Nicky Staes

Nicky Staes
Centre for Research and Conservation

PhD Biology - Primatology
Post-doctoral researcher studying the evolution of the primate social brain

About

49
Publications
12,369
Reads
How we measure 'reads'
A 'read' is counted each time someone views a publication summary (such as the title, abstract, and list of authors), clicks on a figure, or views or downloads the full-text. Learn more
548
Citations
Introduction
My research focuses on identifying the proximate origins of behavioral variation between and within great ape species. Great apes, much like humans, show remarkable variation in levels of social behavior, communication, and personality. I am interested in figuring out the mechanisms regulating this variation between and across species, with a special focus on the functioning of neuropeptide and neurotransmitter receptors that affect the social brain.
Additional affiliations
October 2018 - present
University of Antwerp
Position
  • PostDoc Position
Description
  • I am a postdoctoral researcher working at UA and CRC, KMDA on the proximate origins of differences in social cognition and behavior between and within bonobos and chimpanzees, using a candidate gene approach.
February 2016 - present
George Washington University
Position
  • PostDoc Position
Description
  • I was a postdoctoral researcher working at the Primate Genomics Lab and the Lab for Evolutionary Neuroscience, where my goal was to study both genetic and neural factors underlying individual and phylogenetic variation on communication in chimpanzees.
December 2010 - January 2016
University of Antwerp
Position
  • PhD Student
Description
  • During my PhD I studied variation in two candidate genes that code for the receptors for oxytocin and vasopressin, and their association with stable individual personality profiles, both within and between bonobos and chimpanzees.
Education
September 2005 - June 2010
University of Antwerp
Field of study
  • Biology

Publications

Publications (49)
Article
Full-text available
Humans show remarkable differences in social behaviour between families, groups, communities and cultures, whereas such group-level within-species variation in socio-behavioural propensities is typically overlooked in other species. Studies on intraspecific variation in animal social structures are needed to inform an evolutionary account of human...
Article
Full-text available
Human evaluation of animal emotional expressivity can inform animal welfare. Qualitative Behavioural Assessment (QBA) has been applied to domesticated and some non-domesticated animals, but its use in primates is limited despite their emotional expressivity. We aimed to develop and apply a QBA for bonobos (Pan paniscus) through two consecutive stud...
Article
Full-text available
Bonobos are typically portrayed as more socially tolerant than chimpanzees, yet the current evidence supporting such a species-level categorization is equivocal. Here, we used validated group-level co-feeding assays to systematically test expressions of social tolerance in sixteen groups of zoo- and sanctuary-housed bonobos and chimpanzees. We foun...
Article
Body condition, a measure for relative fat mass, is associated with primate health, fitness, and overall welfare. Body condition is often influenced by dietary factors, age, and/or sex, but several body condition measures (body weight, weight-to-height ratios, and so on) also show high heritability across primate species, indicating a role of genet...
Article
Full-text available
Individual variation in complex social behavioral traits, like primate grooming, can be influenced by the characteristics of the individual and those of its social group. To better grasp this complexity, social network analysis can be used to quantify direct and indirect grooming relationships. However, multi-group social network studies remain rar...
Article
Joint attention (JA) is an important milestone in human infant development and is predictive of the onset of language later in life. Clinically, it has been reported that children at risk for or with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) perform more poorly on measures of JA compared to neurotypical controls. JA is not unique to humans but...
Method
This is a long-term ethogram compiled to study various aspects of bonobo behaviour, originally set up for Planckendael bonobos, later also used for various other zoos.
Article
Full-text available
While low serotonergic activity is often associated with psychological disorders such as depression, anxiety, mood, and personality disorders, variations in serotonin also contribute to normal personality differences. In this study, we investigated the role of blood DNA methylation levels at individual CpG sites of two key serotonergic genes (serot...
Article
Full-text available
Self-directed behaviours (SDBs) are widely used as markers of emotional arousal in primates, and are commonly linked to negative arousal, or are used as indicators of stress or poor welfare. However, recent studies suggest that not all SDBs have the same function. Moreover, lateralisation in the production of these behaviours has been suggested to...
Preprint
Full-text available
While low serotonergic activity is often associated with psychological disorders like depression, anxiety, mood and personality disorders, variations in serotonin also contribute to normal personality differences. Here we investigate the role of blood DNA methylation levels at individual CpG sites of two key serotonergic genes (serotonin receptor g...
Article
The superior temporal sulcus (STS) is a conserved fold that divides the middle and superior temporal gyri. In humans, there is considerable variation in the shape, folding pattern, lateralization, and depth of the STS that have been reported to be associated with social cognition and linguistic functions. We examined the role that genetic factors p...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to construct a composite model of Dyadic Cofeeding Tolerance (DCT) in zoo-housed bonobos and chimpanzees using a validated experimental cofeeding paradigm and to investigate whether components resulting from this model differ between the two species or vary with factors such as sex, age, kinship and social bond strength. Using dime...
Article
Chimpanzees have consistent individual differences in behaviour, also referred to as personality. Similar to human personality structure, five dimensions are commonly found in chimpanzee studies that show evidence for convergent and predictive validity (Dominance, Openness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Reactivity/Undependability). These dimensi...
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies reported contrasting conclusions concerning bonobo prosociality, which are likely due to differences in the experimental design, the social dynamics among subjects and characteristics of the subjects themselves. Two hypotheses have been proposed to explain the occurrence of prosociality in animals: the cooperative breeding hypothes...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding underlying genetic variation can elucidate how diversity in behavioral phenotypes evolves and is maintained. Genes in the serotonergic signaling pathway, including the serotonin transporter gene ( SERT ), are candidates for affecting animal personality, cognition and fitness. In a model species, the great tit ( Parus major ), we reeva...
Article
Full-text available
Infectious diseases can be considered a threat to animal welfare and are commonly spread through both direct and indirect social interactions with conspecifics. This is especially true for species with complex social lives, like primates. While several studies have investigated the impact of sociality on disease risk in primates, only a handful hav...
Article
Full-text available
Determining the impact that the KIAA0319 gene has on primate brain morphology can provide insight into the evolution of human cognition and language systems. Here, we tested whether polymorphisms in KIAA0319 in chimpanzees account for gray matter volumetric variation in brain regions implicated in language and communication (particularly within the...
Article
Full-text available
Quantifying variation in behaviour-related genes provides insight into the evolutionary potential of repeatable among-individual variation in behaviour (i.e. personality). Yet, individuals typically also plastically adjust their behaviour in response to environmental conditions and/or age, thereby complicating the detection of genotype-phenotype as...
Article
Full-text available
Cortisol is often measured as a marker for stress. Therefore, a profound validation of the time-lag between the stressor and the increase and peak in cortisol levels is needed. No study measured both the urinary and salivary cortisol time-lag after a psychological stressor. In this study, we used a frequent sampling study design to (1) describe the...
Article
Humans have unique cognitive capacities that, compared with apes, are not only simply expressed as a higher level of general intelligence, but also as a quantitative difference in sociocognitive skills. Humans' closest living relatives, bonobos (Pan paniscus), and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), show key between-species differences in social cogniti...
Article
Abnormal behaviours are often used as a welfare indicator in zoo-housed great apes. While previous studies report on the occurrence of abnormal behaviours in zoo-housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), there is currently a lack of knowledge about such behaviours in the closely related bonobo (Pan paniscus). Here we report on the prevalence, diversity...
Article
Full-text available
Methylation levels have been shown to change with age at sites across the human genome. Change at some of these sites is so consistent across individuals that it can be used as an ‘epigenetic clock’ to predict an individual's chronological age to within a few years. Here, we examined how the pattern of epigenetic ageing in chimpanzees compares with...
Article
To understand the nature and evolutionary origins of culture — one of humans’ most influential characteristics — a comparative approach is required. One line of scientific inquiry has focused on whether culture might be uniquely human, or shared with other animals, including humans’ closest living relatives: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobo...
Article
Full-text available
In bonobos, strong bonds have been documented between unrelated females and between mothers and their adult sons, which can have important fitness benefits. Often age, sex or kinship similarity have been used to explain social bond strength variation. Recent studies in other species also stress the importance of personality, but this relationship r...
Article
Full-text available
Facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) is associated with social dominance in human and non-human primates, which may reflect the effects of testosterone on facial morphology and behaviour. Given that testosterone facilitates status-seeking motivation, the association between fWHR and behaviour should be contingent on the relative costs and benefits o...
Article
Studying genetic mechanisms underlying primate brain morphology can provide insight into the evolution of human brain structure and cognition. In humans, loss‐of‐function mutations in the gene coding for ASPM (Abnormal Spindle Microtubule Assembly) have been associated with primary microcephaly, which is defined by a significantly reduced brain vol...
Article
Serotonin is a neurotransmitter that plays an important role in regulating behavior and personality in humans and other mammals. Polymorphisms in genes coding for the serotonin receptor subtype 1A (HTR1A), the serotonin transporter (SLC6A4), and the serotonin degrading enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) are associated with anxiety, impulsivity, and...
Article
Full-text available
Despite being closely related, bonobos and chimpanzees exhibit several behavioral differences. For instance, studies indicate that chimpanzees are more aggressive, territorial, and risk-taking, while bonobos exhibit greater social tolerance and higher rates of socio-sexual interactions. To elucidate the potential neuroanatomical variation that acco...
Article
Given their close genetic relatedness to humans, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) offer an essential comparative framework for studying the evolution of uniquely human traits. These two species differ markedly in their socio-behavioral repertoires, which is reflected in neuroanatomical differences that have been reported in...
Article
Although many studies stress the distinctive aspects of human language abilities, others argue that its foundations stem from a complex reconfiguration of ancestral systems that are shared with other species. This homology is crucial for identifying the genetic basis of human language, as much of the current research focuses on the recent evolution...
Article
Full-text available
The gene coding for the forkhead box protein P2 (FOXP2) is associated with human language disorders. Evolutionary changes in this gene are hypothesized to have contributed to the emergence of speech and language in the human lineage. Although FOXP2 is highly conserved across most mammals, humans differ at two functional amino acid substitutions fro...
Chapter
The study described in this chapter examines whether individual differences in six rating-based bonobo personality dimensions—Assertiveness, Conscientiousness, Openness, Attentiveness, Agreeableness and Extraversion—are related to sex, age, behaviors and dominance. To these ends, the study tested predictions based on previous studies of human and c...
Article
Full-text available
Despite being closely related, bonobos and chimpanzees show remarkable behavioral differences, the proximate origins of which remain unknown. This study examined the link between behavioral variation and variation in the vasopressin 1a receptor gene (Avpr1a) in bonobos. Chimpanzees are polymorphic for a ~360 bp deletion (DupB), which includes a mic...
Article
Full-text available
L'être humain possède des traits de personnalité uniques, qui demeurent relativement stables et permettent de prédire son comportement jusqu'à un certain point. Mais nous ne sommes pas les seuls êtres vivants dotés de ces caractéristiques. Les scientifiques s'accordent à dire qu'une grande variété d'espèces animales possèdent aussi, à l'instar des...
Article
Full-text available
Mensen beschikken over unieke persoonlijkheden, die relatief stabiel zijn en ervoor zorgen dat ons gedrag tot op een bepaalde hoogte voorspelbaar is. We zijn echter niet de enige levende wezens met deze kenmerken. Wetenschappers zijn er het in‑ middels over eens dat een grote variatie aan diersoorten, net zoals mensen, ook over individuele persoonl...
Chapter
Full-text available
We use Principal Component Analyses (PCA) to describe components of social relationship quality in bonobos. We find a three component structure, with the first two components, labelled Value and Compatibility, closely matching the theoretical constructs as well as components reported for chimpanzees and other primates. The third component differed...
Article
The importance of genes in regulating phenotypic variation of personality traits in humans and animals is becoming increasingly apparent in recent studies. Here we focus on variation in the vasopressin receptor gene 1a (Avpr1a) and oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and their effects on social personality traits in chimpanzees. We combine newly availabl...
Article
Full-text available
To better understand human and chimpanzee personality evolution, we obtained trait ratings of personality for 154 captive bonobos (~80% of the U.S. and European population). We found factors that we labeled Assertiveness, Conscientiousness, Openness, Agreeableness, Attentiveness, and Extraversion. The interrater reliabilities and test-retest reliab...
Article
We use Principal Component Analyses (PCA) to describe components of social relationship quality in bonobos. We find a three component structure, with the first two components, labelled Value and Compatibility, closely matching the theoretical constructs as well as components reported for chimpanzees and other primates. The third component differed...
Article
Full-text available
Recent literature has revealed the importance of variation in neuropeptide receptor gene sequences in the regulation of behavioral phenotypic variation. Here we focus on polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and vasopressin receptor gene 1a (Avpr1a) in chimpanzees and bonobos. In humans, a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the th...

Questions

Question (1)
Question
I am running MCMCglmm models to determine the heritability of several personality traits, but I am now searching for how to determine whether the heritability values are significant. Is there a way to determine p-values of the estimate? And how is this done? Do you compare the null model to the best model?
Also, I am correcting for a few random and fixed factors in the model (sex, age, a genetic marker and group) and do model selection based on lowest DIC values. Is this sufficient?

Network

Cited By