ArticlePDF Available

Pluralistic Ignorance and Hooking Up

Taylor & Francis
The Journal of Sex Research
Authors:

Abstract

Hooking up--when two people agree to engage in sexual behavior for which there is no future commitment--has become popular on college campuses. In this study we examined the extent to which pluralistic ignorance affects hooking up. One hundred thirty-six female and 128 male college students answered questions regarding their own comfort and their perceived peers comfort in engaging in a variety of sexual behaviors while hooking up. We hypothesized and found that both women and men rated their peers as being more comfortable engaging in these behaviors than they rated themselves. Men expressed more comfort than did women in engaging in these behaviors, and both sexes overestimated the other gender s comfort with hooking up behaviors. Pluralistic ignorance appears to apply to hooking up on college campuses, and we explore some potential consequences of pluralistic ignorance in this context.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (Taylor & Francis Group)
Pluralistic Ignorance and Hooking up
Author(s): Tracy A. Lambert, Arnold S. Kahn, Kevin J. Apple
Source:
The Journal of Sex Research,
Vol. 40, No. 2 (May, 2003), pp. 129-133
Published by: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (Taylor & Francis Group)
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3813749
Accessed: 12/03/2009 10:52
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.
Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=lebtaylorfrancis.
Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the
scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that
promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (Taylor & Francis Group) is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to The Journal of Sex Research.
http://www.jstor.org
Pluralistic
Ignorance
and
Hooking
Up
Tracy
A.
Lambert,
Arnold
S.
Kahn,
and Kevin
J.
Apple
James
Madison
University
"Hooking
up
"-when
two
people
agree
to
engage
in
sexual behavior
for
which there is no
future
commitment-has
become
popular
on
college campuses.
In
this
study
we
examined
the
extent to which
pluralistic ignorance
affects hooking
up.
One hundred
thirty-six
female
and
128
male
college
students
answered
questions regarding
their own
comfort
and
their
perceived peers
'comfort
in
engaging
in a
variety of
sexual behaviors while
hooking
up.
We
hypothesized andfound
that both
women and men
rated their
peers
as
being
more
comfortable engaging
in these behaviors
than
they
rated themselves. Men
expressed
more
comfort
than
did women
in
engaging
in these
behaviors,
and both sexes overestimated the other
gender's
comfort
with
hooking
up
behaviors.
Pluralistic
ignorance appears
to
apply
to
hooking up
on
college
campuses,
and we
explore
some
potential
consequences
of pluralistic
ignorance
in
this
context.
Although
one-night
stands
and
uncommitted sexual
behaviors
are
not a recent
phenomenon,
past
research
has
focused
on
personality
traits,
attitudes,
and individual
dif-
ferences
in
willingness
to
engage
in such behaviors
(e.g.,
Gerrard,
1980;
Gerrard
&
Gibbons,
1982;
Simpson
&
Gangestad,
1991;
Snyder,
Simpson,
&
Gangestad,
1986).
The
tacit
assumption
in
this
past
research
was that
sexual
behaviors
within
a committed
and
loving relationship
were
unproblematic,
but
that
unloving,
uncommitted sexual
relations
had
to be
explained.
However,
today
on
college
campuses
across
the United States
what was once
viewed
as
problematic
has now become
normative,
and students
refer to this
process
as
"hooking up."
Hooking up
occurs
when two
people
who are
casual
acquaintances
or
who
have
just
met that
evening
at a bar
or
party
agree
to
engage
in
some
forms
of sexual behavior
for
which there
will
likely
be
no future
commitment
(Boswell
&
Spade,
1996;
Kahn
et
al., 2000;
Paul,
McManus,
&
Hayes,
2000).
The
couple
typically
does
not communicate
what sexual
behaviors
they
will or
will not
engage
in,
and
frequently
both
parties
have been
drinking
alcohol
(Kahn
et
al.,
2000;
Paul et
al.,
2000).
Paul
et
al.
(2000)
found that
78%
of
women and
men
on the
campus
being
studied
had
engaged
in
hooking up
at least once.
In the Kahn et
al.
(2000)
sample
of
college
students,
86%
of
the
women and
88%
of the
men indicated
they
had
hooked
up.
Almost one
half
(47%)
of the
men
and one
third of the
women
in
the
Paul et al.
sample
engaged
in
sexual
intercourse
during
the
hookup,
and
Kahn
et al.
found
that their
sample
believed
petting
below the
waist,
oral
sex,
and
sexual intercourse
occurred
with some
regularity
in the
process
of
hooking up.
Pluralistic
ignorance,
a
concept
first coined
by
Floyd
Allport
(1924,
1933),
exists
when,
within
a
group
of
indi-
Tracy
A. Lambert
is now at
the
University
of
Georgia.
This
paper
is based
on an honors
thesis
by
the first
author under
the direction
of the second
author. We
wish to thank
Steven
Wise for his statistical
assistance.
Address
correspondence
to
Arnold
S.
Kahn,
School
of
Psychology,
MSC
7401,
James Madison
University,
Harrisonburg,
VA
22807;
e-mail:
kahnas
@jmu.edu.
viduals,
each
person
believes his or her
private
attitudes,
beliefs,
or
judgments
are
discrepant
from the norm dis-
played
by
the
public
behavior of others.
Therefore,
each
group
member,
wishing
to be seen as a desirable member
of
the
group, publicly
conforms to the
norm,
each believ-
ing
he or she is the
only
one in
the
group experiencing
conflict between his or
her
private
attitude and his
or her
public
behavior.
Group
members believe
that most others
in their
group,
especially
those
who
are
popular
and
opin-
ion leaders
(Katz
&
Lazarsfeld,
1955),
actually
endorse
the
norm and want to behave
that
way,
while
they
them-
selves
privately
feel
they
are
going
along
with the norm
because of a desire to fit
in
with
the
group
and
exemplify
the norm
(Prentice
&
Miller, 1993,
1996).
In
this
study
we
examined
the extent to which
pluralistic
ignorance might
be related to U.S.
college
students' comfort
levels with
sexual
behaviors involved
in
hooking up.
Consistent
with
the
premise
of
pluralistic ignorance,
we
hypothesized
that
college
students would
perceive
others as
having
a
greater
comfort level
engaging
in a
variety
of sexual behaviors
than
they
themselves
would have.
Prentice and
Miller
(1993)
demonstrated
pluralistic
ignorance among
college
students
in
the area
of
alcohol
consumption.
On
a
campus
where
heavy
alcohol use was
the
perceived
norm,
Prentice
and Miller found that stu-
dents estimated both
the
average
student and their
friends
to have less discomfort
with
the
level of alcohol
consump-
tion
on
campus
than
they
reported
for themselves.
Furthermore,
for
male
but not female
students,
they
found
greater
consistency
between
respondents'
comfort levels
with
alcohol
consumption
and the
perceived
norm and
between
respondents' reported drinking
levels and the
per-
ceived
norm at the end of the semester
than at the
begin-
ning
of the semester.
Although
correlational
in
nature,
these
results
suggest
that over
time,
male students
may
have
changed
their attitudes and behaviors
to
bring
them
more
in
line
with the
perceived
norm.
Perkins and
Berkowitz
(1986)
reported
similar
findings
with
regard
to
the
discrepancy
between
college
students' own comfort
The
Journal
of
Sex
Research
Volume
40,
Number
2,
May
2003:
pp.
129-133
129
Pluralistic
Ignorance
and
Hooking Up
with the amount
of
drinking
at
the
university
and
what
they
estimated to
be the
general campus
attitude.
Although
pluralistic
ignorance
was
originally conceptu-
alized as
a
discrepancy
between
public
behavior and
pri-
vate beliefs
(Miller
&
McFarland,
1987),
others have
used
the
concept
to refer to situations
in which there is
not
direct
evidence of behavioral
similarity (e.g.,
Fields
&
Schuman, 1976;
O'Gorman
&
Garry,
1976).
More
recent-
ly,
Cohen and Shotland
(1996)
invoked the
concept
of
plu-
ralistic
ignorance
in
a
variety
of
dating
situations
for which
public
scrutiny
was absent.
They
found
that
both men
and
women believed that the
average
other
person
of their sex
had
more liberal sexual
expectations
than
they
set
for
themselves,
both sexes
believing
the
average
other
person
of their
sex would
expect
sexual intercourse much sooner
in
a
relationship
than
they
themselves would
expect
it.
When asked
whether
a same-sex
peer
would
expect
to
have sexual
intercourse
with a
person
with whom
they
were
emotionally
involved
but for whom
they
felt no
phys-
ical
attraction,
both men and women
believed the
average
man
and
woman
would
expect
sexual
intercourse,
while
only approximately
50%
of the
participants
would
expect
sex themselves
in
such a
relationship,
and an
even smaller
percentage reported having
had sex
in
such a
relationship.
Finally,
when
there was neither
emotional
nor
physical
attraction to a
partner,
few women or men
expected
that
they
would have sexual intercourse with the
partner,
but
believed
the
average
man and woman would
indeed
expect
sexual intercourse.
Pluralistic
ignorance might
have
consequences
when
beliefs about
the norm
condone intimate sexual
behaviors.
In
the
process
of
hooking
up, pluralistic ignorance
may
lead one
or both
sexual
partners
to
act
according
to the
per-
ceived norm rather than to their own
convictions. There is
a
large
literature
showing
that
men have
more
liberal atti-
tudes towards sexual behaviors and
expect
sexual inter-
course sooner
in
a
relationship
than
do women
(Cohen
&
Shotland, 1996;
Knox
&
Wilson, 1981;
Oliver
&
Hyde,
1993)
and
that
men are much
more
receptive
than are
women
to offers of
sexual intercourse
(Clark
&
Hatfield,
1989).
Byers
and
Lewis
(1988)
found
that
disagreements
among dating partners
on
the desired level of
sexual
behavior
was almost
always
in the
direction
of
the male
partner wanting
a
higher
level of sexual
intimacy
than that
desired
by
the
female
partner.
Thus,
it is
possible
that
many
men
go
into
hooking-up
situations
hoping
to
engage
in
more intimate
sexual behaviors than are
desired
by
their
female
partners.
Because men are
expected
to
initiate
sex-
ual
activity
(DeLamater,
1987;
Peplau
&
Gordon,
1985),
it
is
possible
that
in the
process
of
hooking
up,
some women
will
experience
unwanted
sexual advances and
possibly
even sexual assault
or
rape.
In
their research on
hooking
up,
Kahn et
al.
(2000)
asked 92 female and
50 male
college
students
if
they
had
ever had a
"really
terrible
hooking up
experience."
Nearly
one half
of the women
(42%)
and the men
(46%)
indicat-
ed
they
had had
such an
experience.
A
"terrible
experi-
ence"
for the men
was
usually
due to
the women
wanting
a
relationship
or to the use of too
much alcohol or
drugs;
none mentioned
pressure
to
go
further than
they
desired.
However,
nearly
one half
of
the
women
(48.3%)
who
reported
having
a terrible
hooking-up
experience
indicated
that
they
were
pressured
to
go
further than
they
had
want-
ed
to
go. They gave responses
such as
"I
hooked
up
with
a
guy
who didn't understand
the
meaning
of
'no"'
and
"I
didn't want to-he did-he wouldn't back
off."
These
women
may
have
experienced
sexual assault
during
a
hook
up
but did not label their
experiences
as
such
because
they
believed
the
behaviors
to be
normative.
In
addition,
10.3%
of the women
and
11.1% of the
men
in
this
sample
said the hook
up
was
terrible
because
they
had
gone
too
far
without
mentioning pressure
from
partner. Going
too
far
might
have been the
consequence
of
pluralistic ignorance,
conforming
to a
presumed
norm.
The
present study
sought
to
extend the
findings
of
Cohen and
Shotland
(1996),
which were restricted to
expectations
of sexual
intercourse
in
dating
situations,
to
the
area of
hooking up.
Further,
we
wanted to examine
whether
pluralistic ignorance
occurred with other sexual
behaviors
besides sexual intercourse.
Based
on
the
research
on
pluralistic
ignorance
and
gender
differences
in
expected
sexual
behaviors,
we
hypothesized
that both
male and female
students would
see
other
students as more
comfortable with various
hooking-up
behaviors
than
they
were
themselves.
Although
we
expected
individuals would
vary
in
their own
comfort levels with
various
hooking-up
behaviors,
we
expected
they
would
believe other students
to be
uniformly
more comfortable
engaging
in
those
behaviors than
they
were themselves.
Furthermore,
consis-
tent with
previous
literature,
we
hypothesized
that men
would be
significantly
more comfortable than women
with
engaging
in
all
hooking-up
behaviors.
Finally,
we
hypoth-
esized that due to
pluralistic
ignorance,
both women and
men would
overestimate the other
gender's
comfort
with
all
hooking-up
behaviors.
METHOD
Participants
One
hundred
seventy-five
female and
152 male under-
graduate
students from a
mid-sized residential
southeast-
ern
public
university
that has few nontraditional
students
served as
participants
for the
study.
The
convenience sam-
ple
represented
a
moderately
even
distribution
of
year
in
school:
for first
years,
n
=
79
(41
females,
38
males);
for
sophomores,
n
=
70
(37
females,
33
males);
for
juniors,
n
=
84
(45
females,
39
males);
and for
seniors,
n
=
93
(52
females,
41
males).
A
female
experimenter
approached
students as
they
entered
the
university library
and
asked
them to
volunteer to
answer
some
questions
about
hooking
up
and
sexual
behaviors as
part
of
her senior honors
pro-
ject.
She
approached
other
students in
their
residence halls.
No
differences
appeared
between
these two
samples
for
any
of the
dependent
measures.
Analyses concerning
plu-
130
Lambert,
Kahn,
and
Apple
ralistic
ignorance
and
comfort with
hooking up
are
based
on the
data from
136 women
(77.7%)
and
128
men
(84.2%)
who
indicated that
they
had hooked
up.
Materials and
Procedure
The
questionnaire
consisted of
questions
developed by
the
authors to
examine student
attitudes toward
hooking
up.
On the
first
page,
students read
that the
investigator
was
interested
in
"students'
attitudes and
behaviors with
regard
to
dating
and
'hooking
up',"
and
hooking up
was
defined
as "a
sexual encounter between two
people
who
may
or
may
not know each
other
well,
but
who
usually
are
not
seriously
dating." Participants
also
signed
an informed
consent
form,
which
indicated
all
information would be
anonymous
and confidential. To
insure
anonymity
and
encourage
honest
responding,
the
only
demographic
infor-
mation obtained was the
participants' year
in
school. We
made no
attempt
to determine sexual
orientation or
marital
status; however,
on this
residential
campus
the over-
whelming
majority
of students come to the
university
directly
from
high
school,
identify
themselves
as
hetero-
sexual,
and have never been married
(James
Madison
University
Office
of Institutional
Research,
2001-2002).
Participants
were told
they
were not
required
to
complete
the
survey
if
they
became
uncomfortable.
Students who said
they
had
hooked
up
were
instructed
to continue to the next
page.
We
constructed
11-point
scales
modeled
after those used
by
Prentice and Miller
(1993)
on
which
3
points
were labeled:
1
=
very uncomfortable,
6
=
neutral,
and
11
=
very
comfortable.
Participants responded
to
the
question
"How comfortable are
you
with the
amount of
hooking
up
that
goes
on
at
[school name]?"
They
then
responded
to
the
question
"How comfortable are
you
with
engaging
in the
following
activities
during
a hook
up?"
with
regard
to
"petting
above the
waist,"
"petting
below
the
waist,"
"oral
sex,"
and
"sexual intercourse."
Participants
used
the same scales
in
response
to
the
questions
"How comfort-
able
do
you
think the
average
female
student is with
the
amount
of
hooking up
that
goes
on at
[school
name]"
and
"How comfortable do
you
think
the
average female
student
is
with
engaging
in
the
following
activities
during
a hook
up?" Finally, they
responded
to
the
questions
"How com-
fortable
do
you
think the
average
male student is with the
amount
of
hooking
up
that
goes
on at
[school
name]"
and
"How
comfortable
do
you
think the
average
male
student
is
with
engaging
in
the
following
activities
during
a hook
up?"
Students who indicated that
they
had never hooked
up
were asked to
skip
to
a different
page
of
the
survey,
which
contained
questions
regarding why they
did not hook
up
and
whether or not
they
believed
that there
was
a
relationship
between
hooking up
and sexual assault. This
part
of
the
questionnaire
was included so that all
participants
would
work
on the
survey
for
approximately
the same amount of
time without
knowing
who
had or had
not hooked
up.
Participants completed
the
questionnaire privately,
usu-
ally
within a short distance
of
the researcher. Those
partic-
ipating
at the
library
placed
their
completed
questionnaires
in a
large
box,
and
those
participating
in
their
residence
hall
placed
completed
questionnaires
in
a
large
envelope.
When
they
were
finished,
participants
were
encouraged
to
ask
questions
and
discuss
the
questionnaire
with the
researcher,
who
provided
them
with
a
debriefing
statement
and a list of
campus
resources
for sexual
assault.
RESULTS
We
tested the
hypothesis
that
students
would
experience
pluralistic
ignorance
regarding
hooking
up
with a 2
(Gender)
X 2
(Target:
self or
other)
ANOVA,
with
target
as
a
within-subjects
variable. As
hypothesized,
participants
demonstrated
pluralistic
ignorance
by
evaluating
their
own
comfort level with
the
amount of
hooking up,
M
=
7.08,
SD
=
2.31,
significantly
lower than their
estimate of a
same-sex
peer's
comfort
level,
M
=
7.75,
SD
=
2.08,
F
(1,
262)
=
24.24,
p
<
.0001,
partial
1f2
=
.085.
Participants
believed that
other
college
students
were more
comfort-
able with
the amount of
hooking up
than
were
they.
This
main effect
of
target
was
qualified
by
a
significant
gender
by
target
interaction,
F
(1, 262)
=
7.55,
partial
rl2
=
.028,
p
<
.01. Both
men
and
women
showed the
same
pattern
of
overestimating
their
peers'
comfort
levels;
however,
the
pattern
was more
pronounced
among
the male
students
(9.01peer
vs.
7.95self),
t
(127)
=
4.68,
p
<
.0001,
d
=
.502,
than
among
the
female students
(6.57peer
vs.
6.26seIf),
t
(135)
=
1.85,p
<
.05,
one-tailed,
d=
.185.
Table
1
presents
these means and
standard
deviations.
A
dependent
t test
for
equality
of
variances
revealed
that
participants
showed
significantly
less
variability
in
their
ratings
of
peer
comfort level in
hooking
up
than in
their
self-ratings,
t
(262)
=
1.96,
p
<
.05,
one-tailed.
When
rating
their
own
comfort
levels,
the
standard
deviation
in
partici-
pants' responses
was 2.31.
However,
the
standard
deviation
significantly
decreased to
2.08 when
participants
estimated
their
peers'
comfort
with
hooking up.
This
decrease in vari-
ability
when
estimating
others'
comfort
provides
some evi-
dence
for
an
illusion of
universality.
That
is,
participants
showed
greater
uniformity
in
their
beliefs
about
others'
comfort
levels
compared
to their
own actual
comfort levels.
We
used multivariate
analysis
of
variance
(MANOVA)
to
examine the
hypothesis
regarding
men's and
women's own
comfort levels
with various
sexual
behaviors,
with
partici-
pant
gender
as the
between-subjects
variable.
Men
reported
significantly
greater
comfort with
these
behaviors than
did
women,
F
(4,
259)
=
35.17,
p
<
.0001,
partial
r12
= .352.
Table
2
shows that men's
greater
comfort
occurred with
all
four
hooking-up
behaviors.
Table
1.
Ratings
of Own
and
Average
Same-Sex
Student's
Comfort With
Hooking
Up
Measure
Self
Average
student
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
Women
6.26
(1.79)
6.57
(1.63)
Men
7.95
(2.48)
9.01
(1.74)
Note.
Ratings
were
made on
11-point
scales
(1
=
not
at
all
comfortable
and
11
=
very
comfortable).
131
Pluralistic
Ignorance
and
Hooking Up
Table
2.
Ratings
of
Men and
Women's
Own
Comfort With
Hooking-Up
Behaviors
Petting
above
the
waist
Petting
below
the waist
Oral sex
Sexual
intercourse
Men
M
(SD)
9.12
(2.47)
8.42
(2.60)
7.56
(3.05)
5.65
(3.57)
Women
M
(SD)
7.29
(2.68)
5.13
(2.73)
3.49
(2.63)
2.15
(2.31)
We tested the
hypothesis
that both men and women
would overestimate the other
gender's
comfort with
hook-
ing-up
behaviors
using
two
separate
MANOVAS.
The
first examined the men's estimates of the
average
woman's
comfort,
and the
second examined the
women's
estimates
of
the
average
man's comfort.
To
evaluate the
accuracy
of these
estimates,
the
comfort estimates were
compared
with the
means
of
the actual comfort
ratings
of these
hooking-up
behaviors.
Both the
men,
F
(4,
259)
=
7.82,
p
<
.0001,
partial
112
=
.108,
and
the
women,
F
(4,
259)
=
16.25,
p
<
.0001,
partial
112
=
.201,
significantly
overestimated the other
gender's
actual
comfort levels
with various
hooking-up
behaviors. As shown in
Table
3,
this overestimation occurred for
both sexes on each of the
four
hooking-up
behaviors.
DISCUSSION
Cohen and
Shotland
(1996)
found evidence of
pluralistic
ignorance regarding expectations
of sexual
intercourse on
a date. The current
research extended these
findings
to
other sexual
behaviors,
and
did so in
the context of
hook-
ing up.
We found that both
women
and men
reported
less
comfort with their
perceived
norm
of
hooking
up
than
they
believed was
experienced by
their
same-sex
peers,
with
men
showing
a
greater
difference between self- and
peer-
ratings
than women.
In
addition,
both
men
and women
believed
members of
the other
gender
experienced greater
comfort with
hooking-up
behaviors than
members of the
other
gender
actually
reported.
Men
were
less comfortable
with
engaging
in
hooking-up
behaviors than women
believed
them to
be,
and women were less
comfortable
with
engaging
in
hooking-up
behaviors than men
believed
them
to be. These
findings appear
to
be due to
pluralistic
ignorance: Hooking up
has become
the
norm
for hetero-
sexual sexual
relationships
on this
campus,
and since the
great majority
of
students
do in
fact hook
up,
it
appears
that most students believe that others are comfortable-
more comfortable
than
they
are themselves-with
engag-
ing
in a
variety
of uncommitted
sexual behaviors. It is like-
ly
that most
students believe others
engage
in
these hook-
ing-up
behaviors
primarily
because
they
enjoy doing
so,
while
they
see themselves
engaging
in
these behaviors
pri-
marily
due to
peer pressure.
Consistent with other
pluralistic ignorance
research
(e.g.,
Prentice &
Miller,
1993),
this
study
showed evidence
of an illusion of
universality.
The
students failed to
appre-
ciate the extent
to
which others have
different comfort lev-
els with
hooking-up
behaviors.
That
is,
students
wrongly
assumed
that the attitudes of
others about
hooking up
were
more
homogenous
than
they
actually
were.
Similar
to
other
researchers
(Cohen
&
Shotland,
1996;
Knox
&
Wilson,
1981;
Oliver
&
Hyde,
1993),
we
found
that men
expressed greater
comfort than did
women with
sexually
intimate
hooking-up
behaviors. In the
context of
hooking up,
this could lead to
serious
consequences.
Our
study
suggests
that men believe
women are
more com-
fortable
engaging
in
these
behaviors than
in
fact
they
are,
and also that
women believe other
women
are more com-
fortable
engaging
in
these
behaviors than
they
are
them-
selves. As a
consequence,
some men
may
pressure
women to
engage
in
intimate sexual
behaviors,
and some
women
may
engage
in
these
behaviors or resist
only
weakly
because
they
believe
they
are
unique
in
feeling
discomfort about
engaging
in
them. In this
context it is
possible
for a woman
to
experience
sexual
assault but not
interpret
the behavior as
such,
believing
it to be
norma-
tive
behavior with which her
peers
are
comfortable.
"Most
of
Us" is a
campaign implemented
on
many
col-
lege campuses
in
an
attempt
to
reveal
pluralistic
igno-
rance about
alcohol
consumption among
college
students
(DeJong
&
Langford,
2002;
Haines,
1998).
The
campaign
is
based
on
providing
students with
statistical evidence
about actual
student attitudes
and behaviors
regarding
alcohol
consumption.
The
goal
of the
campaign
is to show
that
pluralistic ignorance
exists
regarding college
stu-
dent's
heavy
alcohol
consumption,
and that
most students
prefer
to drink
less than
what is
commonly
perceived
to
be the
norm.
Considering
the results of
this
study,
we
pro-
pose
that a
similar
campaign
highlighting
students' beliefs
about and
comfort
levels with
sexual
behaviors while
hooking
up might
help
reduce
pluralistic
ignorance
about
hooking up.
Table 3. Differences Between
Each Gender's Own
Comfort Level With
Hooking-Up
Behaviors and
Estimates
of
the Other
Gender's Comfort
Levels
Petting
above the waist
Petting
below the waist
Oral sex
Sexual intercourse
Women's
estimate of men
M
(SD)
9.80
(1.45)
9.30
(1.67)
8.61
(1.93)
7.62
(2.24)
Men's
actual
comfort level
M
(SD)
9.12
(2.47)
8.42
(2.60)
7.56
(3.04)
5.65
(3.57)
Men's
estimate
of
women
M
(SD)
7.73
(1.92)
6.38
(2.07)
5.49
(2.19)
4.28
(2.36)
Women's actual
comfort level
M
(SD)
7.29
(2.68)
5.13
(2.73)
3.49
(2.62)
2.15
(2.31)
132
Lambert,
Kahn,
and
Apple
REFERENCES
Allport,
F.
H.
(1924).
Social
psychology.
Boston:
Houghton
Mifflin.
Allport,
F. H.
(1933).
Institutional behavior.
Chapel
Hill:
University
of
North Carolina
Press.
Boswell, A.,
&
Spade,
J.
(1996).
Fraternities and
collegiate rape
culture.
Gender
and
Society,
10,
133-147.
Byers,
S.,
&
Lewis,
K.
(1988).
Dating
couples'
disagreements
over the desired
level
of
sexual
intimacy.
The Journal
of
Sex
Research,
24,
15-29.
Clark,
R.
D.,
&
Hatfield,
E.
(1989).
Gender differences
in
receptivity
to
sex-
ual
offers. Journal
of Psychology
and Human
Sexuality,
2,
39-55.
Cohen,
L.
L.,
&
Shotland,
R. L.
(1996).
Timing
of first sexual intercourse
in
a
relationship: Expectation, experiences,
and
perceptions
of others.
The Journal
of
Sex
Research, 33,
291-299.
DeLamater,
J.
(1987).
Gender differences
in sexual scenarios.
In
K.
Kelley
(Ed.),
Females,
males,
and
sexuality (pp.
127-139).
Albany,
NY: SUNY
Press.
DeJong,
W.,
&
Langford,
L.
A.
(2002).
Typology
for
campus-based
alcohol
prevention:
Moving
toward
environmental
management strategies.
Journal
of
Studies
on Alcohol
Supplement,
14,
140-147.
Fields,
J.
M.,
&
Schuman,
H.
(1976).
Public beliefs and
the beliefs
of the
public.
Public
Opinion
Quarterly,
40,
427-448.
Gerrard,
M.
(1980).
Sex
guilt
and
attitudes towards
sex
in
sexually
active
and
inactive
female
college
students.
Journal
of
Personality
Assessment,
44,258-261.
Gerrard,
M.,
&
Gibbons,
F.
X.
(1982).
Sexual
experience,
sex
guilt,
and
sexual
moral
reasoning.
Journal
of
Personality,
50,
345-359.
Haines,
M.
(1998).
Social
norms:
A wellness
model
for health
promotion
in
higher
education.
Wellness
Management,
14(4),
1-8.
James
Madison
University
Office
of Institutional
Research
(2001-2002).
Retrieved
February
22,
2003,
from
http://www.jmu.edu/instresrch/
statsum/2001_02/2001-02toc.htm
Kahn,
A.
S.,
Fricker, K., Hoffman,
J.,
Lambert,
T.,
Tripp,
M.,
Childress,
K.,
et
al.
(2000,
August).
Hooking
up:
Dangerous
new
dating
methods?
In
A.
S.
Kahn
(Chair),
Sex,
unwanted
sex,
and sexual
assault
on
college
campuses. Symposium
conducted
at
the
annual
Meeting
of the
American
Psychological
Association,
Washington,
DC.
Katz,
E.,
&
Lazarsfeld,
P.
F.
(1955).
Personal
influence:
The
part played
by
people
in the
flow
of
mass communication.
Glencoe,
IL: Free
Press.
Knox, D.,
&
Wilson,
K.
(1981).
Dating
behaviors of
university
students.
Family
Relations, 30,
255-258.
Miller,
T.
D.,
&
McFarland,
C.
(1987).
Pluralistic
ignorance:
When
similar-
ity
is
interpreted
as
dissimilarity.
Journal
of
Personality
and
Social
Psychology,
53,
298-305.
O'Gorman,
H.
J.,
&
Garry,
S.
L.
(1976).
Pluralistic
ignorance:
A
replication
and extension.
Public
Opinion
Quarterly,
40,
449-458.
Oliver,
M.
B.,
&
Hyde,
J.
S.,
(1993).
Gender differences
in
sexuality:
A
meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 114,
129-151.
Paul,
E.
L., McManus,
B.,
&
Hayes,
A.
(2000).
"Hookups":
Characteristics
and correlates
of
college
students'
spontaneous
and
anonymous
sexual
experiences.
The
Journal
of
Sex
Research,
37,
76-88.
Peplau,
L.
A.,
&
Gordon,
S. L.
(1985).
Women and
men in love: Gender dif-
ferences
in
close heterosexual
relationships.
In V. E.
O'Leary,
R. K.
Unger,
&
B. S. Wallston
(Eds.),
Women,
gender,
and social
psychology
(pp.
257-292).
Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Perkins,
H.
W.,
&
Berkowitz,
A. D.
(1986).
Perceiving
the
community
norms of alcohol use
among
students: Some
research
implications
for
campus
alcohol
education
programming.
International
Journal
of
the
Addictions, 21,
961-976.
Prentice,
D.
A.,
&
Miller,
D. T.
(1993).
Pluralistic
ignorance
and alcohol use
on
campus:
Some
consequences
of
misperceiving
the
social
norm.
Journal
of
Personality
and Social
Psychology,
64,
243-256.
Prentice,
D.
A.,
&
Miller,
D.
T.
(1996).
Pluralistic
ignorance
and the
per-
petuation
of
social
norms
by
unwitting
actors.
In M. P. Zanna
(Ed.),
Advances
in
experimental
social
psychology
(Vol.
28,
pp.
161-209).
San
Diego,
CA:
Academic
Press.
Simpson,
J.
A.,
&
Gangestad,
S. W.
(1991).
Individual
differences
socio-
sexuality:
Evidence
for
convergent
and
discriminant
validity.
Journal
of
Personality
and Social
Psychology,
60,
870-883.
Snyder,
M.,
Simpson,
J.
A.,
&
Gangestad,
S.
(1986).
Personality
and sexu-
al
relations.
Journal
of
Personality
and
Social
Psychology,
51,
181-190.
Manuscript accepted
August
21,
2002
133
... Relatively more recently, within the past 30 years there has been a revival in research dedicated to this topic, particularly exploring several negative or risky behaviors among college students. Research has found that there are misperceptions of social norms when it comes to alcohol use (Perkins, 2002;Suls & Green, 2003), sexual behaviors (Kuperberg & Padgett, 2017;Lambert et al., 2003;Paul et al., 2000), smoking and illegal drug use (Hines et al., 2002), and binge eating (Crandall, 1988) among undergraduates. These misperceptions of social norms can contribute to negative outcomes, and individuals may then regret or debate the decisions that led to those outcomes (Sargent & Newman, 2021). ...
... Furthermore, the research indicates that gender differences can play a notable role in the emergence of pluralistic ignorance, with women showing more concern about campus alcohol practices and norms than their perceptions of concern from both same-and opposite-sex peers (Suls & Green, 2003). On a related note, research findings also demonstrate an expected double standard of men conveying more comfort with casual sexual behaviors than women, but both groups overestimate the comfort of their peers concerning these behaviors (Lambert et al., 2003). ...
Article
Co-curricular activities have been shown to impact many measures of academic success, such as academic self-confidence, leadership abilities, identity development, personal development, and student thriving. Borrowing from established research paradigms on pluralistic ignorance, the current study sought to explore whether first-year and senior students' perceptions of themselves and others vary when it comes to attending and participating in campus activities and whether sex has an impact on those perceptions. Using data from the 2018 National Survey of Student Engagement, the study included 10,512 students attending 33 institutions. The results suggest significant differences across the sexes and within reference group (self vs. other) for attending campus activities and participating in co-curricular activities. These findings can help guide faculty, advisors, and administrators to better understand how students' perceptions of peer co-curricular activities might relate to their own, and how those perceptions might affect their behavior positively and negatively.
... Contrary to other social behavior, sexual behavior is not directly observable and is particularly prone to inferences based on peer conversations. Consequently, young adults tend to overestimate how sexually active their peers are (Chia & Lee, 2008), the frequency with which their peers engage in risky sexual behavior (Lewis et al., 2014;Scholly et al., 2005), and how comfortable peers are engaging in various sexual activities with hookups (Lambert et al., 2003). While young adults tend to rely on friends as one of the primary sources of sexual health information (Chia, 2006;Trinh et al., 2014) they generally finding it challenging to communicate honestly about sex with peers (Chang, 2014;Noar et al., 2006). ...
... Pluralistic ignorance describes a social phenomenon in which individuals privately disagree with a group's perceived norms, but mistakenly believe that their views are uncommon [1]. This often stems from a desire for social acceptance, leading individuals to conform outward while hiding their true beliefs [2,3,4,5]. Pluralistic ignorance can have important implications for group dynamics, decision-making, and social behavior, as it can lead to the perpetuation of norms and beliefs that might not actually reflect the majority's true sentiments. It can also hinder open and honest communication within a group or society [6,7]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Pluralistic ignorance is a social-psychological phenomenon that occurs when individuals privately hold beliefs that differ from perceived group norms. Traditional models, based on opinion dynamics with private and public states, fail to account for a key aspect: when nonexpression aligns with normative behavior, initial social pressure can induce pluralistic ignorance. We show that pluralistic ignorance persists under infrequent imitation and strong initial minority influence. Although individuals can overcome this ignorance by the end of interactions, it reemerges in subsequent meetings. However, excessive imitation erases pluralistic ignorance, leading to a uniform state in which internal and external states align. Furthermore, incorporating memory into the internalization process shows that pluralistic ignorance peaks at moderate imitation levels.
... Suitable methodologies that would allow the determination of selection coefficients include: monitoring the release of various neurotransmitters in recipients in response to stimuli (Doll et al., 2016;Lee Y-A and Goto, 2018;Rogers, 2011;Sebold et al., 2019;Stanley, 2021); psychological profiling (Brady et al., 2017;Kleider et al., 2008;Latané, 1981;Lambert et al., 2003;Suls and Green, 2003;Stevens, 1957;Willer et al., 2009); and through literature review (Smaldino and McElreath, 2016). However, while selection can be included in quasispecies models (Wilke, 2005), they do not require that selection be considered within them (e.g., Bull, Meyers and Lachmann, 2005). ...
Article
We present a novel mode of cultural evolution whereby some forms of transmission may be modelled as quasispecies. The model incorporates the effect of high rates of error in certain forms of communication; while also building on the structural similarities between biological molecules and written language. Firstly, both written language and key biological molecules, such as RNA and proteins, are modular. Within these molecules, structural domains may be recombined, while retaining their function. Likewise, sentences are structured as combinations of clauses, in which each clause contains a domain of information. The clausal structure permits the recombination of information to adopt different meanings, while allowing each unit to retain its identity. Secondly, by virtue of intrinsically-high error rates, we show that some, but not all, aspects of communicated culture information exists as rapidly evolving clouds within the population. These clouds of cultural information behave as quasispecies, which we model with varying mutation rates and suitable selection coefficients. We then integrate these ideas with the application of Shannon Diversity Index to produce a more holistic view of culture that is centred on the evolution of its information. Re-imagining culture, as evolving clouds of information, unifies the mode in which information is stored culturally and biologically, and opens up new avenues of comparative analysis.
... At this time of increased independence, good decisions can lead to greater well-being; however, this is not always the case. In American college populations, it has been estimated that the lifetime prevalence rates of casual sexual relationships and experiences (CSREs) are around 62% for 'friends with benefits' relationships and booty calls (a non-long-term relationship partner with the intent of having sex) (Bisson & Levine, 2009;Jonason et al., 2009) and between 53% and 84% for one-night stands and hook-ups Lambert et al., 2003;Paul et al., 2000), with these CSREs uncommitted and sexual in nature (Claxton et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Chronic insecurities that emerge from adverse experiences in early intimate partner relationships in adolescence and emerging adulthood can have profound impacts on mental health and well‐being. Less clear is the extent to which these experiences for parents impact subsequent relationships within and across generations. We examine the extent to which secure, dismissing, pre‐occupied, and fearful intimate partner relationships in adolescence and emerging adulthood, well before becoming a parent, are associated with next‐generation patterns of attachment between mothers and infant offspring. Data were drawn from a nested study of infant–mother attachment ( n = 220) within the Australian Temperament Project Generation 3 Study ( N = 1167, est. 1983). Intimate partner relationships in adolescence and young adulthood were assessed by self‐report at 23–24 years of age. Over a decade later, infant–mother attachment security was assessed at 12 months post‐partum. Young adult intimate partner relationships defined by high levels of fearful, pre‐occupied, and dismissing attachment styles were reported in 11%, 17%, and 38% of young mothers, respectively. Increases in fear of intimacy in relationships were associated with an increase in the odds, by around 50%, of infant–mother insecure attachments (vs secure; OR = 1.56, 95% CI = 1.07, 2.28) and disorganised attachments (vs organised; OR = 1.49, 95% CI = 1.00, 2.22). A mother's self‐reported history of fear of intimacy within young adult relationships predicts later insecure and disorganised mother–infant attachments. Guidance and greater support for young people navigating their earliest intimate relationships may not only prevent adverse relational experiences at the time but also on becoming a parent. Findings have relevance for family and infant mental health therapies. Translating these findings into supported conversations may help prevent infant–mother attachment difficulties, or later repair them, through validation of the lingering effects of early fear of intimacy and empowerment of parents to prevent next‐generation infant experiences of distrust.
... Some r/IncelExit members questioned men's presumed entitlement to sex and women's bodies by critiquing unrealistic expectations surrounding teen sexual relationships. Mirroring social scientific research that has demonstrated young people often overestimate the amount of sex their peers are having (Chia & Gunther, 2006;Cohen & Shotland, 1996;Lambert et al., 2003), some Incels blamed such unrealistic expectations for fostering the sexual disappointment that many Incels exhibit. One member reflected this by stating: ...
Thesis
Full-text available
In a gender-segregated digital space known as the Manosphere, a group of men calling themselves “Incels,” or involuntary celibates, express feelings of hostility and hatred towards women. Incels hold a low position on a masculine hierarchy where the men who hold the most power in society are those who have access to women’s emotional and sexual services. Incels are characterized by feelings of entitlement to women’s services and aggrievement by their inability to access them. As a result, they often appear to fantasize about or engage in acts of violence to compensate for their lack of masculine privilege. However, there are men who try to escape these harmful ideologies and leave the Incel community altogether. In this study, I analyze 130 Reddit threads from a community of men attempting to leave the Incel community. This case study permits the examination of the ways low-status men challenge and/or reinforce the masculine hierarchy. Findings demonstrate the pervasive power of the masculine hierarchy. Aside from a few exceptions, men who were presumably attempting to leave the Incel community embraced their low status on the masculine hierarchy and accepted the inevitability of that hierarchy. Their reluctance to challenge the masculine hierarchy suggests they recognize that this hierarchy both justifies their subjugation under more privileged men and their power over women. In other words, for these men, the masculine hierarchy is simultaneously a prison and a bastion of masculine privilege.
... Similar outcomes can also be induced by few "contrarians" loudly expressing their minoritarian opinions and by spiralling mechanisms that lead a disagreeing majority into silence [12]. Despite evidences of the effect of misperceptions in supporting unpopular behaviors and opinions in various real world settings [11,13,14,15,16,17], we still lack a clear understanding of how social biases emerge and impact opinion expression in online social systems. arXiv:2307.04475v1 ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In the last decades an increasing deal of research has investigated the phenomenon of opinion misperception in human communities and, more recently, in social media. Opinion misperception is the wrong evaluation by community's members of the real distribution of opinions or beliefs about a given topic. In this work we explore the mechanisms giving rise to opinion misperception in social media groups, which are larger than physical ones and have peculiar topological features. By means of numerical simulations, we suggest that the structure of connections of such communities plays indeed a role in distorting the perception of the agents about others' beliefs, but it is essentially an indirect effect. Moreover, we show that the main ingredient that generates the misperception is a spiral of silence induced by few, well connected and charismatic agents, which rapidly drives the majority of individuals to stay silent without disclosing their true belief, leading minoritarian opinions to appear more widespread throughout the community.
... Contrary to other social behavior, sexual behavior is not directly observable and is particularly prone to inferences based on peer conversations. Consequently, young adults tend to overestimate how sexually active their peers are (Chia & Lee, 2008), how often their peers engage in risky sexual behavior (Lewis et al., 2012;Scholly et al., 2005) and how comfortable peers are with engaging in various sexual activities with hook-ups (Lambert et al., 2003). In accordance with social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), individuals tend to adjust their behavior to what they perceive to be the norm, including risky sexual behavior among young adults (Lewis et al., 2012). ...
Preprint
To defeat sexually transmitted infections, it is crucial to understand the social dynamics that shape sexual health norms. The current research tested whether, peer-to-peer interactions strengthened young adults’ intentions to use a condom and to perceive condom use as normative. Data from 3,041 young adults collected after an annual condom campaign during four years were analysed and combined into a meta-analysis. Results revealed that interacting about topics and engaging in activities related to the condom campaign was associated with higher condom use intention and more positive perceptions of pro-condom norms. Implications for the design of intervention campaigns are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
In the last decades an increasing deal of research has investigated the phenomenon of opinion misperception in human communities and, more recently, in social media. Opinion misperception is the wrong evaluation by community’s members of the real distribution of opinions or beliefs about a given topic. In this work we explore the mechanisms giving rise to opinion misperception in social media groups, which are larger than physical ones and have peculiar topological features. By means of numerical simulations, we suggest that the structure of connections of such communities plays indeed a role in distorting the perception of the agents about others’ beliefs, but it is essentially an indirect effect. Moreover, we show that the main ingredient that generates misperception is a spiral of silence induced by few, well connected and charismatic agents, which rapidly drives the majority of individuals to stay silent without disclosing their true belief, leading minoritarian opinions to appear more widespread throughout the community.
Article
Sexual violence (SV) experienced by higher education students is a prevalent public health problem. Collecting data on SV through self-report surveys in higher education institutions (HEIs) is essential for estimating the scope of the problem, the first step to adequately resourcing and implementing prevention and response programming and policies. However, in the United Kingdom, data is limited. We used data from the cross-sectional Oxford Understanding Relationships, Sex, Power, Abuse and Consent Experiences survey, administered to all students at a university in the United Kingdom in May 2021 ( n = 25,820), to estimate the past year prevalence of SV. We analyzed data from respondents who answered at least one question on SV ( n = 1,318) and found that 20.5% of respondents experienced at least one act of attempted or forced sexual touching or rape, and 52.7% of respondents experienced at least one act of sexual harassment (SH). We found that women experienced the highest rates of SV. Attempted forced sexual touching was far more common than forced sexual touching, or rape. Sexist remarks or jokes were the most common act of SH. Most acts of SV took place at the university. These findings reveal that the prevalence of SV in HEIs in the United Kingdom could be far higher than what is experienced in the general population. While this study reflects the context in only one institution, it underlines the need for continued monitoring to develop rigorous, evidence-based, and targeted prevention and response strategies.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis surveyed 177 usable sources that reported data on gender differences on 21 different measures of sexual attitudes and behaviors. The largest gender difference was in incidence of masturbation: Men had the greater incidence (d = .96). There was also a large gender difference in attitudes toward casual sex: Males had considerably more permissive attitudes (d = .81). There were no gender differences in attitudes toward homosexuality or in sexual satisfaction. Most other gender differences were in the small-to-moderate range. Gender differences narrowed from the 1960s to the 1980s for many variables. Chodorow's neoanalytic theory, sociobiology, social learning theory, social role theory, and script theory are discussed in relation to these findings.
Article
Data on 334 university students in a random sample revealed how students meet, where they go, and what they do on dates. Sexual behaviors of these students also indicate that men expect more sexual intimacy sooner in a fewer number of dates than women. Influences/involvements by their parents in their dating relationships are also discussed. Most students feel positive about limited parental involvement.
Article
Pluralistic ignorance begins with a discrepancy between public actions and private sentiments, typically produced by widespread behavioral adherence to a social norm. Pluralistic ignorance is a pervasive feature of social life and is found to characterize the dynamics of social situations, social groups, and social movements. Group identification is the root cause for many cases of pluralistic ignorance—that individuals often act out of a desire to be good group members but interpret others' similarly motivated behavior as reflecting personal beliefs and opinions. This chapter presents two cases of pluralistic ignorance: (1) concerning the attitudes of college students toward alcohol use on campus and (2) concerning the gender stereotypes held by elementary school children. Mean responses of students in percentage are graphically represented in the chapter. Pluralistic ignorance describes the case in which virtually every member of a group or society privately rejects a belief, opinion, or practice, yet believes that virtually every other member privately accepts it. The term “pluralistic ignorance” is something of a misnomer, for in these cases, group members are not, in fact, ignorant of one another's private sentiments; rather, they think they know, but are mistaken.
Article
Social interactions at fraternities that undergraduate women identified as places where there is a high risk of rape are compared to those at fraternities identified as low risk as well as two local bars. Factors that contribute to rape are common on this campus; however, both men and women behaved differently in different settings. Implications of these findings are considered.
Article
We investigated 242 college students’ expectations about when sexual intercourse would first occur within different types of relationships. Participants reported their personal expectations, experiences, and beliefs about the sexual expectations of the “average woman” and “average man” in relationships with or without strong physical attraction and emotional involvement. Results show that, in general, men expect sexual intercourse after significantly fewer dates (approximately 9–11) than women do (approximately 15–18). In addition, expectations were related to actual experiences for women, but not men. Participants also tended to exhibit pluralistic ignorance about sex, believing in a norm for the average person of their gender that was considerably more permissive than were their own standards. The average man and woman were perceived as expecting sex earlier within a relationship and irrespective of physical attraction or emotional involvement. In contrast, most men and women only expected sex when they were attracted to a partner, although men were more likely than women to expect sex in the absence of emotional closeness. Implications of pluralistic ignorance and gender differences in sexual expectations are discussed.
Article
According to cultural stereotypes, men are more eager for sex than are women; women are more likely to set limits on such activity. In this paper, we review the work of theorists who have argued in favor of this proposition and review the interview and correlational data which support this contention. Finally, we report two experimental tests of ihis hypothesis. In these experiments, conducted in 1978 and 1982, male and female confederates of average attractiveness approached potential partners with one of three requests: "Would you go out tonight?" "Will you come over to my apartment?" or "Would you go to bed with me?" The great majority of men were willing to have a sexual liaison with the women who approached them. Women were not. Not one woman agreed to a sexual liaison. Many possible reasons for this marked gender difference were discussed. These studies were run in 1978 and 1982. It has since become important to track how the threat of AIDS is affecting men and women's willingness to date, come to an apartment, or to engage in casual sexual relations.
Article
This study focused on a specific risky practice common among contemporary college students: the hookup. Hookups are defined as a sexual encounter which may or may not include sexual intercourse, usually occurring on only one occasion between two people who are strangers or brief acquaintances. The aim of this study was to determine the relative importance of a variety of social and psychological predictors in understanding differences among undergraduate students who had never hooked up, those who had hooked up without sexual intercourse, and those who had hooked up with sexual intercourse. Analyses revealed that, as predicted, social, individual, and relational psychological variables helped to explain the variance among college students' varied hookup experiences. By examining the full range of sexual involvement characteristic of the casual sexual phenomenon of hooking up within a multivariate model, we were able to achieve a more differentiated understanding of college students' casual sexual experimentation.
Article
A collection of essays reinterpreting contemporary social organization in psychological terms. Harvard Book List (edited) 1938 #227 (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)