts.
A limitation of the present study is that our sample was exclu-
sively non-Hispanic white and included girls only. We selected
this sample because, by adolescence, a high prevalence of over-
weight; high rates of maladaptive eating behaviors, including
binge eating; weight concerns; body dissatisfaction; and high rates
of chronic use of unhealthy weight-control practices are endemic
among non-Hispanic white girls. However, because our findings
were obtained in a sample of exclusively non-Hispanic white fam-
ilies with girls, we cannot generalize these findings to other racial,
ethnic, or income groups or to boys. In this instance, because par-
enting is so culturally specific, there is good reason to predict that
our findings would not generalize to other groups. Parenting prac-
tices reflect, in part, parents’ responses to perceived environmen-
tal threats to goals for their children (18), and although a few
parental goals for children are universal (eg, child health), other
goals differ by ethnicity, race, income, education, and child sex.
Furthermore, parents may differ in their beliefs about the envi-
ronmental conditions that either threaten or promote the attain-
ment of particular parenting goals.
For parents of non-Hispanic white girls, physical attractiveness
is an important goal for daughters (19, 20). Especially among this
group, overweight is stigmatized, and thinness, as a dimension of
physical attractiveness, is especially highly valued (21). Restric-
tive feeding strategies are used to protect daughters from the obe-
sigenic environment’s threat to their thinness and attractiveness. In
contrast, among Hispanics and African Americans, both of whom
have a particularly high prevalence of overweight and obesity dur-
ing childhood, thinness is not a goal, and childhood overweight is
not typically viewed as a problem or a threat to parental goals but
as a sign of success in meeting parental goals. The accelerated
growth and higher weight status of overweight children is viewed
positively, as evidence that the child is eating well, growing well,
and attaining the goal of good health (22, 23). We would not
expect restrictive feeding practices to be used by parents who are
interpreting children’s overweight status positively. These con-
trasting parental evaluations of childhood overweight among
racial, ethnic, and income groups highlight the need for qualitative
and quantitative research in these groups to address links between
cultural beliefs and parental goals and beliefs regarding children’s
eating and weight status and to determine how parenting practices
influence or are influenced by children’s weight status.
An increasingly obesigenic environment has been implicated in
the dramatic increases in the prevalence of overweight and obe-
sity among both children and adults of all racial and ethnic groups
(1, 24). For young children, parents play a central role in deter-
mining the extent to which the child’s eating environment is obe-
sigenic and may play a central role in determining the child’s sus-
ceptibility to environmental factors. In the present study, increases
in EAH were noted for all the groups across middle childhood, and
these increases in overeating were greater among the girls who
received high levels of restriction at 5 y of age than among those
who received low levels of restriction. The girls who were already
overweight by5yofageappeared to be especially susceptible to
the effects of maternal restriction, perhaps reflecting a genetic pre-
disposition for a heightened responsiveness to various environ-
mental factors as controls of food intake.
Although the present research shows that mothers’ attempts to
restrict daughters’ eating can have negative effects on the girls’
development of food-intake controls, this is only one avenue of
parental influence over what, when, and how much children eat.
Parents also select the foods and the size of food portions that are
offered to children; structure the timing, frequency, and social
contexts of eating (ie, determining whether family members eat
in front of the TV or together at the table); set an example through
their own eating behavior; and directly guide children’s eating via
feeding practices. Effective prevention programs must address
these multiple avenues of parental influence while being sensitive
to differences among racial, ethnic, and socioeconomic groups.
The findings of the present study provide additional support for
the view that prevention of childhood overweight must begin in
early childhood and include anticipatory guidance on parent-child
interactions in feeding. Our findings, which indicate that restric-
tion is counterproductive and not an effective approach to limiting
girls’ food intake, emphasize the importance of providing guid-
ance to parents on alternative methods of setting limits for chil-
dren in the feeding context that allow the development of adequate
self-control mechanisms. Primary prevention programs should
promote parenting skills that help children learn to like healthy
food choices, to consume appropriate portion sizes, and to be
responsive to hunger and satiety cues as determinants of when and
how much they eat. Because our findings indicate that restrictive
feeding practices tend to make girls more vulnerable to obesigenic
environments, programs to promote healthy eating and weight status
by guest on June 4, 2013ajcn.nutrition.orgDownloaded from
220 BIRCH ET AL
among this group should provide parents with alternatives to
restrictive approaches to feeding.
The services provided by the General Clinical Research Center of the The
Pennsylvania State University are appreciated.
LLB contributed to the study design and the writing of the manuscript, and
JOF and KKD contributed to data collection and analysis and the writing of the
manuscript. None of the authors had any financial or personal interest in the
organizations sponsoring this research.
REFERENCES
1. Hill JO, Peters JC. Environmental contributions to the obesity epi-
demic. Science 1998;280:1371–4.
2. Birch LL, Zimmerman S, Hind H. The influence of social-affective
context on preschool children’s food preferences. Child Dev 1980;
51:856–61.
3. Fisher JO, Birch LL. Restricting access to palatable foods affects chil-
dren’s behavioral response, food selection, and intake. Am J Clin Nutr
1999;69:1264–72.
4. Birch L, Johnson S, Andresen G, Peters J, Schulte M. The variability
of young children’s energy intake. N Engl J Med 1991;324:232–5.
5. Rolls BJ, Engell D, Birch LL. Serving portion size influences 5-year
old but not 3-year-old children’s food intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 2000;
100:232–4.
6. Birch LL, McPhee L, Shoba BC, Steinberg L, et al. “Clean up your
plate”: effects of child feeding practices on the conditioning of meal
size. Learn Motiv 1987;18:301–17.
7. Fisher J, Birch L. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in
girls at 5 and 7 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr 2002;76:226–31.
8. Fisher JO, Birch LL. Parents’ restrictive feeding practices are asso-
ciated with young girls’ negative self-evaluation about eating. J Am
Diet Assoc 2000;100:1341–6.
9. Carper J, Fisher J, Birch L. Young girls’ emerging dietary restraint
and disinhibition are related to parental control in child feeding.
Appetite 2000;35:121–9.
10. Birch LL, Fisher JO. Mothers’ child-feeding practices influence
daughters’ eating and weight. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71:1054–61.
11. Yanovski SZ. Binge eating disorder: current knowledge and future
directions. Obes Res 1993;1:306–24.
12. Fisher JO, Birch LL. Restricting access to foods and children’s eat-
ing. Appetite 1999;32:405–19.
13. Kuczmarski R, Ogden C, Grummer-Strawn L, et al. CDC growth
charts: United States. Advance data from Vital and Health Statistics.
Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics, 2000.
14. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R,
Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding
Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices
about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite 2001;36:201–10.
15. Troiano RP, Flegal KM, Kuczmarski RJ, Campbell SM, Johnson CL.
Overweight prevalence and trends for children and adolescents: the
National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys, 1963 to 1991.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1995;149:1085–91.
16. Cutting TM, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Birch LL. Like mother,
like daughter: familial patterns of overweight are mediated by
mothers’ dietary disinhibition. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;69:608–13.
17. Marcus MD, Moulton MM, Greeno CG. Binge eating onset in obese
patients with binge eating disorder. Addict Behav 1995;20:747–55.
18. LeVine RA. Parental goals: a cross-cultural view. In: Leichter H, ed.
The family as educators. New York: Teachers College Press, Colum-
bia University, 1974:52–65.
19. Striegel-Moore RH, Schreiber GB, Pike KM, Wilfley DE, Rodin J.
Drive for thinness in black and white preadolescent girls. Int J Eat
Disord 1995;18:59–69.
20. Striegel-Moore RH, Wilfley DE, Caldwell MB, Needham ML,
Brownell KD. Weight-related attitudes and behaviors of women who
diet to lose weight: a comparison of black dieters and white dieters.
Obes Res 1996;4:109–16.
21. Collins ME. Body figure perceptions and preferences among pread-
olescent children. Int J Eat Disord 1991;10:199–208.
22. Kaiser LL, Martinez NA, Harwood JO, Garcia LC. Child feeding
strategies in low-income Latin households: focus group observations.
J Am Diet Assoc 1999;99:601–3.
23. Jain A, Sherman S, Chamberlain L, Carter Y, Powers S, Whitaker R.
Why don’t low-income mothers worry about their preschoolers being
overweight. Pediatrics 2001;107:1138–46.
24. Brownell KD. Public policy and the prevention of obesity. In:
Fairburn CG, Brownell KD, eds. Eating disorders and obesity: a com-
prehensive handbook. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford, 2001:619–24.
by guest on June 4, 2013ajcn.nutrition.orgDownloaded from
... When children experience restricted access to food, they display increased intake when restrictions are lifted (Tapper, 2017). In addition, research suggests (Birch et al., 2003) that high levels of dietary restriction aimed at controlling child weight have the effect of reducing self-regulation in eating, which may indirectly contribute to increased food intake and consequently excessive body weight. A number of studies (Birch et al., 2003;Gregory et al., 2010) also indicate that children exposed to high levels of restriction tended to exhibit adverse eating behaviors such as emotional eating, reduced fruit and vegetable intake and higher frequency of salty snack consumption. ...
... In addition, research suggests (Birch et al., 2003) that high levels of dietary restriction aimed at controlling child weight have the effect of reducing self-regulation in eating, which may indirectly contribute to increased food intake and consequently excessive body weight. A number of studies (Birch et al., 2003;Gregory et al., 2010) also indicate that children exposed to high levels of restriction tended to exhibit adverse eating behaviors such as emotional eating, reduced fruit and vegetable intake and higher frequency of salty snack consumption. Thus, restrictive practices cannot be recommended to parents as effective in counteracting high consumption of certain foods in adulthood. ...
Conference Paper
Consumption of highly processed foods may be determined by social factors, including the family environment. There is still a lack of knowledge on the consumption of highly processed foods in adults in relation to their childhood food experiences. To fill this gap, the aim of the study presented here was to evaluate the relationship between childhood food experiences resulting from parental feeding practices and the use of restrictions on consumption of highly processed foods in adulthood. The cross-sectional study was conducted in 2020/2021 using the CAWI (Computer Assisted Web Interview) method among 702 Polish adults aged 35 and under. The questionnaire included questions from AMoFiC (Adults’ Memories of Feeding in Childhood), questions on current use of food restrictions, including highly processed foods, and socio-demographic characteristics. Frequency analysis and cross-tabulations were performed, means and standard deviations were calculated. Pearson's Chi2test and Mann-Whitney’s test were used, with an accepted level of significance of p < 0.05. About 80% of the study sample declared using restrictions on the consumption of foods, including highly processed foods. The use of restrictions by parents to limit a child's intake of high-fat foods and sweets has shown an association with the use of restrictions in adulthood, including restrictions on the intake of highly processed foods. Low-intensity restriction combined with provision good role models and nutritional knowledge can encourage children to adopt healthy eating behaviors. At the same time, such parental feeding practices may increase the likelihood of reducing consumption of highly processed foods in adulthood.
... Both types of nonresponsive feeding practice have been associated with negative health and wellbeing outcomes. For example, research exploring coercive feeding practices suggests that increased maternal control (restriction) of AFAB children's intake may lead to increased eating in the absence of hunger (Birch, Fisher, & Davison, 2003) and for both AMAB and AFAB adolescents, is associated with disordered eating behaviours (Loth, MacLehose, Fulkerson, Crow, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2014). ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
While eating disorders have been estimated to affect at least 4% of the Australian population, research demonstrates that feeding difficulties and eating disorders are overrepresented in neurodivergent people, including in autism, ADHD, intellectual disability, giftedness, and Tourette’s disorder. However, despite there being a substantial body of literature spanning decades evidencing links between neurodivergence and eating disorders, awareness among clinicians and researchers of this existing knowledge base is only emerging in Australia. NEDC commissioned Eating Disorders Neurodiversity Australia (EDNA) to write a report, Eating Disorders and Neurodivergence: A Stepped Care Approach, that synthesizes research and lived experience evidence regarding the prevention, early identification and treatment of eating disorders and disordered eating for neurodivergent people. This report aims to encourage collaboration among stakeholders to co-produce and co-design appropriate, effective, culturally valid, and safe neurodiversity-affirming support systems and care pathways. It is designed for the use of a wide range of stakeholders, especially health care professionals (e.g., psychiatrists, psychologists, dietitians, general practitioners, paediatricians, occupational therapists), researchers, academics, educators (e.g., teachers), service managers, and lived experience experts. This report draws on fundamental constructs relating to human rights, bioethics, humanistic psychology, phenomenology, and social justice. It challenges traditional understandings of neurodivergence as pathological. It seeks to destigamtise neurodivergent body awareness and image, feeding, and eating experiences and behaviours. It is a call to action for all eating disorder stakeholders to engage in a radical rethink of how neuronormative feeding and eating practices, which influence research and clinical practice across all levels of eating disorder care, may prove harmful for neurodivergent people.
... The aim of this study is to provide clinicians with a guideline regarding food selectivity concerning possible explanations of the phenomenon, along with a direct/indirect assessment gathering detailed and useful information about target feeding behaviors. Finally, a description of evidence-based sensorial and behavioral strategies useful also for parent-mediated intervention is reported addressing food selectivity in children with ASD. by social and contextual aspects such as parental practices and/or observation of peer behavior [6]. Hence, weaning is described as the transactional period that starts from 4 months until the end of the second year of life and marks the transition to the assumption of more solid consistencies [7]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Autisms Spectrum Disorders (ASD) are characterized by core symptoms (social communication and restricted and repetitive behaviors) and related comorbidities, including sensory anomalies, feeding issues, and challenging behaviors. Children with ASD experience significantly more feeding problems than their peers. In fact, parents and clinicians have to manage daily the burden of various dysfunctional behaviors of children at mealtimes (food refusal, limited variety of food, single food intake, or liquid diet). These dysfunctional behaviors at mealtime depend on different factors that are either medical/sensorial or behavioral. Consequently, a correct assessment is necessary in order to program an effective clinical intervention. The aim of this study is to provide clinicians with a guideline regarding food selectivity concerning possible explanations of the phenomenon, along with a direct/indirect assessment gathering detailed and useful information about target feeding behaviors. Finally, a description of evidence-based sensorial and behavioral strategies useful also for parent-mediated intervention is reported addressing food selectivity in children with ASD.
... High food motivation is thought to be a dimension of appetite regulation in children [11,12] that shapes hunger and satiety responses and the quantity/composition of consumption. A wide range of food motivated behaviors have been associated with higher weight status among young children [13,14], including parent report of children's enjoyment of food and food responsiveness [15][16][17][18][19][20] as well as direct observations of eating in the absence of hunger [21][22][23][24]. Importantly, food motivated behaviors have a strong genetic component [25,26] and show stability over time [23,27]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although parental feeding plays an important role in child eating and weight status, high food motivation among children may also be a factor shaping how feeding impacts child weight. This study explored whether individual differences in preschool children’s food motivation interacted with mothers’ feeding styles in predicting subsequent child weight status. Participants included 129 Hispanic Head Start mother/child dyads. Data were collected at ages 4–5 years (Time 1) and 7–9 (Time 3). Staff measured heights/weights and observed children in an eating in the absence of hunger task. Mothers reported on feeding styles/practices and children’s eating behaviors. A principal components analysis derived a measure of highly motivated eating in children. Multiple regressions predicted Time 3 child BMI z-scores. Time 3 BMI z-scores were positively predicted by authoritative and indulgent feeding styles and negatively predicted by monitoring. Since feeding style interacted with highly motivated eating, separate regressions were run for high and low food motivation in children. Unexpectedly, results showed that authoritative feeding positively predicted Time 3 child BMI z-scores only for children showing low levels of food motivation. Characterizing differential parental feeding and child eating phenotypes may assist in tailoring childhood obesity prevention programs for the target populations.
... years. 17,57 Studies have shown that indulgent and uninvolved feeding styles were associated with increased risk of overweight and higher BMI z-scores, [15][16][17][58][59][60][61] whereas responsive feeding was protective against overweight risk. 62,63 The association between maternal feeding styles with the risk of overweight may differ in infancy from those in later childhood, given the rapid changes in growth and development, and dependency on caregivers to mediate the food environment. ...
Article
Full-text available
Parental feeding styles in the first years of life may increase childhood overweight risk. There are few studies on this association in children under 2 years. The current study aimed to investigate the association between maternal feeding styles and Caribbean infants’ risk of overweight at 18 months and determine whether maternal depression is an important confounder in the association. It is a prospective study of mother–child dyads that participated in a three‐island parenting trial. Maternal and infant characteristics were collected at infant age 6–10 weeks with maternal depression assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES‐D) scale questionnaire. Feeding styles were measured using items from the Toddler Feeding Behaviour Questionnaire at 12 months. Infant weight and length were measured at 18 months, and infants at risk of overweight were defined as weight‐for‐length z‐scores (WLZ) > 1. Factor analysis was used to identify feeding styles and associations with overweight risk assessed using multilevel logistic regression accounting for country and health clinic. Logistic regression models were adjusted for infant and maternal covariates, with a final model including depressive symptom scores. Of 366 infants assessed, 18.9% were affected by the risk of overweight. Five feeding styles were identified—uninvolved, indulgent, forceful, restrictive and responsive. Only restrictive feeding was associated with an increased risk of overweight (OR [95% CI], 1.61 [1.22, 2.13]). The association strengthened after adjusting for maternal depressive symptoms (OR [95% CI], 1.79 [1.30, 2.30]), representing an 11.18% increase in the risk of overweight odds. Interventions that address restrictive feeding may be one strategy to reduce overweight risk among Caribbean maternal–infant dyads.
... The diet quality of parents and children has been shown to be directly associated, suggesting that the eating behaviors of children are likely to stem from parental dietary practices [3]. Restrictive feeding practices in children can increase the desire for restricted food as well as eating in the absence of hunger and, ultimately, a higher calorie intake [4][5][6][7]. Increasing the frequency of healthy meals/snacks can instead be beneficial as it helps to regulate the appetite and ward off hunger frustration [3]. In children under 5 years of age, longitudinal studies have shown no significant association or very little clinical significance between the meal frequency and the weight status [8,9]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Childhood obesity is a major international problem, and unhealthy eating habits remain widespread. Increasing the frequency of meals of nutritious food can help children to regulate their appetite and maintain a healthy weight. However, there is scarce prospective evidence on the relationship between the meal frequency and weight outcomes. Therefore, the objective of the present study was to determine the prospective association between the meal frequency, body mass index, and waist circumference in Spanish children. Additionally, we analyzed the impact of the meal frequency on the incidence of excessive weight and abdominal obesity. The study included 1400 children with a mean (SD) age of 10.1 (0.6) and an average follow-up of 15 months. Anthropometric measurements, including the body weight, height, and waist circumference, were measured by trained personnel, and children were asked about whether they usually had the following meals: breakfast, a mid-morning snack, lunch, an afternoon snack, and dinner. Multiple linear regression models revealed a significant (p < 0.05) inverse association between the meal frequency with a standardized BMI (zBMI) and the waist-to-height ratio (WHtR) after adjusting for sex, age, allocation to an intervention group, school, maternal education, physical activity, diet quality, and for the corresponding outcome variable at the baseline. Furthermore, the odds of developing abdominal obesity or excessive weight during the follow-up significantly decreased with an increase in the meal frequency after controlling for the same confounders. In conclusion, a higher meal frequency at the baseline was predictive for a lower zBMI, WHtR, and odds of the incidence of excessive weight and abdominal obesity.
Article
Based on the Self-Determination Theory, this study examined: 1) how mothers' autonomous and controlled motivation to regulate their own eating behaviors relate to their food parenting practices, and 2) whether and how child food responsiveness (i.e., reactivity and attraction to food) interact with mothers' motivation to predict maternal food parenting practices. Participants were 296 French Canadian mothers of at least one child aged between 2 and 8 years old. Results of partial correlation analyses (controlling for demographics and controlled motivation) showed that maternal autonomous motivation to regulate their own eating behaviors was positively related to autonomy-promoting (i.e., child involvement) and structure-based (i.e., modeling, creating a healthy environment, monitoring) food parenting practices. In contrast, controlling for demographics and autonomous motivation, maternal controlled motivation was positively associated with food-related practices based on coercive control (i.e., the use of food to regulate the child's emotions, the use of food as a reward, pressure to eat, restriction for weight reasons, and restriction for health reasons). Furthermore, the child's food responsiveness was found to interact with mothers' motivation to regulate their own eating behaviors in the prediction of maternal food parenting practices such that mothers with high autonomous motivation or low controlled motivation were found to react with more structure-based (i.e., creating a healthy environment) and autonomy-based (i.e., child involvement) practices, as well as less controlling practices (i.e., the use of food to regulate the child's emotions), to a child who is highly responsive to food. In conclusion, findings suggest that guiding mothers toward developing a more autonomous and less controlled motivation to regulate their own eating behaviors might help them adopt more autonomy-promoting and structure-based and less controlling feeding practices, especially with children who are highly responsive to food.
Article
Background: Parental child-feeding practices have been associated with child body mass index (BMI) and specific food group consumption, however, their role in the development of dietary patterns is less understood. We aim to study the association between parental child-feeding practices at 4 years old (y) and dietary patterns at 7y that explain BMI z-scores at age 10. Methodology: Participants were children from the Generation XXI birth cohort (n=3272). Three patterns of feeding practices at 4y were previously identified: "Perceived monitoring"; "Restriction" and "Pressure to eat". At 7y, two dietary patterns were derived: "Energy-dense foods", higher in energy-dense foods and drinks, and processed meats, and a lower consumption of vegetable soup (significantly associated with BMI z-scores at 10y) and "Fish-based", higher in fishery intake and lower in energy-dense foods intake. Associations were estimated by linear regression models, adjusted for potential confounders (mother's age, education, and pre-pregnancy BMI). Results: Girls whose parents used more Restriction, Perceived monitoring, and Pressure to eat at 4y were less likely to follow the "Energy-dense foods" dietary pattern at 7y (β̂=-0.082; 95%CI: -0.134; -0.029; β̂=-0.093; 95%CI: -0.146; -0.039; β̂=-0.079; 95%CI: -0.135; -0.04, respectively). In both sexes, children whose parents used more Restriction and Perceived monitoring at 4y were more likely to follow the "Fish-based" dietary pattern at 7y (Girls: β̂=0.143; 95%CI:0.077;0.210; β̂=0.079; 95%CI:0.011;0.148; Boys: β̂=0.157; 95%CI:0.090;0.224; β̂=0.104; 95%CI:0.041;0.168). Conclusions: Children whose parents used more Restriction and Perceived monitoring at pre-school age were more likely to follow healthier dietary patterns at age 7. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Chapter
A nutrição é uma característica inerente à vida. A aquisição de matéria-prima proveniente do meio ambiente é considerado fator imprescindível para a existência dos mais variados seres, dos unicelulares, mais simples, aos pluricelulares, mais complexos. Assim o fenômeno da vida e o da nutrição são, reciprocamente, codependentes. Os elementos essenciais e a energia necessários para manter as atividades mais primordiais do organismo vivo estão presentes nos alimentos. Estes devem atender às demandas dos tecidos vivos tais como de crescimento, manutenção, restauração e trabalho. Nos alimentos encontram-se os nutrientes, seus constituintes químicos mais simples, tais como carboidratos, lipídios e proteínas (macronutrientes – fornecedores de energia metabólica); sais minerais e vitaminas (micronutrientes – elementos reguladores); e também a água. Todos estes, em conjunto, apresentam relevante papel nos mais diversos eventos biológicos como regulação das reações orgânicas, síntese de moléculas, crescimento, desenvolvimento e reparo de tecidos, entre outros. O sistema nervoso exerce a função integradora e de comando sobre os outros sistemas fisiológicos, favorecendo o dinamismo homeostático que o nosso organismo necessita para estar vivo. Assim o sistema nervoso é originado, desenvolve-se, passa pelo processo de maturação, pode modificar-se com a capacidade de reorganizar-se ao longo da vida, envelhece, degenera e morre. Essa capacidade de modificar-se, tanto morfologicamente como funcionalmente, é conhecida como plasticidade e proporciona ao organismo a adaptação ao meio em que vive. Alguns fatores externos, como a nutrição, influenciam diretamente os processos de desenvolvimento e são vitais para guiar toda a organização, estrutura, função e metabolismo do sistema nervoso. No decorrer do capítulo iremos correlacionar os processos de formação, desenvolvimento e crescimento, que envolvem os aspectos morfofuncionais das células neurais com nutrição adequada, utilização de nutrientes, durante os períodos pré e pós-natal, infância, adolescência e envelhecimento do sistema nervoso. Ressaltaremos as alterações da nutrição inadequada na fase precoce da vida, a qual pode interferir na plasticidade neural, modificando características fenotípicas do sistema nervoso maduro.
Chapter
No decorrer da evolução da humanidade, observamos mudanças no comportamento de ingestão dos alimentos. Antes, havia um maior consumo de alimentos naturais, ricos em nutrientes. Contudo, atualmente, após a evolução industrial, houve uma mudança no padrão de vida das pessoas e, associada a isso, verifica-se também uma alteração nos hábitos alimentares. Hoje em dia, as pessoas ingerem muito mais alimentos processados, pobres em nutrientes, rápidos e de fácil ingestão. Nas fases iniciais da vida, durante o período crítico do desenvolvimento, muitas crianças consomem predominantemente alimentos tipo fast food. Dessa forma, podem comprometer o seu desenvolvimento craniofacial e, por conseguinte, funções básicas para o comportamento alimentar, como a mastigação e a deglutição. Tais atos motores são funções vitais para o ser humano, e agem de forma sincrônica, haja vista que a mastigação prepara o alimento para a deglutição. Tanto a mastigação como a deglutição são de extrema importância para a nutrição, já que controlam, direta ou indiretamente, os mecanismos de apetite e saciedade. Ademais, as características dos alimentos, a exemplo da textura, do sabor e do odor, podem interferir na dinâmica das supracitadas funções motoras, podendo interferir no tempo de trânsito dos alimentos, controlando, assim, o tamanho e a duração das refeições. Portanto, no decorrer deste capítulo, abordaremos os conceitos e o processamento básico do alimento durante a mastigação e a deglutição, além de como ocorre o seu desenvolvimento e como se processa durante o envelhecimento. Ressaltaremos também a importância das mencionadas funções motoras para a nutrição, abordando a influência das características dos alimentos sobre tais funções motoras. E, por último, destacaremos o efeito da manipulação nutricional, sobretudo da desnutrição, sobre a mastigação e a deglutição.
Article
Background: Obese parents are more likely to have obese children. Parents provide both the genes and eating environment for their children and familial patterns of adiposity are the result of gene-environment interactions. Environmental factors are implicated in the rapid increases in prevalence of childhood overweight that have occurred in the past 2 decades. Examination of aspects of the family environment may provide insight into increases in childhood overweight over time. Objective: We examined parental characteristics associated with overweight and eating behaviors in preschool children. Design: Seventy-five preschool children and their parents were recruited from local daycare centers. Information was obtained on parents' body mass indexes (BMIs), dietary restraint, and dietary disinhibition. A behavioral index of disinhibited eating in children was used to measure children's eating when given free access to palatable snack foods in the absence of hunger. Children's weight-for-height values were also calculated. Results: Maternal dietary disinhibition (R² = 0.35, P < 0.01) and maternal BMI (R² = 0.19, P < 0.05) positively predicted daughters' overweight. Maternal disinhibition (R² = 0.35, P < 0.05) mediated the relation between mothers' BMI and daughters' overweight when both maternal disinhibition and maternal BMI were used to predict daughters' overweight. Furthermore, when both mothers' disinhibition and daughters' free access intakes were used to predict daughters' overweight, mothers' disinhibition (P < 0.05) showed independent prediction. Conclusions: These findings suggest that familial influences on child overweight differ according to parent and child sex. Also, these results suggest that mothers' dietary disinhibition mediates familial similarities in degree of overweight for mothers and daughters.
Article
Binge eating disorder (BED) is a newly characterized eating disorder that encompasses individuals who have severe distress and dysfunction due to binge eating, but who do not regularly engage in inappropriate compensatory behaviors. While relatively uncommon in the general community, BED becomes more prevalent with increasing severity of obesity. BED is associated with early onset of obesity, frequent weight cycling, body shape disparagement, and psychiatric disorders. These associations occur independent of the degree of obesity. Although many individuals with BED have good short-term weight loss regardless of treatment modality, as a group they may be prone to greater attrition during weight-loss treatment and more rapid regain of lost weight. Current treatments geared toward binge eating behaviors include antidepressant medications, cognitive behavioral psychotherapy, and interpersonal psychotherapy; however, these treatments have little efficacy in promoting weight loss, and only modest success in long-term reduction of binge eating. As a significant proportion of obese individuals entering weight-loss treatment and research programs are likely to meet criteria for BED, those conducting clinical research should be aware of this distinct subgroup and determine the contribution of BED to outcome measures. (OBESITY RESEARCH 1993; 1:306–324)
Article
The effects of several social-affective presentation contexts on the formation of preschool children's food preferences were investigated. The children's preferences for a set of snack foods were initially assessed and a neutral food, neither highly preferred nor nonpreferred, was selected for each child. This snack food was then presented to the child in one of four social-affective contexts: (1) as a reward; (2) noncontingently, paired with adult attention; (3) in a nonsocial context; (4) at snack time. 16 children participated in each condition. Half of the children in each condition received a sweet snack food, half a nonsweet snack food. Results indicated that presenting foods as rewards or presenting them noncontingently paired with adult attention produced significant increases in preference, and the effects persisted for at least 6 weeks following termination of the presentations. In contrast, no consistent changes in preference were noted when the foods were presented in a nonsocial context or at snack time. The results suggest that the social-affective context in which foods are presented is extremely important in the formation of young children's food preferences.
Article
[This volume] is devoted to a comprehensive and current review of all aspects of obesity and the eating disorders—including the interface of these with factors affecting weight regulation. Section I . . . covers such factors as genetic influences and physiological, psychological, and energy related factors. Section II focuses on the importance of dieting in our world today; social and cultural influences and their relation to body image and the eating disorders are covered in some detail, as is the entire controversy over dieting itself. Section III highlights the issue of measurement and covers everything from assessments of body energy stores to food intake, energy expenditure, and measures of psychopathology. Sections IV through VII cover all aspects of the eating disorders including history, clinical features, theories of pathogenesis and complications, as well as detailed views on approaches to treatments. Sections VIII and IX cover obesity in a similar fashion. This book contains . . . information on all aspects of eating disorders and obesity so that professionals with clinical or research questions in this area [and students] will have the . . . information at hand in one volume. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A pictorial instrument was developed to examine perceptions of body figure in a cross-sectional survey of 1118 preadolescent children. Hypothesis testing related to differences in figure selections by gender, grade, race, and school/community setting revealed males selected Ideal Self slightly thinner than Self. However, females selected Ideal Self significantly thinner than Self, as well as thinner than males' selection of Ideal Self and Ideal Girl. Additionally, females made thinner Ideal Adult figure selections than males. Though blacks chose heavier figures than whites, females of both races desired thinner figures. Bias toward thinness among females occurred across all levels of age, weight, race, and school/community setting, with 42% desiring thinner figures. Results of this study suggest that the onset of disparate figure perceptions and expectations regarding thinness among females may be evident as early as 6 and 7 years of age.
Article
To obtain information on the development of the learned control of food intake, preschool children (N = 22) participated in conditioning and extinction trials to determine whether meal size could be conditioned. Each trial consisted of a two-part snack in which consumption of a fixed amount of a distinctively flavored high (145 kcal) or low (60 kcal) caloric density preload was followed by ad lib consumption of snacks. Following the pairs of conditioning trials, extinction test trials were given in which the flavors previously paired with either high or low caloric density were presented in intermediate, isocaloric preloads. Conditioning occurred in one of two child feeding contexts: one that focused children on internal cues of hunger and satiety, or one that focused the children on external cues, including the amount of food remaining on the plate. The external group also could receive rewards for eating. Only the children in the internal context showed evidence of responsiveness to caloric density cues and associative conditioning, eating more ad lib following the low-density preload than following the high density preload during conditioning, and eating more during extinction following the flavor previously paired with low caloric density. Children in the external context showed no evidence of responsiveness to caloric density cues, but increased in ad lib consumption across the conditioning trials.