The Psychological Record

Published by Springer Nature

Online ISSN: 2163-3452

·

Print ISSN: 0033-2933

Articles


Fig. 1 Probability discounting values (h). Mean h estimates obtained by male (white bars) and female gamblers (shaded bars), presenting with and without antisocial personality disorder (ASPD). Error bars represent standard errors. The asterisk refers to p<0.05. For this figure, h-values were reverted to their natural scale in order to ease interpretation  
Fig. 2 Delay discounting values (k). Mean k estimates obtained by male (white bars) and female gamblers (shaded bars), presenting with and without antisocial personality disorder (ASPD). Error bars represent standard errors. For this figure, k-values were reverted to their natural scale to ease interpretation  
Associations Between Antisocial Personality Disorder and Sex on Discounting Rates
  • Article
  • Full-text available

December 2014

·

233 Reads

·

·

Numerous studies show that individuals with substance use and gambling problems discount delayed and probabilistic outcomes at different rates than controls. Few studies, however, investigated the association of discounting with antisocial personality disorders (ASPD), and none evaluated whether sex impacts these relationships. Because females with ASPD exhibit different patterns of antisocial behavior than their male counterparts, they may also differ in their decision-making tendencies. This study examined the effects of ASPD and sex on discounting in pathological gamblers. Results revealed effects of ASPD, and an interaction between ASPD and sex, on probability discounting rates. None of these variables, however, were related to delay discounting. Females with ASPD highly preferred probabilistic outcomes, suggesting that female gamblers with ASPD are particularly impulsive when it comes to probabilistic rewards. Greater understanding of sex differences in ASPD might help guide the selection of more effective sex-specific prevention and treatment programs.
Download
Share


Figure 1. 
Figure 2. 
Figure 3. 
Figure 4. 
Figure 5. 
A Novel Method for Teaching the First Instances of Simple Discrimination to Nonverbal Children with Autism in a Laboratory Environment

February 2008

·

42 Reads

A novel method for initiating discrimination training with nonverbal children combines a delayed S+ procedure that requires children to refrain from responding to either of 2 physically different choice stimuli until a prompt stimulus is added onto 1 of the choices, and a delayed prompting procedure that presents the same 2-choice stimulus display, but stimuli are initially added onto both choices. After a short delay, the added stimulus on the S- is removed, and the choice of the S+ is thus prompted. If the children learn to observe and respond to the defining features of the S+ choice stimulus, then they may respond to the S+ prior to the added-stimulus removal. Implementation was successful with 8 nonverbal children who had not previously exhibited simple simultaneous discrimination, suggesting a useful methodology for initiating discrimination training with populations for whom verbal instruction is ineffective.

Figure 1. The stimuli used in Experiment 1. S+ and S− indicate the function of each stimulus in the first discrimination (prior to reversal training).  
Figure 2. Portions of the automated teaching lab used in Experiment 2. The top panel shows the stimulus display in the teaching area, and the bottom panel shows the same view from the experimenter area.  
Reversal Learning Set and Functional Equivalence in Children with and Without Autism

December 2008

·

199 Reads

·

·

·

[...]

·

To evaluate whether children with and without autism could exhibit (a) functional equivalence in the course of yoked repeated-reversal training and (b) reversal learning set, 6 children, in each of two experiments, were exposed to simple discrimination contingencies with three sets of stimuli. The discriminative functions of the set members were yoked and repeatedly reversed. In Experiment 1, all the children (of preschool age) showed gains in the efficiency of reversal learning across reversal problems and behavior that suggested formation of functional equivalence. In Experiment 2, 3 nonverbal children with autism exhibited strong evidence of reversal learning set and 2 showed evidence of functional equivalence. The data suggest a possible relationship between efficiency of reversal learning and functional equivalence test outcomes. Procedural variables may prove important in assessing the potential of young or nonverbal children to classify stimuli on the basis of shared discriminative functions.

Perhaps More Consideration of Pavlovian-Operant Interaction May Improve the Clinical Efficacy of Behaviorally Based Drug Treatment Programs

September 2013

·

132 Reads

Drug abuse remains costly. Drug-related cues can evoke cue-reactivity and craving, contributing to relapse. The Pavlovian extinction-based cue-exposure therapy (CET) has not been very successful in treating drug abuse. A functional operant analysis of complex rituals involved in CET is outlined and reinterpreted as an operant heterogeneous chain maintained by observing responses, conditioned reinforcers, and discriminative stimuli. It is further noted that operant functions are not predicated on Pavlovian processes but can be influenced by them in contributing to relapse; several empirical studies from the animal and human literature highlight this view. Cue-reactivity evoked by Pavlovian processes is conceptualized as an operant establishing/motivating operation. CET may be more effective in incorporating an operant-based approach that takes into account the complexity of Pavlovian-operant interaction. Extinction of the operant chain coupled with the shaping of alternative behaviors is proposed as an integrated therapy. It is proposed that operant-based drug abuse treatments (contingency management, voucher programs, and the therapeutic work environment) might consider incorporating cue-reactivity, as establishing/motivating operations, to increase long-term success-a hybrid approach based on Pavlovian-operant interaction.

Figure 1. Box plots of AuC values for the BED, obese, and control groups on the delay discounting tasks. Each panel presents the data for a different type of delayed reward. The bottom and top of each box represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively, and the horizontal line within each box represents the 50th percentile (group median). The vertical lines extending from the boxes represent the minimum and maximum values that are not outliers, and outliers (i.e., values above the 90th percentile or below the 10th percentile) are represented by the solid circles.
Figure 2. Relative subjective value as a function of delay for the BED, obese, and control groups. Each panel presents the data for a different type of reward.  
Figure 4. Relative subjective value as a function of odds against the receipt of the reward for the BED, obese, and control groups. Each panel presents the data for a different type of reward.
Figure 3. Box plots of AuC values for the BED, obese, and control groups on the probability discounting tasks. Each panel presents the data for a different type of probabilistic reward. As in Figure 1, the bottom and top of each box represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the horizontal line within each box represents the group median. The vertical lines extending from the boxes represent the minimum and maximum values that are not outliers, and the solid circles represent outliers (i.e., values above the 90th percentile or below the 10th percentile).  
Discounting of Various Types of Rewards by Women with and Without Binge Eating Disorder: Evidence for General Rather Than Specific Differences

September 2011

·

173 Reads

The present study compared the extent to which obese women with binge eating disorder (BED), obese women without BED, and controls discounted delayed and probabilistic money and directly consumable rewards: food, massage time, and preferred sedentary activity. Of special interest was whether the BED group differed from the other groups in terms of their discounting of all three types of directly consumable rewards or only in their discounting of food. Overall, the BED group tended to discount both delayed and probabilistic rewards of all types more steeply than the obese and control groups. Thus, rather than finding differences specific to particular types of rewards, we find that women with BED are generally more impatient when choices involve delayed rewards and more risk averse when they involve probabilistic rewards. These results suggest a temperamental difference associated with BED that cannot be accounted for by the concomitant obesity.

Exclusion Learning and Emergent Symbolic Category Formation in Individuals With Severe Language Impairments and Intellectual Disabilities

April 2009

·

269 Reads

We evaluated formation of simple symbolic categories from initial learning of specific dictated word-picture relations through emergence of untaught or derived relations. Participants were 10 individuals with severe intellectual and language limitations. Three experimental categories were constructed, each containing 1 spoken word (Set A), 1 photograph (Set B), and 1 visual-graphic "lexigram" (Set C). Exclusion-based learning procedures were used to teach first the 3 auditory-visual relations (A-B relations) and then the 3 visual-visual relations (B-C relations) for each category. Seven participants acquired these initial relations. The untaught relations C-B and A-C were then assessed to evaluate the emergence of symbolic categories. Participants demonstrated virtually error-free performances on C-B and A-C derived relations. The study helps to define operationally a highly useful procedural path for systematic instruction in symbolic functioning for persons with intellectual and language disabilities associated with autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders.

Table 1 Description of the Stimuli Used in the Study Stimuli Description/characteristics 
Conditional Discriminations by Preverbal Children in an Identity Matching-to-Sample Task

June 2011

·

55 Reads

This study sought to develop methodology for assessing whether children aged 16-21 months could learn to match stimuli on the basis of physical identity in conditional discrimination procedures of the type routinely used in stimulus equivalence research with older participants. The study was conducted in a private room at a daycare center for children and toddlers. The child and the research sat together on the floor facing an apparatus with two windows. Stimuli to be discriminated were toys especially designed to attract the child's attention and maintain continued interest. On simple discrimination and discrimination reversal trials that were programmed in initial training, S+ and S- toys were displayed within the two windows. When the child touched the window containing the toy defined as S+ on a given trial, s/he was allowed to manipulate/play with that toy. Selections of the S- toy ended the trial without a play opportunity. On subsequent identity matching-to-sample trials, the child was first allowed to manipulate a sample toy. Then, S+ (matching) and S- (nonmatching) comparison toys were displayed within the windows, and the selection consequences were the same as on simple discrimination trials. The study provides evidence that preverbal children can master simple and conditional discrimination performances via such procedures, perhaps setting the stage for subsequent studies aimed at establishing procedural control of the discrimination baselines needed to assess the stimulus equivalence potential of children in this age range.

Lana Chimpanzee Learns to Count by “Numath”: A Summary of a Videotaped Experimental Report

September 1989

·

35 Reads

This paper summarizes a videotaped presentation (Rumbaugh, Savage-Rumbaugh, Hopkins, Washburn, & Runfeldt, 1987) of computerized training programs whereby an adult female chimpanzee, Lana (Pan troglodytes), learned to use a joystick to remove from a screen the number of boxes appropriate to the value of a randomly selected Arabic numeral 1, 2, or 3. Initial training provided a variety of cues, both numeric and otherwise, to support correct performance. Across software programs, all cues other than numeric ones were deleted. In the final test, Lana was correct on over 80% of trials in which there was no residual feedback of intratrial events and where only her memory of those events could provide the cue to indicate that she had removed boxes in accordance with the value of the target numbers and should terminate the trial. The tape is narrated and consists of video recordings of Lana's performance on each software program.

The Effects of Reduced Cigarette Smoking on Discounting Future Rewards: An Initial Evaluation

March 2008

·

84 Reads

To determine whether reduction of smoking via contingency management in dependent smokers would decrease the discounting of delayed reinforcers compared with smokers who did not reduce their smoking, moderate to heavy cigarette smokers were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: a contingency management condition and a control condition. In three phases (baseline discounting determination for hypothetical money and cigarettes, implementation of a contingency management or control condition, and postintervention discounting determination), the procedure to reinforce reduction in cigarette smoking produced CO decreases in all subjects exposed to that procedure. Discounting decreased significantly for both cigarettes and money among the group for whom smoking reductions were reinforced, whereas the control group showed no significant change for either commodity. Reductions in smoking can lead to reductions in discounting, and increased discounting in current smokers may be a reversible effect of nicotine dependence.

Membership of Defined Responses in Stimulus Classes

September 2013

·

21 Reads

Sidman (2000) has suggested that in addition to conditional and discriminative stimuli, class-consistent defined responses can also become part of an equivalence class. In the current study, this assertion was tested using a mixed-schedule procedure that allowed defined response patterns to be "presented" as samples in the absence of different occasioning stimuli. Four typically developing adults were first trained to make distinct response topographies to two visual color stimuli, and then were taught to match those color stimuli to two different form-sample stimuli in a matching task. Three separate tests were given in order to determine whether training had established two classes each comprised of a response, a color, and a form: a form-response test in which the forms were presented to test if the participants would make differential responses to them; and two response-matching tests to test if the participants would match visual stimulus comparisons to response-pattern samples. Three of the four participants showed class-consistent responding in the tests, although some participants needed additional training prior to passing the tests. In general, the data indicated that the different response patterns had entered into a class with the visual stimuli. These results add to a growing literature on the role of class-consistent responding in stimulus class formation, and provide support for the notion that differential responses themselves can become a part of an equivalence class.

Figure 1.
Figure 2.
Intertemporal Decision-Making for a Group

September 2010

·

57 Reads

Temporal discounting assessments measure the reduction in the subjective value of a reward as a function of the delay to that reward, and are correlated with behavior in social dilemma. Among the solutions proposed for defection in social dilemmas is a single individual making the decisions for the group. The present study examined the influence of group context on temporal discounting. Participants completed temporal discounting procedures when the outcomes affected only the individual and when outcomes affected a group of 10, including the individual. Though no overall difference was observed between the individual and group conditions, sex was found to be a moderating variable: Males discounted significantly more when discounting for the individual, but females discounted significantly more when discounting for the group. These results indicate that sex is an important variable when making intertemporal decisions for a group.

The Effects of Fixed-Ratio Size in Multiple and Mixed Fixed-Ratio Schedules

February 1971

·

23 Reads

In compound fixed-ratio schedules, the factors controlling intercomponent pausing are not clear. One group of pigeons was trained on a multiple schedule in which ratio size of one of the two components was varied. Comparable manipulations were made for a second group trained on a mixed schedule. As the ratio was increased, the pause preceding that ratio also increased, while the pause before the other ratio decreased. Stimulus control was greater in the multiple schedule.

Jean Henri Masers De Latude (1725–1805): an Unappreciated Practical Psychologist

April 1972

·

6 Reads

Presents 2 detailed descriptions of behavioral events from the memoires of J. Latude. The descriptions are analyzed in terms of their correspondence to modern psychological concepts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Human or Machine: A Subjective Comparison of Piet Mondrian’s “Composition with Lines” (1917) and a Computer-Generated Picture

January 1966

·

54 Reads

A digital computer and microfilm plotter were used to produce a semirandom picture similar in composition to Piet Mondrian's painting "Composition With Lines." Reproductions of both pictures were then presented to 100 Ss whose tasks were to identify the computer picture and to indicate which picture they preferred. Only 28% of the Ss were able to correctly identify the computer-generated picture, while 59% of the Ss preferred the computer-generated picture. Both percentages were statistically different (0.05 level) from selections based upon chance according to a binomial test. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

When historians disagree: B. F. Skinner and E. G. Boring, 1930.

July 1985

·

39 Reads

Reviews the disagreement between B. F. Skinner and E. G. Boring over the historical portions of a manuscript that Skinner wished to present as his doctoral dissertation, which was later published as "The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior" (see record 1932-00184-001). The sources of their disagreement are examined in relation to their contrasting uses of history, the meaning of the concept of reflex, the contemporary debate over behaviorism, conflicting metaphysical commitments, and the role of the institutional setting in the debate. (43 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Major Trends in Interbehavioral Psychology from Articles Published in The Psychological Record (1937–1983)

October 1984

·

12 Reads

Examined major trends in interbehavioral psychology through articles published in the 33 volumes of The Psychological Record between 1937 and 1983. Results suggest that interbehaviorism evolved over a period of history in theoretical and experimental disciplines of science. Comparisons between interbehavioral and noninterbehavioral articles reveal that interdisciplinary philosophy (historical and synthetical) was present in more volumes than experimental interbehavioral research. Comparisons between experimental and theoretical articles in interbehaviorism indicate that experimental articles concerned stimulus and response functions, multiple causation of behavior within a systems approach, reactional biography, and culturalization. Theory articles attempted to clarify technical, philosophical, or socially controversial points regarding naturalism and traditional branches of psychology. Examination of articles from The Psychological Record and articles and books published elsewhere during the last 10 yrs indicates that systems analysis and clinical treatment abound in the research potential of interbehaviorism. Annotations of the experimental and theoretical articles with interbehavioral content are provided. (7½ p ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Avian Open-Field Research and Related Effects of Environmental Novelty: An Annotated Bibliography, 1960–1988

July 1989

·

15 Reads

Presents an annotated bibliography of 174 articles on avian open-field behavior (OFB) research. The bibliography includes reports on (1) descriptions of OFB; (2) OFB as an index of fear; (3) the causality and consequences of certain OFB responses and their relationship with other behavioral, physiological, or production characteristics; and (4) the potential influence of environmental novelty in studies of other behavioral constructs (e.g., aggression, spontaneous alternation, imprinting). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Scientific creed, 1961: Philosophical credo. Abductory principles. The centrality of self.

January 1961

·

44 Reads

The 3 papers begin with "an affirmation of faith in conclusions, generalizations, laws and invariances." Then there is an advocacy of abduction as a guiding principle of scientific endeavor; "the emphasis is on the discovery of hypotheses, not their deduction from postulates." Finally, an argument is developed for conducting scientific enquiry on single cases rather than being dominated by sampling methodology. Briefly, the author contends that the study of the single case "makes science possible in the place of scientism." He concludes with a plea for the "end of the nineteenth century 'error' dogma." From Psyc Abstracts 36:02:2AD01S. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Relationships Between Scores on the General Purpose Abbreviated Battery of Stanford-Binet IV, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised, Columbia Mental Maturity Scale, and Goodenough-Harris Drawing Test

January 1987

·

19 Reads

This study investigated the correlations among the “General Purpose Abbreviated Battery” of the Stanford-Binet IV: Fourth Edition, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised, the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale, and the Goodenough-Harris Drawing Test. A third-grade class of 23 children (11 boys, 12 girls) took all four of the tests, and the intercorrelations of the total scores of the four tests were calculated. The Binet IV and Peabody correlation was statistically significant (p < .01) as was the correlation between Binet IV and Columbia (p < .05). The correlation between Binet IV and Goodenough-Harris was not statistically significant (p > .05).

Extinction of an Instrumental Running Response in Rats in the Absence of Frustration and Nonreinforcement

January 1973

·

5 Reads

Trained 4 groups of 10 female albino Sprague-Dawley rats to run a straight alley for food reward. During Phase I reward magnitude was either 1 or 10 food pellets, and 1/2 of the Ss were food-deprived. During this phase only the deprived Ss exhibited increasing response speed. During Phase II all Ss were tested under "food satiation" with 10 pellets as reward. Those Ss originally trained while food-deprived initially ran faster in Phase II, but with continuing trials their speed declined toward that of nondeprived Ss. Results are interpreted in terms of the theories of K. Spence and A. Amsel, which imply that the present procedure should lead to the extinction of incentive motivation in the absence of frustration. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Odd Ball, Abstract, Olfactory Learning in Laboratory Rats

October 1972

·

12 Reads

Investigated the ability of male Long-Evans rats to master oddity problems using olfaction as a modality. It was demonstrated, with 8 different olfactory stimuli, that experimental Ss (n = 11) could learn the abstract relation of oddity while controls (n = 12) could not. 69% correct responses were achieved on the 1st trial of a problem and 93% correct on the 2nd trial after 29 problems. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

University student apathy: Sex, race, and academic class variables.

January 1985

·

28 Reads

Examined student apathy among 112 female and 88 male Black undergraduates using the Purpose in Life Test. Comparisons of data with those of a heterogeneous university sample by K. E. Coffield (see record 1982-02060-001) showed that both groups had similar apathy levels. In applying sex–race dichotomies, however, Black males showed significantly less apathy than Black females or heterogeneous university males. Black juniors and incoming heterogeneous freshmen displayed greater apathy behaviors than did other academic classes. Potential explanations are offered for the differential behavior noted. (7 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Analysis of Cheating on Academic Assignments

October 1988

·

68 Reads

Instances of cheating on study guide assignments were observed for 245 college students. Mean cheating rate was 50.8% (i.e., the typical student cheated on about half the questions). Cheating tended to increase across the semester and was associated with lower grades on exams. The tendency to cheat varied across the semester, suggesting that transient setting factors were major determinants of cheating on assignments. Admission of cheating was increased by reinforcement, but this increase did not result in a change in rates of cheating. A positive correlation was found between cheating and admission of cheating, except when there were penalties for admissions. Neither an honor pledge nor values counseling diminished cheating. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Personality Factors in Acceptance of Loss among the Physically Disabled

January 1985

·

6 Reads

330 physically disabled adults (mean age 38.5 yrs), undergoing the 1st wk of a course of industrial rehabilitation, were administered an acceptance of loss scale (ALS), the Raven Progressive Matrices (RPM), and the Maudsley Personality Inventory (MPI). On completing 5 wks, Ss were readministered the ALS. The difference between Ss' 1st and 2nd scores was used to determine gain or loss in ALS, and each S's mean score was calculated. Ss were grouped in terms of the nature of their disability into visible, hidden, or sensory/mental and then were classified on the basis of their ALS mean score into good, average, and poor accepters of loss. The mean score of the 3 groups on the MPI and RPM were compared within the 3 disability categories. ANOVA revealed personality factors as the only significant source of variance; the nature of disability was not significant. The more intelligent, extraverted, and less neurotic Ss were more likely to accept the disability. Mean gain in ALS was highest among the poor accepters of loss, showing the industrial rehabilitation program's beneficial effect on adjustment. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

The effects of differential and nondifferential outcomes on response rates and accuracy under a delayed-matching-to-sample procedure

January 1991

·

178 Reads

Four White Carneaux pigeons were exposed to a delayed-matching-to-sample procedure in which food or a flash of the feeder light followed correct responses. When these consequences were correlated with a particular sample stimulus (e.g., food followed matching responses to red and a flash of the feeder light followed matching responses to green), accuracy was significantly higher than when consequences were not correlated with sample stimuli. The sample stimulus correlated with food engendered a much higher rate of responding than the stimulus correlated with the light flash. The difference in response rates during the 2 types of trials may have acquired discriminative properties that enhanced accuracy in the correlated condition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Overconfidence in a Multiple-Choice Test and its Relationship to Achievement

October 1992

·

148 Reads

43 university students completed a multiple-choice test consisting of 32 questions with 4 alternative answers each. Ss' confidence in the correctness of their chosen responses was assessed by their self-rating on a scale of 0 to 100 the degree of confidence for the answer chosen. Ss' scores were the number of correct answers and the average confidence level across questions. Overconfident Ss received significantly lower grades as compared with Ss who were not overconfident. The cognitive mechanisms that may be responsible for the influence of overconfidence on performance in multiple-choice tests are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

The Relationship between Achievement Motivation and Ethnicity in Anglo-American and Mexican-American Junior High School Students

April 1967

·

6 Reads

IN AN EFFORT TO DETERMINE THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION AND ETHNIC GROUP MEMBERSHIP IN JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS, 2 GROUPS, 69 SS IN EACH, WERE MATCHED FOR INTELLIGENCE. 1 GROUP WAS COMPOSED OF MEXICAN-AMERICANS, AND THE OTHER OF ANGLO-AMERICANS. AFTER GIVING BOTH GROUPS NEED ACHIEVEMENT TESTS, IT WAS FOUND THAT, WITH THESE SAMPLES MATCHED FOR INTELLIGENCE, THERE WAS NO STATISTICALLY SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCE BETWEEN THE MEAN NEED TO ACHIEVE OF ANGLO-AMERICAN AND MEXICAN-AMERICAN 8TH-GRADE STUDENTS. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Acoustical determinants of the "neurotic pattern" in rats.

4 Reads

An auditory stimulus has always been present in any experiments to date dealing with neurotic pattern. Experiments are reported by the writers to show that such a behavior pattern as described by Maier depends upon the intensity and frequency of auditory stimulation. When intensity is controlled, the most effective frequencies producing seizures are supersonic. Either acoustic conditions in experiments dealing with conflict must be better controlled, or the concept itself must be reinterpreted. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Effects of Training Method and Word Order on Adults’ Acquisition of Miniature Linguistic Systems

January 1987

·

5 Reads

Conducted 3 experiments to evaluate the effects of training method, English vs non-English syntax, and number of training items on the learning of 2-word miniature linguistic systems. 100 university students were exposed to nonoverlap (diagonal) or overlap (edgewise or stepwise) training conditions in 3 experiments. There were no significant differences in the initial rate of learning among the 3 conditions. Overlap training, however, provided more extensive generalization to untrained items than did nonoverlap training. An analysis of errors revealed that object words were produced correctly more often than modifiers, regardless of syntax and training condition. It is suggested that careful selection of training items and frequent monitoring of generalization can enhance the efficiency of intervention programs focusing on lexical and syntactic development. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Effects of the Acquisition of Prerequisite Behavior on the Learning of Nonvocal Verbal Behavior and Vocal Imitation in Children With Severe Retardation

July 1986

·

9 Reads

Investigated the relationship between prerequisite behaviors and the 1st steps of a verbal acquisition program, using 4 retarded nonverbal children (aged 6–9 yrs). Ss were selected on the basis of low level or absence of target behaviors (nonvocal verbal behavior and vocal imitation) and failure to reach the acquisition criterion on prerequisite behavior. The effects of acquisition and maintenance of prerequisite interactions, such as attending behavior, nonverbal imitative behavior, and disruptive behavior, on the learning of nonvocal verbal behavior and vocal imitation were evaluated. Results show that the acquisition of prerequisite interactions led to superior performance and fewer trials to reach criteria in this language intervention. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Acquisition and Extinction of the Conditioned Avoidance Response: a Comparison Between Male Rats and Estrus and Non-Estrus Female Rats

April 1972

·

5 Reads

Compared 9 male Long-Evans hooded rats with 18 nonestrus females in acquisition and extinction of an avoidance response. No differential extinction rates were found, although terminal performance differences were present in acquisition. Females, which during acquisition were not in estrus, were divided into estrus and nonestrus groups in extinction. No differences were significant during early trials, but both performance and rate changes were noted thereafter, the nonestrus Ss being superior to estrus Ss on both measures. Results are discussed in terms of the influence of the estrus variable on male-female comparative studies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Self-punitive behavior: Effects of percentage of shocked acquisition trials and percentage of goal-shocked extinction trials.

January 1985

·

1 Read

Trained 48 female Sprague-Dawley rats to escape shock in a runway, with shock present on each acquisition trial for half the Ss and on 50% of trials for the other Ss. During extinction, Ss were punished in the goalbox on 0, 10, 50, or 100% of the trials. Results indicate little effect of percentage of shocked acquisition trials on either acquisition or extinction performance. Percentage of punished extinction trials did affect extinction performance, with suppression of response measures (relative to nonpunished controls) found in Ss punished on 50 or 100% of the extinction trials and facilitation for Ss punished on 10% of the trials. Results are inconsistent with either 2-factor or discrimination theories of self-punitive behavior and consistent with a stimulus-directive interpretation of this phenomenon. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Acquisition of a simple running response as a function of partial and continuous schedules of reinforcement

January 1959

·

4 Reads

Rates of acquisition of a simple running response are compared under 100% and 50% schedules of reinforcement. 46 male hooded rats were given 90 trials in a block runway during 15 consecutive days. The Ss were divided equally into a 100% reinforcement group and a random 50% reinforcement group. Analyses of both evocation speed and running speed indicated the 100% group responded more quickly during the first 24 trials. Later the curves converged and during the final trials the 50% group significantly exceeded the 100% group. An interpretation in terms of an anticipatory frustration response concept was discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

The distribution of muscular action potentials during imaging

January 1938

·

34 Reads

An increase in muscular potentials is reported in all of the muscle groups tested during the imaging of such tasks as squeezing a hand dynamometer, typing, singing, and playing a wind instrument. "There is no good evidence of localization to the muscle groups commonly thought to be involved in such performances. While such action potentials seem to be necessarily concomitant, as shown by the report of the unsuccessful subjects and the control groups, they are not localized in any particular part of the body, nor are they exclusively peculiar to imaging, since other workers have shown that action potentials accompany other implicit activities. The distribution of these potentials seems to indicate that during the revival of vestigial responses one can expect to be present any muscular activity that accompanied the original response." (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

A visual acuity test for infants under six months of age

October 1959

·

49 Reads

A visual preference test of visual acuity was administered to 21 infants ranging in age from 2 to 21 weeks. The test consisted of a graded series of striped patterns, 6-inch squares of vertical black and white stripes varying in width. The group of Ss under 2 months of age showed no significant differential preference for any size pattern. On the other hand, the infants between 2 and 5 months showed a preference for both the 1/8 in. and 1/16 in. stripes to the very fine pattern, 1/64 in. width stripes. The use of the preference test as a research tool to investigate pattern and spatial vision in infants 6 months of age or younger is discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Effects of Adaptation Level, Context and Face Validity on Responses to Self-Report Psychological Inventories

July 1968

·

4 Reads

INVESTIGATED THE ADAPTATION LEVEL OR CONTRAST AND CONTEXT EFFECTS AS CONCEIVED BY HELSON ON SELF-REPORT QUESTIONNAIRES USING THE ADDICTION RESEARCH CENTER INVENTORY, MMPI, AND CPI ON OVERLAPPING ITEMS. THE ABSENCE OF CONTRAST OR CONTEXT EFFECTS SUGGESTS THAT THE POINT OF REFERENCE OR DETERMINANT FOR A RESPONSE TO A SELF-REPORT OR PERSONALITY QUESTION FOR A GROUP IS DETERMINED BY THE ITEM ITSELF AND IS UNAFFECTED BY ADJACENT ITEMS OR BY ITEMS AS A WHOLE. SIGNIFICANT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES WERE FOUND FOR A CONTRAST TENDENCY, INDEXED BY SCORING REVERSALS OF RESPONSES TO ADJACENT ITEMS, IRRESPECTIVE OF THE SELECTION OF ADJACENT ITEMS, BUT THE GENERALITY OF THE REVERSAL TENDENCY, AS EVALUATED BY CORRELATIONS ACROSS TESTS, WAS A FUNCTION OF THE DEGREE OF DISSIMILARITY OF ADJACENT ITEMS. THE REVERSAL TENDENCY BASED ON A CHANGE OF RESPONSE TO DISSIMILAR ITEMS ACCOUNTS FOR AS MUCH VARIATION ACROSS TESTS AS SOCIAL DESIRABILITY OR RESPONSE SET. THESE TENDENCIES APPEAR TO BE OVERESTIMATED IN EACH TEST, SINCE THEY ACCOUNT FOR MUCH MORE VARIATION WITHIN THAN ACROSS TESTS. (19 REF.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Shock history and adaptation as parameters of elicited aggression in rats

January 1967

PAIRS OF RATS WITH A HISTORY OF SHOCKS RECEIVED INDIVIDUALLY IN SEPARATE CHAMBERS, WITHOUT OPPORTUNITY TO ENGAGE IN AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR, EXHIBIT THE SAME PROBABILITY OF FIGHTING IN RESPONSE TO SHOCK AT 71 DAYS OF AGE AS DO PAIRS OF RATS WITH AN EQUALLY LONG HISTORY OF SHOCK-ELICITED AGGRESSION. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Strike-induced chemosensory searching in elapid snakes (cobras, taipans, tiger snakes, and death adders) at San Diego Zoo.

54 Reads

Compared strike-induced and visually-induced chemosensory searching in 10 elapids. Ss were 5 tiger snakes ( Notechis scutatus), 2 taipans ( Oxyuranus scutellatus), 2 Indian cobras ( Naja naja), and 1 death adder ( Acanthophis antarcticus). Two members of a viperid species (rhinoceros vipers [ Bitis nasicornis]) were also observed. Results indicate that visual and other cues arising from rodents in the no-strike condition stimulated some degree of chemosensory searching in elapids but not in the rhinoceros vipers and that most of the elapids were capable of both styles of predation. It is concluded that long-term-captive elapids retain their ability to exhibit sustained interest in chemical information following the delivery of a predatory strike. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Adjunctive and Operant Bolt Pecking in the Pigeon

January 1973

·

18 Reads

Reinforced the key pecking of a male White Carneaux pigeon under fixed-ratio (FR) schedules of reinforcement. During postreinforcement pauses, the S was observed to peck on a bolt head in a hole in the panel. This behavior was recorded and appeared to be adjunctive. Bolt pecking probability was a function of FR size, was excessive in frequency, and occurred immediately after a reinforcement. When bolt pecking was made the operant response under FR schedules, performance was atypical of that usually seen under such schedules. Under requirements ranging from FR 85-650, postreinforcement pauses were absent while responding appeared at a steady rate from 1 reinforcement to the next. When reinforcement was presented independently of responding, rate of bolt pecking decreased and was confined to periods immediately following reinforcement. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Intermittent escape schedules employed in conjunction with a signaled adjusting avoidance schedule.

January 1983

·

5 Reads

Four male albino rats were trained on a continuous escape-adjusting avoidance schedule. When the S failed to avoid, continuous pulsed grid shock was presented. A leverpress terminated shock and produced a 10-sec avoidance component. Each additional response added another 10-sec to the shock-off avoidance period. The upper limit was 110 sec produced by 10 responses. One of 10 different auditory warning stimuli (WS) provided feedback as to where the S was in the avoidance interval. Later sessions provided an FI 5 sec, 10 sec, VR 5 sec, or fixed time 5-sec escape schedule. Each escape schedule maintained response rates comparable to those produced in appetitive studies. Data produced during the shock off intervals reveal that avoidance was improved by the intermittent escape schedules. Furthermore, each S shifted its distribution of avoidance responses to intervals and WS distal to shock when intermittent escape schedules were in effect. (40 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Relative reinforcing effects of pentobarbital solutions orally self-administered by rhesus monkeys under fixed-ratio and signaled DRL schedules

January 1991

·

3 Reads

For 3 (Exp 1) or 4 (Exp 2) rhesus monkeys, po pentobarbital (PB) solutions were concurrently available with water from separate drinking spouts during daily 3-hr sessions under FR (Exp 1) and signaled differential-reinforcement-of-low-rates (DRL; Exp 2) schedules. Results show a positive relationship between reinforcer magnitude (amoung of PB) and relative reinforcing effects. These findings were consistent across both the FR (time-based) and DRL (response-based) schedules. Behavior maintained by greater drug amounts persisted to a greater degree, relative to baseline, as access to drug was constrained by the imposition of progressively larger response requirements or longer time intervals. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

The Reactivity of Adult Retardates’ Self-Monitoring: A Comparison among Behaviors of Different Valences, and a Comparison with Token Reinforcement

April 1976

·

8 Reads

Conducted 2 experiments with retarded residents of a short-term institution. In Exp I, 3 groups of 5 Ss were assigned to self-record the frequency of 1 of 3 target behaviors: talking, face touching, or object touching. Mean ages of the groups were 28.8, 23.8, and 18.2 yrs, respectively. Trained observers simultaneously recorded the frequency of the behaviors during 4 conditions: baseline, self-recording, reinforcement, and return-to-baseline. Self-recording increased the frequency of a positively evaluated behavior (talking) but less consistently reduced the frequency of a negatively evaluated behavior (face touching). Reinforcement increased talking and object touching and less consistently decreased face touching. Reinforcement also increased the relatively good agreement between the self-recorders and the observers. In Exp II, observers recorded the frequency of conversation in the dining room, participation in lounge activities, and tidiness of the bedrooms of 12 18–44 yr old Ss during 6 conditions: existent token economy, improved token economies 1 and 2, baselines 1 and 2, and self-recording. Self-recording was more effective in increasing the frequency of the 3 desirable target behaviors than was the token economy. (27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Problem-Solving Ability as Affected by Mild Stress Versus Minimal or No Stress

April 1969

·

7 Reads

36 undergraduates were assigned randomly either to take a short analogies test or to observe the test from the next room through a 1-way vision mirror. A stooge was employed to get the O to participate actively. Results indicate that Ss from whom no response was required and, therefore, no pressure or stress was involved, performed significantly better than Ss who were required to take the test, and who were told that the outcome would influence their school grade. This latter situation created at least mild stress and simulated a true-life situation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Factors Affecting the Strength of the Vicious Circle Phenomenon

October 1973

·

9 Reads

Evaluated the effects of punishment applied to 40 male hooded rats during a learned escape response, using a drop-start or a gate-lift-start procedure with various dependent measures. Absolute level of responding was enhanced for all Ss by the drop-start procedure. Punishment provided greater relative persistence on certain measures but not on those measures appropriate to tests of the vicious circle hypothesis. It is argued that literature concerned with punishment effects on adient responding is more uniform than is generally assumed. (20 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

The Aftereffects of Reinforcement Magnitude and Stimulus Intensity

April 1975

·

11 Reads

Trained 3 male hooded Lister rats on a fixed-interval schedule where each interval had an equal probability of being terminated by reinforcement or by an auditory stimulus. In testing, the concentration of the milk reinforcer and the intensity of the sound were varied. The durations of both the postreinforcement and the poststimulus pauses were found to be increasing functions of reinforcer magnitude and stimulus intensity, respectively. It is suggested that the effect of changes in the reinforcer magnitude upon the duration of the postreinforcement pause reflects the enhancement or impairment of the discriminative function of the reinforcer in the same way that the discriminative function of any stimulus may be affected by changes in its intensity. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Toward a definition of aggresion

January 1977

·

22 Reads

Difficulties in defining and manipulating aggression have resulted in many highly contrived and relatively meaningless studies. Based on a belief that "real" people in the "real" life situation would be capable of reasonable communication about this matter, the present study took the form of a questionnaire survey of 522 male and 411 female adults (18 yrs and older) concerning the behavioral characteristics they use to describe aggressive and nonaggressive males and females. They were also asked to state what typical situational conditions provoked aggression. Ss generally were in agreement about what the word aggression stands for, and aggression was seen to be a dichotomous construct having a good–bad dimension. It was also found that males typically claimed stronger aggressive feelings in interpersonal types of situations; females considered themselves to be more aggressive (than males) in intrapsychic events. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Time-Out from Reinforcement: Effect on Sibling Aggression

January 1971

·

16 Reads

Made time-out (to) from social reinforcement (5-min isolation) contingent upon each occurrence of aggressive behavior against an 11-mo-old boy by his 26-mo-old sister. Aggressive behaviors (a) occurred at a relatively high rate during a 10-day base-line period, (b) were reduced to essentially a 0 rate by the to contingency during the 1st 10-day experimental period, (c) recovered during a 10-day reversal period, and (d) were reduced again by contingent to during a 2nd 10-day experimental period. Incidental behavioral changes observed during the 2 experimental periods included increased cooperative play with the brother, increased affectional approach to the parents and brother, and decreased oppositional behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Pain-elicited aggression

January 1965

·

179 Reads

Aggressive behavior has been found to be related to a number of environmental and hereditary factors. The specific variable discussed in the present paper as it relates to aggression is that of pain. It is shown that aggression in response to aversive stimulation is a readily identifiable response which appears prior to any specific conditioning in a variety of species. The exact aspects of painful stimulation necessary for the elicitation of aggression are presented, and other variables such as sex, chamber size, number of animals in the chamber, length of session, sensory impairment and reinforcement are reviewed in relation to the pain-aggression phenomenon. Conditioning procedures which employ painful stimuli and which have proved effective in exerting control over single Ss will not, in all cases, exert the same control when more than one S is in the experimental environment and since pain-elicited aggression occurs in higher mammals such as the monkey, the question is raised of its possible existence in man as well. (40 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

The Role of the Temporal Lobe in Feline Aggression and Defense

April 1991

·

10 Reads

Reviews several studies that examine the neural and molecular mechanisms involved in limbic system modulation of spontaneous defense and predatory aggression. Four lines of research are reviewed that investigated the role of the amygdala, ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH), bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), ventral hippocampus, and the benzodiazepine receptor in predatory aggression and defense in the cat. The amygdalo–BNST pathway is implicated in suppression of some types of predatory aggression. Neural inhibition and facilitation in particular cell fields of the ventral hippocampus seem to play a role in modulation of predatory aggression. The benzodiazepine receptor may be involved in modulating excitability in the amygdalo–VMH pathway, and it may also participate in a separate neural substrate that suppresses approach-attack behaviors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)