About
19
Publications
5,115
Reads
How we measure 'reads'
A 'read' is counted each time someone views a publication summary (such as the title, abstract, and list of authors), clicks on a figure, or views or downloads the full-text. Learn more
208
Citations
Introduction
I completed my PhD in children's capacity to learn from social robots at the University of Queensland. I work at the intersection of several disciplines, however primarily focus on early cognitive development and how robots and screens shape this trajectory. I employ methodologies from psychology, cognitive science, robotics and design.
I am also an avid science communicator and post daily on my social media pages reaching over 3 million viewers a month on TikTok and Instagram.
Current institution
Publications
Publications (19)
Autistic people are at elevated risk of mental health conditions and experience significant barriers to effective support. While adaptations to therapy for autistic people have been proposed by clinicians, there is limited research on how helpful autistic people themselves rate these. We aimed to address this gap. Participants were 130 autistic adu...
Background. Autistic people are at elevated risk of mental health conditions and experience significant barriers to receiving effective support. While adaptations to therapy for autistic people have been proposed by clinicians, there is limited research on how helpful autistic people themselves feel these would be. Our aim was to address this gap b...
The "video deficit" is a well-documented effect whereby children learn less well about information delivered via a screen than the same information delivered in person. Research suggests that increasing social contingency may ameliorate this video deficit. The current study instantiated social contingency to screen-based information by embodying th...
The video deficit is a well-documented effect whereby children learn less well about information delivered via a screen than the same information delivered in person. Research suggests that increasing social contingency may ameliorate this ‘video deficit’ (Nielsen et al., 2008). The current study instantiated social contingency to screen-based info...
Robots are an increasingly prevalent presence in children’s lives. However, little is known about the ways in which children learn from robots and whether they do so in the same way as they learn from humans. To investigate this, we adapted a previously established imitation paradigm centered on inefficient tool use. Children (3- to 6-year-olds; N...
Traditionally, infants have learned how to interact with objects in their environment through direct observations of adults and peers. In recent decades these models have been available over different media, and this has introduced non-human agents to infants’ learning environments. Humanoid robots are increasingly portrayed as social agents in on...
Developmental research typically relies on face-to-face testing at laboratories, childcare centers, museums or playgroups. Current social distancing measures have led to a halt in research. Although face-to-face interaction is considered essential for research involving young children, current technology provides viable alternatives. This paper int...
Commensurate with constant technological advances, social robots are increasingly anticipated to enter homes and classrooms; however , little is known about the efficacy of social robots as teaching tools. To investigate children's learning from robots, 1-to 3-year-olds observed either a human or a robot demonstrate two goal-directed object manipul...
Past research has indicated that young children have a propensity to adopt the causally unnecessary actions of an adult, a phenomenon known as overimitation. Among competing perspectives, social accounts suggest that overimitation satisfies social motivations, be they affiliative or normative, whereas the "copy-all/refine-later" account proposes th...
This study examined children’s moral concern for robots relative to living and nonliving entities. Children (4–10 years of age, N = 126) watched videos of six different entities having a box placed over them that was subsequently struck by a human hand. Children were subsequently asked to rate the moral worth of each agent relating to physical harm...
The success of robotic agents in close proximity of humans depends on their capacity to engage in social interactions and maintain these interactions over periods of time that are suitable for learning. A critical requirement is the ability to modify the behavior of the robot contingently to the attentional and social cues signaled by the human. A...
This paper presents an analysis of children's interactions with an early prototype of a robot that is being designed for deployment in early learning centres. 23 children aged 2-6 interacted with the prototype, consisting of a pair of tablets embedded in a flat and vaguely humanoid form. We used a Wizard of Oz (WoZ) technique to control a synthesiz...
Questions
Questions (3)
Looking for some advice on the use of eye-tracking glasses on toddlers (18 months+). We are interested in the TOBII glasses but are also open to recommendations if experienced users have a more kid friendly product in mind. We're interested in testing in rich environments (science museums, labs, classrooms) and for use in human robot interaction and imitation tasks (amongst other potential uses).
Any advice on whether commercially available eye trackers can deal with little heads would be very appreciated!
Thanks!
Does anyone know of a validated survey on prior experience for animals, objects and (possibly even) robots?
We are looking to control for prior experiences on how children respond to stimuli involving varied agents. We would like to know whether children have exposure to different types of agents in their lifetime, and whether the exposure has a positive, negative or neutral valence.
We are looking brief survey which can be answered by parents (although a companion survey for the child might help too!). Anything that involves animals, robots or other agents (possibly strangers of the human persuasion) would be amazing!
Possibly quite a naive question, but the answer doesn't appear easy to find when wondering why only some children are terrified of robots and others are not. It seems more than just individual difference, with significant numbers of kids responding with terror to robots (at least 1/10, sometimes higher).
Does anyone have any resources on children who are terrified of any type of robot (humanoid or not, e.g., NAO or Roomba)? Or a suggestion for any measures which might capture this?