Developmental Psychobiology

Published by Wiley

Online ISSN: 1098-2302

·

Print ISSN: 0012-1630

Articles


Delta (0.5-1.5 Hz) and sigma (11.5-15.5 Hz) EEG power dynamics throughout quiet sleep in malnourished infants
  • Article

June 1999

·

10 Reads

Igino Fagioli

·

·

The present study aimed to quantify, through power spectral analysis, the dynamics (temporal pattern and temporal interrelationship) of the EEG power in the low-delta (delta) and in the sigma-spindle (sigma) frequency band during quiet sleep (QS) in 5 malnourished infants (MI), 5.5 to 13.5 months old, and in 5 age-matched, healthy control infants (CI). Malnutrition results in modification of the temporal pattern of delta and sigma band power during QS. The delta band power increases faster in MI than in CI, leading to higher power levels in MI at the same time segment. However, the overall trend of the delta band power throughout QS is similar in MI and CI. The premature ending of QS phases (uninterrupted QS periods), and the reduced total amount of QS which have been reported in MI could result in an increased slow wave sleep (SWS) "pressure". This SWS pressure could account for both the higher level and the faster increase of the delta band power during the QS phase which are found in MI when compared to CI.
Share

Mice reared with rats: Effects of prenatal and postnatal maternal environments upon hybrid offspring of C57BL/10J and Swiss Albino mice

January 1973

·

10 Reads

The hybrid offspring of reciprocal crosses between C57BL/10J and Swiss Albino mice were fostered at birth to Swiss Albino mouse mothers or to Purdue-Wistar rat mothers. Measures were taken of incidence of survival, body weight, open-field performance, passive avoidance learning, and fighting. As compared to mice reared by mouse mothers after birth, mice reared by rat mothers (a) had a lower incidence of survival at weaning; (b) weighed more; (c) were less active in the open field, defecated less in the field, and had a greater latency to initiate movement in the field; and (d) had poorer passive learning scores. The findings indicate that the postnatal maternal environment has pervasive effects upon a number of behavioral and morphological characters, but reveal a minimal effect of the prenatal environment. Moreover, they indicate that the absence of differences in fighting scores among the 4 groups may be caused by dominant genes.

Learned persistence at 11-12 days but not at 10-11 days in infant rats

September 1980

·

8 Reads

In 2 experiments using milk-suckling from an anesthetized dam as the reinforcer, evidence is presented that the transitional age in rat pups for learning of persistence as a result of appetitive partial reinforcement is between 11 and 12 days of age. In Experiment I, pups 12-13 days of age showed the partial reinforcement extinction effect (PREE) whereas 10-11-day olds did not. In Experiment II, pups trained at 11-12 days and tested at Day 13 did show a PREE at Day 13 but those trained at 10-11 days did not.

Effects of chronic and acute methylphenidate hydrochloride (Ritalin) administration on locomotor activity, ultrasonic vocalizations, and neuromotor development in 3- to 11-day-old CD-1 mouse pups

November 2001

·

146 Reads

The present study examined the effects of chronic and acute treatment with methylphenidate hydrochloride (Ritalin) on isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations, spontaneous locomotor activity, and neuromotor coordination in 3- to 11-day-old CD-1 mouse pups. In Experiment 1, 3- to 11-day-old pups received daily injections of saline, 5 mg/kg or 20 mg/kg of methylphenidate hydrochloride, or no injection and were tested on postnatal Days 3, 5, 7, 9, and 11. Both doses of methylphenidate resulted in significant increases in locomotor activity at all ages, but had no significant effect on body weight, neuromotor development, or emission of ultrasonic vocalizations. In Experiment 2, pups were given a single dose of methylphenidate (5 or 20 mg/kg), saline, or no injection on one of postnatal Days 5, 7, 9, or 11. This acute methylphenidate treatment increased locomotor activity, but had no significant effects on ultrasonic vocalizations or neuromotor coordination. These results indicate that short-term, chronic methylphenidate treatment elevates locomotor responses, but has no immediate effects on anxietylike responses or on the development of neuromotor behavior of CD-1 mice in the first 11 days of life.

Sapid savvy in sucklings: The effect of quinine hydrochloride on intraoral negative pressure and intake by 11-13-day-old rat pups

May 1987

·

10 Reads

Three experiments describe the consummatory behavior of 11-13-day-old rat pups during and following experience with a model aversive taste, quinine hydrochloride. Pups were observed while away from the dam and while suckling. Results show that pups actively reject quinine adulterated solutions in both situations. They do so by spitting the solution from the mouth when away from the nipple and by leaving the nipple and/or decreasing their sucking effort when with the dam.

Table 4 . Distribution of Hand-Use Preference Across the Three Age Periods for Classi®cation Procedure B
Table 7 . Distribution of Hand-Use Preference Across the Three Age Periods for the Simple Classi®cation Procedure
Evidence of a right-shift factor affecting infant hand-use preferences from 7 to 11 months of age as revealed by latent class analysis
  • Article
  • Full-text available

January 2002

·

262 Reads

Infant hand-use preferences for apprehending objects were assessed three times at 7, 9, and 11 months of age for 154 infants (79 males) using a reliable and valid procedure. Two classification procedures (differing in Type I classification error rates) were used to identify an infant's preference (right, left, no preference) at each age, and these data were examined using two- and three-group latent class analysis models. These analyses revealed the importance of using a handedness classification procedure with low Type I error rates and evidence of a right-shift factor similar to that expressed in child and adult handedness. Thus, infant hand-use preferences for apprehending objects are likely a developmental precursor of adult handedness. The relation of the right-shift factor to increased susceptibility to social influences during development and the evolution of human abilities also is discussed.
Download

Einon DF, Morgan MJ. A critical period for social isolation in the rat. Dev Psychobiol 2: 123-132

March 1977

·

90 Reads

Rats housed in social isolation show heightened levels of object-contact in an open-field and are slower than socially-housed controls to emerge from a small enclosure into an unfamiliar environment. Isolation between 25 and 45 days of age produced an irreversible effect upon object contact but had no lasting effect if between 16 and 25 days or after 45 days. In contrast to object contact, emergence was affected by isolation at any age and the effect was reversed by subsequent social housing. Thus the effects of isolation upon object contact and upon emergence apparently do not depend upon a single underlying variable.

Van den Berg CL, Hol T, Van Ree JM, Spruijt BM, Everts H, Koolhaas JM. Play is indispensable for an adequate development of coping with social challenges in the rat. Dev Psychobiol 34: 129-138

April 1999

·

47 Reads

·

·

·

[...]

·

In this study, young rats were deprived of early social interactions during weeks 4 and 5 of life. Different behavioral tests were conducted in adulthood to study the behavioral responses of rats lacking early social experiences. Juvenile deprivation resulted in decreased social activity and an altered sexual pattern, but did not affect locomotor activity or the performance in the elevated plus maze. Furthermore, behavioral and neuroendocrine responses of juvenile isolated rats were dramatically altered when they were confronted with territorial aggression. Juvenile deprived rats did not readily display a submissive posture in response to the resident and showed no immobility behavior after being returned to the resident's territory, while their plasma corticosterone and adrenaline concentrations were significantly increased compared to nonisolated controls. In contrast, behavioral responses in the shock prod test were not affected by previous isolation. The results suggest that early social experiences are vital for interactions with conspecifics later in life, i.e., aggression, sexual, and social interactions.

Table 1 . Multiple Object Management Act Codes and Descriptions 
Table 2 . List of Significant 2nd-Order Contingencies for 13-Month-Old Infants 
Table 4 . Mean Frequencies (and Standard Errors) of Object Storage and Type of Object Transfer as a Function of Hand-Use Preferences 
Relation of stable hand-use preferences to the development of skill for managing multiple objects from 7 to 13 months of age

July 2008

·

201 Reads

Expression of multiple object management skills (manual acquisition and storage of objects) was examined longitudinally at 7, 9, 11, and 13 months for 38 infants (19 females) whose hand use preference was either stable (consistently right or left across the ages) or nonstable (either no hand-use preference exhibited or inconsistent preference across the ages). Four separate sets of four distinctive objects each were presented singly to the infant's right and left side, with the presentation of each subsequent object contingent on the infant manipulating the previous object. Expression of multiple object management skills significantly increased with age. Infants with stable hand-use preferences produced more object acquisition and storage acts than those without a stable hand-use preference. Older infants with stable hand-use preferences exhibited more "sophisticated" sequences of multiple object management acts than those without. The role of stable hand-use preference in the development of manual skill and cognition is discussed.

The development of fear learning and generalization in 8-13 year-olds

November 2012

·

69 Reads

The current study examined developmental changes in fear learning and generalization in 40 healthy 8-13 year-olds using an aversive conditioning paradigm adapted from Lau et al. [Lau et al. [2008] Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 47:94-102]. In this task, the conditioned stimuli (CS+/CS-) are two neutral female faces, and the unconditioned stimulus is a fearful, screaming face. The second phase of the study also included a generalization stimulus (GS): a 50% blend of the CS± faces. The eye-blink startle reflex was utilized to measure defensive responding. Patterns of fear learning and generalization were qualified by child age. Older children demonstrated greater fear learning (i.e., larger startle during CS+ than CS-) than younger children. In addition, older children exhibited the typical pattern of generalization observed in adults, whereas younger children did not. Finally, fear learning also related to contingency awareness; only children who correctly identified the CS+ demonstrated fear-potentiated startle to the CS+. Clinical implications and future directions are discussed. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals,Inc. Dev Psychobiol 54: 675-684, 2012.

The effects of 8-OH-DPAT and (�)-pindolol on isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations in 3-, 10-, and 14-day-old rats

April 1999

·

7 Reads

It has been established that administration of 5-HT1A agonists attenuates the rate of isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) in 10-day-old rat pups. In this study we extended these findings by examining the effects of administration of the serotonergic 1A receptor agonist, 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT), and mixed 5-HT1A antagonist/beta adrenergic antagonist, (+/-)-pindolol, in 3-, 10-, and 14-day-old rat pups in order to assess the effect of these drugs from a developmental perspective. At all three ages, 8-OH-DPAT significantly reduced the rate of isolation-induced USV. While only the highest dose (1.0 mg/kg) of 8-OH-DPAT administered to the 10- and 14-day-olds significantly reduced the rate of vocalization, both the 0.1- and 1.0-mg/kg doses significantly attenuated the vocalization rate in the 3-day-olds. Pindolol administration did not alter the rate of USV at any age nor did it block the quieting effect that generally occurs when an anesthetized littermate is placed with the isolated pup. We conclude that 8-OH-DPAT is effective as early as 3 days of age in the quieting of isolation-induced USV and that the regional age-dependent development of 5-HT1A receptors and projections are important factors in the observed differential sensitivity to 8-OH-DPAT administration during development.

Hand preferences for bimanual feeding in 140 captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Rearing and ontogenetic determinants

September 1994

·

33 Reads

Hand preference in bimanual feeding was assessed in 140 captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Bimanual feeding was defined as the active use of one hand for feeding with the opposite hand holding other food items. In terms of strength of hand preference, adults were more lateralized than sub-adults. Additionally, mother-reared chimpanzees were more lateralized than nursery-reared chimpanzees. The number of subjects with no hand preference was more prevalent in sub-adults compared with young and older adults. Of those subjects with a significant hand preference, a larger proportion exhibited a right-hand preference. These results are discussed in relation to previous reports of handedness and bimanual feeding in gorillas and bonobos.

Effect of undernutrition on metabolic compartmentation of glutamate and on the incorporation of [14C]leucine into protein in the developing rat brain

October 1975

·

3 Reads

The effect of undernutrition on the rate of protein synthesis and the development of metabolic compartmentation of glutamate in the brain was investigated by using [U -14 C] leucine as precursor. In the brain of normal rats the incorporation rate of [14C] leucine into protein was at a maximum during the 3rd week after birth, but in the undernourished animal this rate was markedly lower. The biochemical maturation of the brain, followed in terms of the age-dependent increase in the glutamine/glutamate specific radioactivity ratio, was severly retarded in the undernourished animals, mainly as a result of a marked depression in the conversion of leucine carbon into glutamine. However these biochemical effects of undernutrition were reversible: on rehabilitation from Day 21-35 the rate of conversion of leucine carbon, both into proteins and glutamate and glutamine, was restored to normal.

Neonatal 6-OHDA lesions and rearing in complex environments: Regional effects on adult brain 14C-2-deoxyglucose uptake revealed by exposure to novel stimulation

May 1992

·

10 Reads

Behavioral and neuromorphological data have suggested at least a partial interaction between the effects of norepinephrine-depleting neonatal 6-OHDA lesions and the effects of rearing in enriched environments. The present study examined the impact of both of these early manipulations upon regional brain uptake of 14C-2-deoxyglucose (14C-2DG) in adulthood. Newborn rats received 6-OHDA (50 mg/kg s.c.) or vehicle and, after weaning at 25 days, were reared in isolated versus enriched conditions. Regional brain 14C-2DG uptake was then examined at 70-80 days of age--either in the home cage or while animals were being exposed to novel, presumably arousing, stimulation. Ninety-seven brain regions were examined in eight separate groups. Results indicated that (1) Under baseline conditions, neither neonatal 6-OHDA nor differential rearing conditions produced widespread alterations in regional brain 14C-2DG uptake profiles. An overall enrichment effect was seen on only five brain areas, with rats reared in enriched environments showing lower levels of 14C-2DG uptake (-20% to -30%) than isolated rats. Neonatal 6-OHDA produced no main effect on 14C-2DG uptake in any brain region. (2) In contrast, when 14C-2DG uptake was assessed during exposure to a novel environment, five brain areas showed differential 14C-2DG uptake in 6-OHDA-treated rats, and 20 brain areas showed differential uptake in rats reared in enriched conditions. (3) No significant interaction effect on brain regional 14C-2DG uptake was observed between neonatal 6-OHDA and environmental complexity factors. These results are consistent with the notion that enduring effects of rearing and early 6-OHDA treatment may, independently, relate to a general reactivity factor. They also indicate that some effects of early neurochemical injury and subsequent experiential factors may not be apparent under normal resting conditions, but only become evident in the presence of appropriate "activating" stimulation.

McCann, L.I. & Matthews, J.J. The effects of lifelong isolation on species identification in zebrafish (Brachydanio rerio). Dev. Psychobiol. 7, 159-163

March 1974

·

2 Reads

Virtually no information is currently available regarding the relative effects of innate and experiential factors on the development of species identification in fish. Those studies which have approached the problem through schooling behavior have confounded species identification with attraction to a single available school of species-mates. In this study, isolated Brachydanio rerio spent significantly less time in nonpolarized schools with species-mates than controls when animals of a related species were available as an alternative. This difference suggests the involvement of experiential factors in species identification and questions the assumption that species identification is entirely innate.

Do 16-week-old infants anticipate stimulus offsets?

July 1979

·

7 Reads

Twenty alert 16-week-old infants were presented with 20-sec tones at variable intertrial intervals for 10 trials while beat-by-beat heart rate responses were recorded to assess response at offset as well as onset of each stimulus. Consistent with past research, onsets elicited deceleratory responses which habituated and showed some evidence of dishabituation with a change in stimulus frequency on the last 2 trials. Offsets also elicited significant deceleratory responses overall, but inspection of pre-offset heart rate suggested that deceleration in anticipation of the offset event appeared after a few stimulus repetitions and increased in magnitude over trials. However, individual variability was considerable and although the anticipatory response was significant averaged over all trials, the apparent increase over trials did not reach statistical significance. The evidence clearly indicates infants quickly process and act upon temporal information in a stimulus.

Longitudinal examination of infant baseline and reactivity cortisol from ages 7 to 16 months: Longitudinal Examination of Infant Cortisol

March 2015

·

199 Reads

This study characterized the longitudinal evolution of HPA axis functioning from 7 to 16 months of age and identified individual and environmental factors that shape changes in HPA axis functioning over time. Participants were 167 mother-infant dyads drawn from a larger longitudinal study, recruited based on maternal history of being maltreated during childhood. Salivary cortisol levels were assessed before and after age-appropriate psychosocial stressors when infants were 7 and 16 months old. Maternal observed parenting and maternal reports of infant and environmental characteristics were obtained at 7 months and evaluated as predictors of changes in infant baseline cortisol and reactivity from 7 to 16 months. Results revealed that infants did not show a cortisol response at 7 months, but reactivity to psychosocial stress emerged by 16 months. Individual differences in cortisol baseline and reactivity levels over time were related to infant sex and maternal overcontrolling behaviors, underscoring the malleable and socially informed nature of early HPA axis functioning. Findings can inform prevention and intervention efforts to promote healthy stress regulation during infancy. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Ontogeny of maternal behavior in the laboratory rat: Early origins in 18- to 27-day-old young

September 1979

·

14 Reads

The responses of unweaned juveniles, 18 to 27 days of age, in brief tests with pups younger than 10 days were observed during initial exposure and at 4-6-day intervals. They showed no aversive responses and actively sought contact with the pups until 24 days when contact-seeking declined sharply. Young exposed to pups before 24 days continued to seek contact with them after 24 days showing an effect of the prior experience. Juveniles given a choice between young pups and a warm bowl chose the pups; given a choice between young pups and age mates, they chose the young pups until day 23, but chose the age mates after then. When housed continuously with young pups at 22 days of age, 5 of 9 retrieved and grouped pups with a latency of 1 day. Our results indicate that timidity or fear of novelty develops in rats in this situation about Day 24 and thereafter is a factor inhibiting the expression of positive social behavior toward pups.

Cortisol Levels in Relation to Maternal Interaction and Child Internalizing Behavior in Preterm and Full-Term Children at 18 Months Corrected Age

March 2011

·

109 Reads

Cortisol levels were compared in children born preterm at extremely low gestational age (ELGA; 24-28 weeks), very low gestational age (VGLA; 29-32 weeks), and full-term in response to cognitive assessment at 18 months corrected age (CA). Further, we investigated the relationship between maternal interactive behaviors and child internalizing behaviors (rated by the mother) in relation to child cortisol levels. EGLA children had higher "pretest" cortisol levels and a different pattern of cortisol response to cognitive assessment compared to VGLA and full-terms. Higher cortisol levels in ELGA, but not full-term, children were associated with less optimal mother interactive behavior. Moreover, the pattern of cortisol change was related to internalizing behaviors among ELGA, and to a lesser degree VLGA children. In conclusion, our findings suggest altered programming of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in preterm children, as well as their greater sensitivity to environmental context such as maternal interactive behavior.

Neonatal sex-steroid hormones and timidity in 6-18-month-old boys and girls

May 1983

·

48 Reads

Five sex-steroid hormones (testosterone, androstenedione, estradiol, estrone, and progesterone) are assayed in umbilical cord blood. Timidity is assessed in home and laboratory observations by reactions to a range of novel toys in children from 6 to 18 months of age. Significant short- (1 week) and long- (several months) term stability in timidity is demonstrated. Girls have higher mean scores on observed timidity than do boys in 2 of the 3 samples tested. An across-age timidity score is computed which shows significant correlations with neonatal progesterone, testosterone, and estradiol for boys, but is not significantly predicted by hormone concentrations in girls.



Exposure to Ethanol on Prenatal Days 19-20 Increases Ethanol Intake and Palatability in the Infant Rat: Involvement of kappa and mu Opioid Receptors

September 2013

·

170 Reads

Prenatal exposure to ethanol on gestation Days 19-20, but not 17-18, increases ethanol acceptance in infant rats. This effect seems to be a conditioned response acquired prenatally, mediated by the opioid system, which could be stimulated by ethanol's pharmacological properties (mu-opioid receptors) or by a component of the amniotic fluid from gestation-day 20 (kappa-inducing factor). The latter option was evaluated administering non-ethanol chemosensory stimuli on gestation Days 19-20 and testing postnatal intake and palatability. However, prenatal exposure to anise or vanilla increased neither intake nor palatability of these tastants on postnatal Day 14. In experiment 2, the role of ethanol's pharmacological effect was tested by administering ethanol and selective antagonists of mu and kappa opioid receptors prenatally. Blocking the mu-opioid receptor system completely reversed the effects on intake and palatability, while antagonizing kappa receptors only partially reduced the effects on palatability. This suggests that the pharmacological effect of ethanol on the fetal mu opioid system is the appetitive reinforcer, which induces the prenatally conditioned preference detected in the preweanling period. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol.

Role of Medial Prefrontal NMDA Receptors in Spatial Delayed Alternation in 19-, 26-, and 33-Day-Old Rats

September 2010

·

9 Reads

Long-Evans rats were trained on spatial delayed alteration (SDA) in a T-maze following medial prefrontal cortical (mPFC) infusions of different doses of the noncompetitive NMDA-receptor antagonist, MK-801 (.125 microl; .25 microl; or .25 microlsaline, bilaterally), on postnatal day (PND) 19, 26, or 33. Pups trained on PND 19 showed almost no learning of SDA, regardless of drug condition (including saline). On PND 26, both doses of MK-801 significantly and equivalently prevented SDA learning, with performance during the final three training blocks remaining near chance levels, in contrast with 85% correct performance in the saline control group. On PND 33, substantial SDA learning was evident regardless of dose, although a modest impairment appeared in mid-training at both doses. These findings confirm previous reports of mPFC involvement in the early postnatal ontogeny of SDA and suggest a developmentally transient role of mPFC NMDA-receptor function in this task.

Renner MJ, Rosenzweig MR. Social interactions among rats housed in grouped and enriched conditions. Dev Psychobiol 19: 303-313

July 1986

·

454 Reads

Two experiments examine the hypothesis that brain differences found in environmental enrichment are due to differences in social interaction. Both experiments compare enriched versus group housed rats using videotape records of home cage activity, scored with a protocol developed by the authors. Experiment 1 examines social interactions in group and enriched housed rats in the first 30 days postweaning; In Experiment 2 rats were housed in the differential environments from 90 to 120 days of age, an age at which rats have been reported no longer to engage in play; in addition, weights of sections of the brain were obtained at sacrifice and these showed typical patterns of differences among rats from differential environments. Neither experiment revealed any consistent pattern of differences in social interaction, either by chi-square comparisons of overall profiles of social activity or by discriminant analysis applied to behavioral observations. No evidence was found in support of the hypothesis that play is responsible for the effects of environmental enrichment.



Gilbert Gottlieb (1929–2006)

May 2007

·

43 Reads

Gilbert Gottlieb, a pioneer in developmental psychobiology, died on July 13, 2006. We pay tribute to the man, to his career, and to his dedication to developmental science.


The ontogeny of autonomic measures in 6- and 12-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(3), 197-208

April 2006

·

33 Reads

The purpose of this study was to develop a standardized protocol to measure preejection period (PEP), a measure of sympathetic nervous system, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), a measure of parasympathetic nervous system, during resting and challenging states for 6- and 12-month-old infants and to determine developmental changes and individual stability of these measures. A 7-min reactivity protocol was administered to Latino infants at 6 months (n=194) and 12 months (n=181). Results showed: (1) it is feasible to measure PEP and RSA in infants, (2) the protocol elicited significant autonomic changes, (3) individual resting autonomic measures were moderately stable from 6 to 12 months, but reactivity measures were not stable, and (4) heart rate and RSA resting and challenge group means changed significantly from 6 to 12 months. Findings suggest that although infants' autonomic responses show developmental changes, individuals' rank order is stable from 6 to 12 months of age.


Prenatal and Postnatal determinants of the 1st suckling episode in the albino rat

July 1982

·

20 Reads

Conditions under which an odor could elicit the 1st nipple attachment in albino rats were investigated. In Experiment I rats exposed prenatally and immediately after birth to citral, a lemon scent, suckled the washed nipples of an anesthetized, parturient dam when the nipples had been scented with citral. Moreover, these rats did not suckle the normal, unwashed nipples of these dams. In Experiment II rats were exposed to citral either (a) in utero, (b) immediately after birth, (c) both pre- and postnatally, or (d) not at all. Only rats in Group (c) attached to washed, citral-scented nipples and did not suckle the normal unwashed nipples that elicited suckling in control rats. These findings suggest that prenatal and postnatal events can determine which olfactory stimuli elicit the newborn rat's 1st nipple attachment.


Gandelman R, Zarrow M, Denenberg V. Maternal behavior: differences between mother and virgin mice as a function of the testing procedure. Dev Psychobiology 3: 207-214

February 1970

·

15 Reads

Retrieval by lactating and virgin mice was compared in a straight alley and a T-maze. In both test situations the retrieval performance of the lactating mouse was superior to that of the virgin animal. No discrimination was seen between a live pup and a dead pup; a rubber toy, however, was retrieved least by both the lactating and virgin mice. Contrary to previous results, these experiments demonstrate a significant difference in maternal behavior between lactating and virgin mice.

Acute ethanol contamination of the amniotic fluid during gestational Day 21

September 1990

·

44 Reads

Two experiments investigated the effects of an acute alcohol prenatal experience during gestational Day 21 in the rat. At postnatal Days 8 and 9, this experience was sufficient to significantly increase ethanol odor preference as well as alcohol intake. Fetuses treated with a nonethanol stimulus (lemon) also exhibited changes suggesting increased lemon olfactory acceptance patterns (Exp. 1). Furthermore, when the olfactory component of the solutions experienced in utero were later paired with a novel tactile cue, responsiveness to such cue was strongly affected. Pups prenatally exposed to alcohol exhibited significantly lower tactile preference scores when texture was postnatally paired with ethanol odor when compared to specific controls. This effect was also observed in lemon-treated subjects after pairing defined by lemon-texture trials (Exp. 2). The results reported in alcohol-treated subjects appear not to be related with postabsorptive effects of the drug. It is suggested that sensory prenatal experience with alcohol is responsible for the reported changes in postnatal alcohol responsiveness patterns.

Laterality in persons with intellectual disability II. Hand, foot, ear, and eye laterality in persons with Trisomy 21 and Williams-Beuren Syndrome

September 2006

·

148 Reads

Laterality (hand, foot, ear, and eye) was assessed in participants with Trisomy 21 (62) and Williams-Beuren syndrome (WBS) (39). Handedness was also assessed in a card reaching task. The comparison group included 184 typically developing persons. Two independent age sub-groups were formed: 7 to 10 years old and 11 to 34 years old. We confirmed previous data: individuals with T21 were more frequently left- or mixed-handed than typically developing persons; individuals with WBS had intermediate scores. The two groups with genetic disorders had less right foot preference. Manual and foot inconsistencies characterized both groups with genetic disorders. Cross hand-foot preference was lower in the typically developing group. Differences in IQ levels did not correlate with differences in laterality scores. Overall laterality profiles were not the same in the two groups with genetic disorders: the greatest differences were observed between typically developing persons and persons with Trisomy 21.

Specific Grasp Characteristics of Children With Trisomy 21

December 2010

·

139 Reads

Children with trisomy 21 display atypical manual skills that change to some extent during development. We examined grasp characteristics and their development in 35 children with trisomy 21, aged 4-18 years, who performed simple manual tasks (two manual tasks of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, and grasping of five wooden blocks whose size was determined by their hand size). The age-matched comparison group included 35 typically developing children. Children with trisomy 21 were found to use fewer fingers than children in the comparison group in each task. They also used specific grasps and tended to extend fingers that were not involved in the grip. While some specific grasp characteristics of children with trisomy 21 decreased with age, other did not, and remained present throughout development. The perceptual-motor development of children with trisomy 21 should be analyzed in terms of atypical development rather than developmental delay.

Absence of effects of corticosterone given at 22 days

January 1976

·

7 Reads

Mice given a single large dose of corticosterone at 2 days of age, and tested when adult, have shown lasting alterations in operant behavior, associated with reductions in cerebral weight and DNA content. When mice were given corticosterone at 22 days, no changes appeared in cerebral weight, DNA, or in operant behavior, but an apparent decrease did occur in open-field activity. The results suggest that the effects of corticosterone treatment at 2 days on the developing brain are mediated by one or more types of sensitivity to the steroid that have been largely lost by 22 days.

Marler, P. Innate learning preferences: signals for communication. Dev. Psychobiol. 23, 557-568

November 1990

·

33 Reads

Research on the ways in which different species of birds learn to sing is used to illustrate the necessity of taking innate factors into account in studies of behavioral development. Experiments on two species of songbirds are described that reveal innate species differences in responsiveness to tape-recorded songs. Conspecific songs are favored over those of other species. These patterns of innately varying responsiveness provide a basis for the development, not of stereotyped behavior, but of variable, individually learned behavior. The viewpoint is presented that mechanisms that differ innately from species to species, some with general functions, others specialized for particular ontogenetic assignments, provide the necessary substrates with which experience interacts.

Differential effects of SCH 23390 on immobility behaviors in developing rats

September 1993

·

7 Reads

The effects of a single injection of a dopamine D1 antagonist, SCH 23390, at doses of 0, 0.05, 0.1, and 0.2 mg/kg on three different kinds of immobility behavior were tested in rats of 10, 15, 20, and 30 days of age. Each animal was tested for the dorsal immobility response (DIR), vertical cling catalepsy, and bar catalepsy. A different pattern of results was found for each of the three immobility behaviors. SCH 23390 significantly increased the DIR at each age except 15 days; there was a progressive increase in effect from 10 to 20 to 30 days of age. At the lowest dose, drug-induced bar catalepsy peaked at 15 days of age and declined at 20 and 30 days of age. The effect of SCH 23390 on vertical cling catalepsy increased with age, plateauing at 20 days. Thus, the developmental pattern of immobility responses to this dopamine D1 antagonist differs with each behavior measured.

Sucking behaviors of normal 3-day-old female neonates during a 24-hr period

January 1984

·

10 Reads

Feeding behaviors were measured during six feeding sessions distributed throughout a 24-hr period in 10 normal 3-day-old female infants. Infants were individually videotaped during feeding sessions at 13:00, 17:00, 21:00, 01:00, 05:00, and 09:00 hours. Total mealtime, nutritive sucking time, pause time, number of nutritive sucks, and amount of nutrient consumed were measured. None of these feeding variables were affected by the time of day an infant was fed. Maternal interactions with their infants such as auditory stimulation, caretaking touches or tender touches were unaffected by the time of a feed. These maternal behaviors did not correlate with any of the feeding variables. These findings suggest that 3-day-old infants experiencing routine nursery care do not express specific day-night feeding differences and that certain maternal behaviors do not influence the infants' feeding pattern.

Lateral Manual Asymmetries: A Longitudinal Study From Birth to 24 Months

January 2014

·

151 Reads

Longitudinal studies tracking the early development of manual asymmetries are fairly rare compared to the large number of studies assessing hand preference in infancy. Moreover, most prior longitudinal studies have performed behavioral observation over relatively short-time spans considering the celerity of early development. This study aims (i) to investigate the direction and consistency of manual lateral asymmetries over a longer period, from birth to 24 months of age, and (ii) to compare individual and group trajectories to better understand discrepancies between prior studies. Nineteen healthy infants were observed eight times in tasks that were adjusted progressively as infants manual skills developed. Results suggested two distinct periods in terms of the direction, strength, and consistency of manual preference. First, infants went through an initial phase characterized by a lack of lateral manual asymmetries. From 9 months of age, however, group analyses revealed an emerging and steadily growing right lateral bias over time, while individual trajectories revealed that the group-level right-bias formed progressively from a background of highly fluctuating and highly variable developmental trajectories. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol.

Temporal organization of fetal behavior from 24-weeks gestation onwards in normal and complicated pregnancies

June 1999

·

20 Reads

Developmental aspects of behavioral organization were investigated in 29 healthy fetuses from 24-weeks gestation onwards: (a) short-term association between body (GM) and eye (EM) movements; (b) linkage of pairs of the three state variables [fetal heart rate pattern (FHRP), GM, and EM]; and (c) sequence of change of state variables during transitions. Linkage and sequence were also studied in complicated pregnancies. Short-term association between GM and EM was well established after 28 weeks. Linkage of state variables improved considerably after 32-34 weeks. FHRP was the first variable to change during synchronized transitions from 1F to 2F between 28-39 weeks, and the last variable during 2F to 1F transitions between 32-39 weeks. Although clear developmental patterns could be recognized, the interfetal variability was such that identification of the abnormal fetus is still difficult. Only transitions were significantly different in growth-restricted fetuses, as they showed no specific sequence of change. Assessing the temporal organization of fetal behavior seems, therefore, until now, not of great clinical value.

The effects of 24-hr maternal separation and of litter-size reduction on the isolation-distress response of 12-day-old rat pups

December 1993

·

33 Reads

We asked whether the mother-infant interaction acted over time to regulate the ultrasonic vocalization (USV) and activity responses of 12-day-old rat pups to isolation in a novel test area. In a series of four experiments, we found that 24 hr of maternal deprivation did not alter USV isolation responses, but that reduction of litter size to 4 pups from 8 markedly attenuated the USV response and increased weight gain, without effects on activity level. Ambient temperatures during 24-hr maternal separation were varied from 23 degrees to 35 degrees C without effect on subsequent USV responses, but activity levels were markedly reduced in pups separated at the low heat level. Possible mechanisms for maternal regulation of USV responses involving nutrient effects, altered processing of thermal stimuli, and the inhibition of thermogenesis by nutrient deprivation are discussed.

Inception of a 24-hr memory capacity in two mouse strains

November 1973

·

18 Reads

Separate groups of C3H and Swiss-Webster (S-W) mice received 25 escape training trials in a straight alley at 8 or 10 days of age. Each of these groups was then divided into 3 retention groups, receiving an additional 25 training trials, 1, 6, or 24 hr following original training. Comparisons of the retest scores with original training scores, as well as with litter-mate controls without prior training, indicated that both strains trained at 8 days of age failed to demonstrate retention of learning when retested 24 hr after training, although they showed reliable 1-hr retention and some evidence of 6-hr retention. However, mice trained at 10 days of age performed better on the retention tests at all retest intervals than they had on original training, as well as better than maturation controls without prior training. These results indicate that a 24-hr retention capacity for escape training develops between 8-10 days of age in both mouse strains.

Cardiac reactivity is associated with changes in negative emotion in 24-month-olds

March 2005

·

90 Reads

Despite the call for multilevel observation of negative affect, including multiple physiological systems, too little empirical research has been conducted in infants and young children, and physiology-affect associations are not consistently reported. We examined changes in heart rate, respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and preejection period in 24-month-olds across four increasingly challenging, emotion-eliciting tasks. We predicted that changes in cardiac reactivity would be systematically related to changes in negative affect. Results largely support the predictions with one important exception. With increasing distress across the tasks, HR increased and RSA decreased. However, no significant changes in PEP were observed. HR was associated with negative affect during all tasks, and changes in HR were related to changes in negative affect. PEP and negative affect were associated, but only marginally so. Within-subject analyses confirmed the predicted associations. Finally, the associations between physiology and negative affect were different for boys and girls. We discuss these results in the context of implications for future research on cardiac-affect associations in young children.

Britton GB, Segan AT, Sejour J, Mancebo SE. Early exposure to methylphenidate increases fear responses in an aversive context in adult rats. Dev Psychobiol 49: 265-275

April 2007

·

34 Reads

The effects of methylphenidate hydrochloride (MPH) administration during development on fear acquisition and retention in adulthood were examined using classical fear conditioning. Male Sprague-Dawley rats were administered MPH (2 mg/kg) or saline twice daily from postnatal day (PD) 25 to 39, and were trained and tested on PD 81 and 82. During training, shock unconditioned stimulus (US) presentations were explicitly paired with an auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) or occurred unsignaled in the training context. No effect of MPH treatment was found during fear acquisition when shock was signaled or unsignaled during training, but 24 h retention tests in the training context revealed enhanced fear responses in MPH-treated animals that received unsignaled training. These results support recent reports of enhanced anxiety-like behaviors in adult rats caused by early developmental MPH treatment and highlight the need for further research into the long-term effects of developmental exposure to stimulants aimed at pediatric populations.

Martel FL, Nevison CM, Simpson MJ, Keverne EB. Effects of opioid receptor blockade on the social behavior of rhesus monkeys living in large family groups. Dev Psychobiol 28: 71-84

March 1995

·

29 Reads

Rhesus monkeys of 9 weeks, 48 weeks, 100 weeks, 150 weeks of age (young subjects), or mature parous females that were not lactating were given acute single doses of the opioid antagonist naloxone (0.5 mg/kg) and vehicle on different days and observed in their familial social groups. Naloxone increased the occurrence of affiliative behaviours. Young subjects spent more time in contact with their mothers but showed no changes in social grooming. Maternal contact was actively sought through contact vocalizations, decreasing proximity, and, for the youngest infants, increased attempts to suckle. Mature females made more solicitations for grooming and received more grooming from their companions. These results are interpreted in terms of naloxone blocking the positive effect arising from social contact and thus causing subjects to seek further affiliative comfort.

Epigenetic influence of social experiences across the lifespan. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 299-311

May 2010

·

145 Reads

The critical role of social interactions in driving phenotypic variation has long been inferred from the association between early social deprivation and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes. Recent evidence has implicated molecular pathways involved in the regulation of gene expression as one possible route through which these long-term outcomes are achieved. These epigenetic effects, though not exclusive to social experiences, may be a mechanism through which the quality of the social environment becomes embedded at a biological level. Moreover, there is increasing evidence for the transgenerational impact of these early experiences mediated through changes in social and reproductive behavior exhibited in adulthood. In this review, recent studies which highlight the epigenetic effects of parent-offspring, peer and adult social interactions both with and across generations will be discussed and the implications of this research for understanding the developmental origins of individual differences in brain and behavior will be explored.

Top-cited authors