Figure 2 - available via license: Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
Content may be subject to copyright.
Scores (in seconds) on the TMT-A and TMT-B and ratio scores on TMT-BA per participant group. Error bars represent the standard error.
Source publication
Background
Glioma (brain tumour) patients can suffer from mild linguistic and non-linguistic cognitive problems when the glioma is localised in an eloquent brain area. Word-finding problems are among the most frequently reported complaints. However, mild problems are difficult to measure with standard language tests because they are generally desig...
Context in source publication
Citations
... These tests are not always sensitive for mild language deficits (Brownsett et al., 2019;Davie et al., 2009;Satoer et al., 2012). In addition, despite the often absent objectified aphasia, LGG patients do report language complaints (Brownsett et al., 2019;Mooijman et al., 2022;Satoer et al., 2012) underlining the need for suitable tests. Standardized tests that are often described to be sensitive in LGG patients are verbal fluency (category and letter) and (rapid) naming (objects and actions) and are therefore recommended in a minimal test-battery to tap into the language domain ( (Papagno et al., 2012) (see also Rofes et al. (2017) for a European survey on cognitive assessment). ...
Brain tumours, which are classified as rare diseases, primarily include glioblastoma, with an annual occurrence of 300,000 cases. Unfortunately, this condition leads to the loss of 241,000 lives each year, highlighting its devastating nature. However, recent advancements in diagnosis and treatment have significantly improved the management and care of brain tumours. In this chapter, we will first provide an overview of the common types of primary brain tumours (gliomas and meningiomas). We will then delve into techniques for identifying and mapping tumours that impact language processing, utilizing both non-invasive and invasive methods. Lastly, we will discuss the effects of surgery on language and cognitive functions. The focus of this chapter is on tumours affecting language processing in the brain and the application of diffusion-weighted tractography to map the white matter language system.
... In recent years, the language abilities of people with brain tumours have received much attention (Mooijman et al., 2021;Rofes, Mandonnet, et al., 2017;Satoer et al., 2013). Tasks and assessments at the sentence and at the word level have been employed. ...
... Satoer et al. (2013) reported that during preoperative spontaneous speech individuals with brain tumours produced more incomplete sentences and had shorter mean length of utterance than a matched control group. However, even though people with brain tumours report word finding problems, single-word object naming tasks are not always sensitive enough to detect any preoperative difficulties (Mooijman et al., 2021). More importantly, the numbers of patients that are classified as 'impaired' seem to differ according to the linguistic modality and the linguistic stimuli used in each neurosurgical centre. ...
People with tumours in specific brain sites might face difficulties in tasks with different linguistic material. Previous lesion-symptom mapping studies (VLSM) demonstrated that people with tumours in posterior temporal regions have more severe linguistic impairments. However, to the best of our knowledge, preoperative performance and lesion location on tasks with different linguistic stimuli have not been examined. In the present study, we performed VLSM on 52 people with left gliomas to examine whether tumour distribution differs depending on the tasks of the Aachen Aphasia Test. The VLSM analysis revealed that single-word production (e.g. object naming) was associated with the inferior parietal lobe and that compound and sentence production were additionally associated with posterior temporal gyri. Word repetition was affected in people with tumours in inferior parietal areas, whereas sentence repetition was the only task to be associated with frontal regions. Subcortically, word and sentence production were found to be affected in people with tumours reaching the arcuate fasciculus, and compound production was primarily associated with tumours affecting the inferior longitudinal and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. Our work shows that tasks with linguistic stimuli other than single-word naming (e.g. compound and sentence production) relate to additional cortical and subcortical brain areas. At a clinical level, we show that tasks that target the same processes (e.g. repetition) can have different neural correlates depending on the linguistic stimuli used. Also, we highlight the importance of left temporoparietal areas.
... However, the outcomes can assist selection of variables in future studies. Language domains that appear most promising in relation to FC networks are word retrieval (as assessed with object naming), grammar, and comprehension of visual input when using a sufficiently sensitive test (e.g., combination of accuracy and speed, Mooijman et al., 2021) and to a lesser extent semantics and writing. FC network characteristics that appear most promising in relation to language functioning are MST degree, eccentricity and diameter in the theta band, and weighted small-world index in the alpha band. ...
Introduction: Preservation of language functioning in patients undergoing brain tumor surgery is essential because language impairments negatively impact the quality of life. Brain tumor patients have alterations in functional connectivity (FC), the extent to which brain areas functionally interact. We studied FC networks in relation to language functioning in glioma and meningioma patients.
Method: Patients with a low-grade glioma (N = 15) or meningioma (N = 10) infiltrating into/pressing on the language-dominant hemisphere underwent extensive language testing before and 1 year after surgery. Resting-state EEG was registered preoperatively, postoperatively (glioma patients only), and once in healthy individuals. After analyzing FC in theta and alpha frequency bands, weighted networks and Minimum Spanning Trees were quantified by various network measures.
Results: Pre-operative FC network characteristics did not differ between glioma patients and healthy individuals. However, hub presence and higher local and global FC are associated with poorer language functioning before surgery in glioma patients and predict worse language performance at 1 year after surgery. For meningioma patients, a greater small worldness was related to worse language performance and hub presence; better average clustering and global integration were predictive of worse outcome on language function 1 year after surgery. The average eccentricity, diameter and tree hierarchy seem to be the network metrics with the more pronounced relation to language performance.
Discussion: In this exploratory study, we demonstrated that preoperative FC networks are informative for pre- and postoperative language functioning in glioma patients and to a lesser extent in meningioma patients.
Background:
Patients with glioma often report language complaints with devastating effect on daily life. Analysing spontaneous speech can help to understand underlying language problems. Spontaneous speech monitoring is also of importance during awake brain surgery: it can guide tumour resection and contributes to maintaining language function. We aimed to investigate the spontaneous speech of patients with glioma in the perioperative period and the additional value of spontaneous speech analyses compared to standardised language testing.
Methods:
We elicited and transcribed spontaneous speech of eight patients with glioma elected for awake brain surgery preoperatively, intraoperatively and 2.0-3.5 months postoperatively. Linguistic errors were coded. Type Token Ratio, Mean Length of Utterance of words, minimal utterances, and errors were extracted from the transcriptions. Patients were categorised based on total error patterns: stable, decrease or increase during surgery. Reliable Change Index scores were calculated for all spontaneous speech variables to objectify changes between time points. Language performance on language tests was compared to spontaneous speech variables.
Results:
Most errors occurred in lexico-syntax, followed by phonology/articulation, syntax, and semantics. The predominant errors were Repetitions, Self-corrections, and Incomplete sentences. Most patients remained stable over time in almost all spontaneous speech variables, except in Incomplete sentences, which deteriorated in most patients postoperatively compared to intraoperatively. Some spontaneous speech variables (total errors, MLUw, TTR) gave more information on language change than a standard language test.
Conclusions:
While the course of spontaneous speech over time remained relatively stable in most patients, Incomplete sentences seems to be a robust marker of language difficulties patients with glioma. These errors can be prioritised in spontaneous speech analysis to save time, especially to determine intra- to postoperative deterioration. Importantly, spontaneous speech analyses can give more information on language change than standardised language testing and should therefore be used in addition to standardised language tests.
Purpose:
To investigate the course of fatigue, subjective and objective language functions in patients with lower-grade gliomas during the first year of disease. Further, to examine if subjective and objective language variables predicted ratings of fatigue.
Methods:
Fatigue was assessed with the Fatigue Severity Scale, subjective language with self-reported word-finding, expression of thoughts, reading and writing from the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy - Brain, and objective language with standardized tests. Mixed models were used to investigate changes in variables over time and predictors of fatigue.
Results:
Twenty-three patients with gliomas (WHO 1-3) were included. Average ratings of fatigue did not change significantly, but altering patterns were observed. Subjective concerns about word-finding and expression of thoughts increased significantly during follow-up. The regression analyses showed that concerns about abilities to read and write significantly predicted fatigue. The results indicated that less concerns about reading and writing were associated with lower levels of fatigue.
Conclusions:
Patients with lower-grade gliomas report fatigue and language concerns throughout the first year. Concerns about reading and writing call for careful consideration as they seem to provoke fatigue. The findings underline the importance of the patients' perspective in treatment and follow-up.
Impaired lexical retrieval is common in persons with low-grade glioma (LGG). Several studies have reported a discrepancy between subjective word-finding difficulties and results on formal tests. Analysis of spontaneous speech might be more sensitive to signs of word-finding difficulties, hence we aimed to explore disfluencies in a spontaneous-speech task performed by participants with presumed LGG before and after surgery. Further, we wanted to explore how the presence of disfluencies in spontaneous speech differed in the participants with and without objectively established lexical-retrieval impairment and with and without self-reported subjective experience of impaired language, speech and communication. Speech samples of 26 persons with presumed low-grade glioma were analysed with regard to disfluency features. The post-operative speech samples had a higher occurrence of fillers, implying more disfluent language production. The participants performed worse on two of the word fluency tests, and after surgery the number of participants who were assessed as having an impaired lexical retrieval had increased from 6 to 12. The number of participants who experienced a change in their language, speech or communication had increased from 9 to 12. Additional comparisons showed that those with impaired lexical retrieval had a higher proportion of false starts after surgery than those with normal lexical retrieval, and differences in articulation rate and speech rate, favouring those not having experienced any change in language, speech or communication. Taken together, the findings from this study strengthen the existing claim that temporal aspects of language and speech are important when assessing persons with gliomas.