Figure 2 - available from: Research in Science Education
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
The five categories, representing possible ways of reaching explanations of phenomena, are distributed in relation to their associations with logical, left side, and intuitive, right side, ways of reaching explanations. N number of preschools where the category appears (total number of preschools = 11)
Source publication
This study explores if and how teachers combine practices of science and of preschool (children 1–5 years old) into preschool science practice. Views of knowing may differ between science practices, traditionally associated with masculinity and rationality, and preschool practices, traditionally associated with femininity and caring. Recognising th...
Similar publications
Uber’s programme of planetary expansion rapidly captures users and integrates cities. Like other platforms, Uber privileges growth at all costs in order to spin up a set of self-reinforcing dynamics like virtuous cycles and network effects. However, drawing on Arrighi, Hobson, and Arendt, Uber can also be understood as a contemporary incarnation of...
Citations
... As a result, the main aim of including science in early childhood education is not for young children to acquire scientific concepts, but to encourage them to question their own models and construct new ones that are increasingly closer to the models of school science [6]. Therefore, it is important to note that young children's models or explanations should not be evaluated as 'right' or 'wrong' [7]. ...
Many studies have been conducted in recent years on the explanations given by preschoolage
children about different natural phenomena. Nonetheless, very few studies have actually focused
on the important domain of matter and its transformations. Specifically, the field of chemical reactions
remains unexplored. This qualitative study aims to investigate the explanations of twenty-two 5- to
6-year-old children about combustion, while at the same time evaluating the effect of prior experience
with science activities on their interpretations. For this study, the following experiment was proposed:
burning a candle inside an inverted vessel. The following data collection tools were used: a Predict-
Observe-Explain (POE) strategy and audio and video recordings. The children’s explanations were
analysed using classification frameworks, which had been developed in previous studies. The results
of this study suggest that young children tend to provide naturalistic explanations about combustion.
This finding is an indicator that young children are able to construct mental representations within
this conceptual domain. Likewise, the results indicate that children who are used to engaging
in inquiry-based activities may be more likely to establish a relationship with previous learning
experiences to interpret other natural phenomena.
... Additionally, providing a positive, safe environment recognizes all developmental domains, including social-emotional, are necessary for science learning (Jaber and Hammer 2016;Klemm and Neuhaus 2017;Lee 2017). Finally, providing this environment recognizes the varied experiences, or funds of knowledge, young children bring to the classroom to be utilized (Dunac and Demir 2017) and highlight young children's different ways of knowing science (Areljung et al. 2016;Siry and Kremer 2011). These different ways of knowing provide a foundation for teachers to scaffold learning in the moment and extend the learning by providing materials or planned activities (Siry and Kremer 2011). ...
Reforms in K-12 science education and their implications for PreK teaching, make this a critical time for developing a unified vision for early childhood science education. The goal of this essay is to provide a vision of what it might look like to leverage the best of both early childhood and K-12 science education reform efforts for meaningful science learning opportunities for young children. The essay suggests to best support young children, PreK science education must implement the holistic approach of early childhood education (i.e., physical, social-emotional, and cognitive development) while at the same time taking up the richer scientific disciplinary practices put forth in recent science reform efforts. Bringing together the strengths of both early childhood and science education will support young children in making sense of natural phenomena in the world around them while valuing them as capable science learners with prior experiences.
... I nästa avsnitt redogör jag för studier som specifikt undersöker naturvetenskap i förskolan ur genusperspektiv. Genusperspektiv på naturvetenskap i förskolan I de förhållandevis få studier som undersöker naturvetenskap i förskolan ur ett genusperspektiv uppmärksammas framförallt förskollärarnas uppfattningar, samt egna erfarenheter av naturvetenskap (se exempelvis Andersson, 2011;Andersson & Gullberg, 2014;Areljung, Ottander, & Due, 2017;Gullberg, Andersson, Danielsson, Scantlebury, & Hussénius, 2018). Exempelvis studeras förskollärares sätt att hantera traditionella normer förknippade med naturvetenskap (maskulinitet, objektivitet, fakta) i kombination med normer som ofta förknippas med förskola (omsorg, femininitet, intuition, subjektivitet) . ...
The aim of this thesis is to explore how scientific phenomena, together with other agents (human and nonhuman) in preschool, participate in and co-create gendering processes as well as children’s emergent scientific explorations. These are seen as mutual processes emerging in the daily doings and routines in preschool. As a theoretical and methodological foundation, a new materialist perspective drawing on Karen Barad’s (2007) theory of agential realism and diffractive methodology were used, as well as de Freitas and Palmer’s (2016) notion concerning how scientific concepts can work as creative playmates in children’s explorations. The thesis includes four papers that build on data conducted during a field study in a Swedish preschool, together with 25 five year-old children and three teachers. Participant observations, including video recordings and field notes were made over a period of 5 months. The results show that, if and how children get to engage with emergent science is linked to if and how they manage to occupy space and co-act with different materials. As the children were co-acting with different materials, scientific phenomena could make themselves known and intelligible to the children. This means that becoming scientific is something that is enabled in entanglements. One important result connected to this is that these entanglements can include ways and agents not commonly thought of as “scientific”, such as a drawing table, hearts, and feminine discourses. Another result is that even though girls and boys explore together within the same activity, this does not automatically lead to a situation that is more equal. From these results I discuss how children’s emergent scientific explorations are always part of larger, gendered processes. I also discuss the importance to highlight how science in preschool can be “done” in various ways. Otherwise there is a risk that the false picture could be created that some children, already at preschool age, are more “suitable” for science, while others are created as “less suitable”, just as can occur in school and higher education. Furthermore, during the discussion I show how scientific phenomena can work as tools for teachers to approach gendering processes together with.
... Consequently, I entered the ScienceMaking project with the assumption that science-arts integration harbour a potential to disrupt the human/nature and objective/subjective binaries that often constrain what counts as valid knowledge production in STEM education. I was aiming for a comprehensive story of knowledge production that allows for children's subjective contributions and entangled being in the world (see Areljung, Ottander & Due, 2017). ...
This chapter examines how STEAM education may transform education in the STEM subjects towards education for a sustainable future. Particularly, it examines the potential of combining science and arts in preschool practice (children aged 1–5 years) for the sake of fostering sustainable knowing and being in the world. Here, it pursues the idea that everyday science verbs (e.g., rolling, bouncing and sticking) may be referents for children–matter relations in which science learning and creativity emerge. The chapter includes two stories from a collaboration with preschool teachers who have implemented verb-based science-arts education in practice. In one story, the verbs “sprout and grow” were combined with painting and drama, and in the other story, the verb “shade” (to cast a shadow) was combined with music, dancing and painting. Grounded in Edvin Østergaard’s plea to make more room for aesthetic experience in science education, in Barbara McClintock’s scientific creativity and “feeling for the organism”, and in Karen Barad’s agential realism, the chapter portrays examples of science-arts education that allow children to be intensely involved in the world. It concludes that the arts may help children not only to communicate and explore science phenomena, but also to sympathise with nature’s goings on from within; from their own multifaceted experiences of what it is like to cast a shadow, sprout and grow.
... Studies using gender perspectives have mostly focused on teachers' approaches and conceptions. For example, how teachers handle and combine traditional norms connected to science (masculinity, objectivity, facts) with norms connected to preschool (care, femininity, intuition, subjectivity) (Areljung, Ottander, and Due 2016), pre-service teachers' reflections and different ways of identifying events related to gender and emergent science (Gullberg et al. 2018) and pre-service teachers' own (gendered) experiences from learning science and technology and how this affects their ability to teach science and technology to children (Andersson 2011;Andersson and Gullberg 2014;Hedlin and Gunnarsson 2014). These studies show that norms and discourses connected to science and gender affect teachers and how they teach, but that these aspects can be less determining if teachers combine scientific facts with children's subjective theories and explorations. ...
This article explores gendered processes in preschool science through Barad's agential realism [2007. Meeting the Universe Halfway. Quantum Physics of the Entanglement of Matter and Meaning. London: Duke Universal Press], and as such, the study makes both theoretical and empirical contributions in how it combines perspectives from emergent science [Siraj-Blatchford, J. 2001. Emergent Science and Technology in the Early Years." Paper presented at the XXIII World Congress of OMEP. Santiago, Chile, August 3], new materialism, and gender theory. Empirically, the study makes use of data constructed during a field study in a Swedish preschool with five-year-old children. The focus of the field study was the children's play and explorations together with the preschool environment, during activities not specifically guided by teachers. The analysis highlights how the children's identities and scientific explorations are made possible as well as constrained together with the preschool's material-discursive environment. As such, the study demonstrates how teachers cannot rely on any environment, activity, choice or subject content to be (gender) neutral. ARTICLE HISTORY
... These seemingly opposing ideals of the preschool and science -in simplified terms framed in an objectivity/subjectivity dichotomy -suggest that epistemological tensions arise as educators implement science activities in preschool. Such tensions are indicated in recent Swedish studies, showing student teachers' and in-service teachers' resistance to telling children what is 'right or wrong' in science activities, thereby avoiding the position of an authoritarian presenter of science facts (Areljung et al., 2017;Sundberg and Ottander, 2013). Other studies show that even though teachers have intentions to steer practice towards science learning, these intentions are often overshadowed by their will to follow children's initiatives and ideas (Gustavsson et al., 2016;Westman and Bergmark, 2014). ...
... But if children are encouraged and supported to conduct investigations based on their own discoveries and questions, children may, as in Siry's (2013) example, gain more multifaceted experiences of the buoyancy concept. Areljung et al. (2017) give another example of productive boundary work in the preschool science arena by pointing out how educators, in their talk about their own preschool science practice, acknowledge a vast range of legitimate ways to learn about the material world, including, and combining, children's systematic observations, experiments, whole-body experiences, personal taste and imagination. One educator tells a story about when they brought paper and pencils to a forest to draw the trees. ...
... One educator tells a story about when they brought paper and pencils to a forest to draw the trees. The children noticed that there were no leaves on the trees, but then 'some of them decided to draw leaves anyway because it was prettier' (Areljung et al., 2017(Areljung et al., : 1186. From this educator's point of view, there did not seem to be much tension involved in the fact that the children's observations of what the trees looked like were blended with their individual taste of what looked pretty. ...
This article seeks to contribute new perspectives to the ontology and epistemology of preschool science education by exploring the idea of using everyday verbs, rather than nouns, to discern possibilities for science learning in preschool. Herein, the author merges empirical examples from preschools with findings from research on children’s noun and verb learning and posthumanist perspectives on matter and concepts. What comes out of the exploration is a radical way of viewing and knowing the world. The verbs trigger a shift from an object-oriented view of the world to seeing action and non-tangible processes and phenomena in one’s surroundings. Further, the verbs highlight the potential science learning that emerges in action and in child–matter relations, opening up to preschool science pedagogies that go beyond subjective/objective and concrete/abstract binaries.
Drawing on new materialist thought (Barad 2003, 2007, 2010, 2011, 2014), this study explores preschool children’s gendered becomings as they play and explore together with two large boulders in a natural setting. The study takes its point of departure in emergent science (Siraj-Blatchford 2001) which refers to science as a social practice that is already explored by children’s daily play. Data for the project was constructed during a field study in a Swedish preschool with 25 five-year old children. The focus of the field study was the children’s play and explorations together with the preschool environment, during activities not specifically guided by teachers. The findings show how also natural materials, as the boulders in this study, can take part in and shape, both the children’s scientific explorations and their gendered becomings. This means that the ways that a child managed to explore and become was dependent on if and how the child managed to co-act and play together with the boulders and friction. However, not only the boulders, but also gender norms functioned as co-creators in this process. This goes against the common thought of children’s play in nature as “free and equal”, a notion that is not often questioned. Studies exploring children’s play and learning outdoors often lack gender perspectives, especially when science is also in focus. One of the article’s conclusions is the need for preschool teachers to engage in how children explore emergent science together with the natural/ material-discursive surroundings. That is, how different natural materials co-act with gender norms in different ways, moment by moment, and thus enable or prevent children from engaging with science and scientific phenomena.
In 2011, a renewed national curriculum for preschools in Sweden introduced explicit pedagogical tasks concerning chemistry and physics. This article is about the analysis of physics teaching supported by video productions with tablets, part of a three-year professional development programme on collaborative inquiry teaching of chemistry and physics in Swedish preschools. The temporal case studies reported here were focused on children’s and teachers’ communication during extended teaching sequences with three- to six-year-old children in two Swedish preschools. Eleven children and two teachers participated in this study. The children worked in small groups with one teacher. Results indicate that children’s video productions by tablets contributed to children’s learning, with differences indicated for children’s experiences of objects of learning in physics at different levels of abstraction. Consequences of the results for future teaching of early years physics are discussed.
There is a tension in Swedish preschool policy when it comes to a subject curriculum and a child-centred curriculum. This article examines how Swedish preschool teachers have dealt with this relationship by focusing on the purpose of the preschool and how preschool teachers become part of the enacted preschool policy. The purpose of the study is to investigate Swedish preschool teachers’ policy talk pertaining to the preschool’s assignment to depart from children’s own interests and school-like subjects. The analysis of interviews with 10 preschool teachers shows how local policy talk is positioned in favour of a child-centred discourse, how tensions can gradually appear in the same sequence and how different actualizations in the national curriculum change the interviewees’ messages. The interviews highlight how enacted preschool policy appears as multi-layered and messy, thereby actualizing a discussion about the basic purpose of the Swedish preschool and its relation to school.
Drawing on new materialist thought (Barad, 2014), this study explores preschool children's gendered becomings as they play and explore together with two large boulders in a natural setting. the study takes its point of departure in emergent science (siraj-Blatchford 2001) which refers to science as a social practice that is already explored by children's daily play. Data for the project was constructed during a field study in a swedish preschool with 25 five-year old children. the focus of the field study was the children's play and explorations together with the preschool environment, during activities not specifically guided by teachers. the findings show how also natural materials, as the boulders in this study, can take part in and shape, both the children's scientific explorations and their gendered becomings. this means that the ways that a child managed to explore and become was dependent on if and how the child managed to co-act and play together with the boulders and friction. however, not only the boulders, but also gender norms functioned as co-creators in this process. this goes against the common thought of children's play in nature as "free and equal", a notion that is not often questioned. studies exploring children's play and learning outdoors often lack gender perspectives , especially when science is also in focus. One of the article's conclusions is the need for preschool teachers to engage in how children explore emergent science together with the natural/ material-discursive surroundings. that is, how different natural materials co-act with gender norms in different ways, moment by moment, and thus enable or prevent children from engaging with science and scientific phenomena.