Figure 2 - uploaded by Kofi adu manyah
Content may be subject to copyright.
This figure shows average oral vowel, nasal vowel and post-vocalic consonant durations and standard deviations for the second speaker (ms)
Similar publications
The article is devoted to analysis of the errors that occur when acoustic diagnostics of stress state for shaped profiles of metal structures. The analysis of methods for acoustic diagnostics of stressed state of shaped profiles was conducted using mirror-shadow method and areas, causing the occurrence of errors, were revealed. The analysis of tool...
Citations
... They are not nasalised because they follow nasal letters (m, n), rather, speakers of the language spontaneously nasalise all vowels. It takes a longer time to pronounce words with nasals, adding to the several reasons why Twi words generally take longer to pronounce (Manyah, 2011). ...
... When visual search for target stimuli are distinguished from distractors by a unique feature, such as colour, it becomes fast, efficient, and subjectively effortless owing to pop-out effects (Treisman & Gormican, 1988). Numerous studies have shown that negative priming effects increase when subjects are induced to anticipate selection difficulty between target and distractor stimuli (Fox, 1994;Gamboz, Russo, & Fox, 2000;Houghton, Tipper, Weaver, & Shore, 1996;Pritchard & Neumann, 2009, 2011. ...
In the current experiments, within- and between-language primed lexical decision tasks with Twi-English bilinguals were used. The aim was to explore the priming effects produced by attended and ignored words, in an effort to draw theoretical and empirical parallels and differences between the mechanisms of excitation and inhibition and to isolate the different circumstances in which these mechanisms operate in bilingual language processing. In the within-language (Twi) experiment, facilitatory (positive) priming resulted when a prime word and subsequent probe target word were identical, whereas delayed decisions to probe targets (negative priming) ensued when the ignored prime word was conceptually identical to the subsequent probe target word. In contrast, while the between-language (Twi-English) experiments replicated the ignored repetition negative priming effect, no evidence of positive priming was observed. These between-language findings undermine episodic retrieval models of selective attention that discount inhibitory processes in negative priming paradigms. Instead, our findings substantiate inhibition-based accounts by showing that there are two sources of inhibition operating at the local word and global language levels of abstraction. The findings also support bilingual language representations in which the words of the two languages are integrated.