Table 14 - uploaded by Camilla Power
Content may be subject to copyright.
Source publication
This book is the first to focus on the African origins of human language. It explores the origins of language and culture 250,000-150,000 years ago when modern humans evolved in Africa. Scholars from around the world address the fossil, genetic, and archaeological evidence and critically examine the ways it has been interpreted. The book also consi...
Contexts in source publication
Context 1
... two possible sexual selection models of male vs. female ritualized coalitions produce diametrically opposed expectations of the signaling system that emerges (Table 14.1). Even if red cosmetic display were favored psychologically in male contests (see Hill and Barton 2005), the prediction here is that such display is necessarily associated with exagger- atedly male characteristics, and certainly not with any reverse sexual signaling. ...
Context 2
... FCC model generates a range of predictions across a wide Weld of disciplines and is potentially testable against evidence from the archeo- logical record, the fossil record, and, importantly, symbolic evidence drawn from the ethnographic record of hunter-gatherer myth and ritual (Table 14.2). ...
Similar publications
This book is the first to focus on the African origins of human language. It explores the origins of language and culture 250,000-150,000 years ago when modern humans evolved in Africa. Scholars from around the world address the fossil, genetic, and archaeological evidence and critically examine the ways it has been interpreted. The book also consi...
Citations
... Finally, it meant finding new ways of dealing with menstruation which, with ovulation effectively concealed, had become salient as a cue to imminent fertility. The eventual solution involved the use of cosmetic substitutes to prevent real menstrual blood from triggering dangerous levels of inter-and intra-sexual competition and conflict (Power 2009(Power , 2010(Power , 2014Power and Aiello 1997). Against this background, we attribute the metaphors and equivalences of ekila and its cross-cultural variants in the first instance to women's collective action in their own reproductive interests (see Finnegan, this volume). ...
... 108-127) even argued that some basic aspects of symbolic culture are already present in wild chimpanzees. Several theorists hypothesize that group ritualization played a key role in the emergence of fully symbolic culture by establishing and stabilizing the necessary relationships of trust between signalers and receivers and generating a shared domain of meaning (Deacon, 1997;Durkheim, 1912;Henrich, 2009;Knight, 1999Knight, , 2014Power, 2009Power, , 2014Rappaport, 1999). If a large proportion of the red ochre from the African MSA does indeed manifest past ritual behavior, then it might represent a material remain of this very critical transitional process in human evolution, one which allows us to glimpse the gradual emergence of symbolic material culture. ...
... From an evolutionary perspective, the human collective ritual is a special mode of behavior composed of different psychologically active building blocks, some older, some younger. Like other theorists (Deacon, 1997;Dissanayake, 2018;Power, 2009Power, , 2014Rossano, 2012Rossano, , 2016, we assume that the oldest components are traceable to non-symbolic ritualization and costly signaling. These behaviors are observable in many non-human species today (Huxley, 1966;Krebs & Dawkins, 1991;Maynard-Smith & Harper, 2003;Zahavi & Zahavi, 1997). ...
... If it is true that trustenhancing and meaning-generating collective rituals were needed to establish the first jointly shared fictions (i.e., symbolism) by solving the cooperative dilemma, then the emergence of habitual collective rituals was one important prerequisite for the evolution of symbolic communication (Deacon, 1997, pp. 402-407;Durkheim, 1912;Power, 2009;Rappaport, 1999, pp. 54-56;Rossano, 2016;Watts, 2009). ...
Over the last two decades, red ochre has played a pivotal role in discussions about the cognitive and cultural evolution of early modern humans during the African Middle Stone Age. Given the importance of ochre for the scholarly debate about the emergence of ‘behavioral modernity’, the lack of long-term spatio-temporal analyses spanning large geographical areas represents a significant gap in knowledge. Here we take a continent-wide approach, rather than focusing on specific sites, regions or technocomplexes. We report the most comprehensive meta-analysis of ochre use to date, spanning Africa between 500 and 40 thousand years ago, to examine data from more than a hundred archaeological sites. Using methods based on time averaging, we identified three distinct phases of ochre use: the initial phase occurred from 500,000 to 330,000; the emergent phase from 330,000 to 160,000; and the habitual phase from 160,000 to 40,000 years ago. The number of sites with ochre increased with each subsequent phase. More importantly, the ratio of sites with ochre compared to those with only stone artifacts also followed this trend, indicating the increasing intensity of ochre use during the Middle Stone Age. While the geographical distribution expanded with time, the absolute number of ochre finds grew significantly as well, underlining the intensification of ochre use. We determine that ochre use established itself as a habitual cultural practice in southern, eastern and northern Africa starting about 160,000 years ago, when a third of archaeological sites contain ochre. We argue that this pattern is a likely material manifestation of intensifying ritual activity in early populations of Homo sapiens . Such ritual behavior may have facilitated the demographic expansion of early modern humans, first within and eventually beyond the African continent. We discuss the implications of our findings on two models of ritual evolution, the Female Cosmetic Coalitions Hypothesis and the Ecological Stress Hypothesis, as well as a model about the emergence of complex cultural capacities, the Eight-Grade Model for the Evolution and Expansion of Cultural Capacities .
... Females within coalitions would begin to use blood-coloured substances as cosmetics to augment their signals. This is the Female Cosmetic Coalitions model of the origins of art and symbolic culture (Power 2009;Power, Sommer and Watts 2013). ...
Lionel Sims’ work has illuminated how Neolithic ritual communities ‘solarised’ the moon, deceptively transforming a lunar syntax into a solar one. But where did the ‘time-resistant’ lunar syntax come from? It is unlikely that patriarchal Neolithic societies invented this form of time-keeping. Yet it persists even in modern patriarchal ‘world’ religions derived from Neolithic forebears.
Marx said ‘All forms of economics can be reduced to an economics of time'. How a society organises time reveals what it truly values. The question of the earliest human economy cannot be solved without a focus on women, the moon and menstruation. African hunter-gatherer cosmology takes the lunar cycle as the crucial timeframe for ritual, sex and economic activities. The shared sources of this cosmology carry us back to earliest human symbolic culture, the very origins of art and ritual itself, over 100,000 years ago.
Contrary to presumed Neolithic gender relations, these hunter-gatherer societies are among the most gender egalitarian on earth. But how does such egalitarianism work? Women especially assert power through their bodies collectively to resist any threat of male exploitation. As the moon waxes and wanes, the dynamic of power switches in more or less playful battles between the sexes. Rather than patriarchy or matriarchy, we observe lunarchy – rule by the moon, expressed in a pulse of waxing and waning, ritual power ON, ritual power OFF.
... Gradualist models have a long pedigree (Darwin 1871), but placed in the time frame of Chomsky's influence, a variety of approaches have emerged that vary in emphasis on the biological or cultural factors influencing the origin of language, and in their interpretation of the archaeological record (e.g. Donald 1991;Dunbar 1996;Noble and Davidson 1996;Mithen 1996;Power 2009;Corballis 2002;Bickerton 1990, Bickerton 2014Coolidge and Wynn 2009;Rossano 2010;Lombard and Gärdenfors 2017). Deacon (1989;, Cousins (2014) and Everett(2017) stand apart from other gradualists in using Peirce's theory of signs. ...
This paper argues that the origins of language can be detected one million years ago, if not earlier, in the archaeological record of Homo erectus. This controversial claim is based on a broad theoretical and evidential foundation with language defined as communication based on symbols rather than grammar. Peirce’s theory of signs (semiotics) underpins our analysis with its progression of signs (icon, index and symbol) used to identify artefact forms operating at the level of symbols. We draw on generalisations about the multiple social roles of technology in pre-industrial societies and on the contexts tool-use among non-human primates to argue for a deep evolutionary foundation for hominin symbol use. We conclude that symbol-based language is expressed materially in arbitrary social conventions that permeate the technologies of Homo erectus and its descendants, and in the extended planning involved in the caching of tools and in the early settlement of island Southeast Asia.
... The Female Cosmetic Coalitions (FCC) model (Power, 2009;Power et al., 2013) fills out this reverse dominance dynamic and makes specific predictions about the type of signal ing that would arise. It focuses on female collective strategic resistance to dominant males who refuse to invest. ...
... Following the logic of concealing ovulation, they might try to hide the menstruant's condition so that males would not know. But because the signal has economic value in attracting male attention, rather than hide it, we predict females will do the opposite-flaunt it (Knight et al., 1995;Power, 2009). Whenever a coalition member menstruated, the whole coalition joined in advertising and amplifying the signal to attract outsider males and their labor to the coalition. ...
This handbook is currently in development, with individual articles publishing online in advance of print publication. At this time, we cannot add information about unpublished articles in this handbook, however the table of contents will continue to grow as additional articles pass through the review process and are added to the site. Please note that the online publication date for this handbook is the date that the first article in the title was published online. For more information, please read the site FAQs.
... The bulk of social anthropologists and a good many evolutionary anthropologists concur that food sharing is a form of intentional consumption leveling integrating a larger set of intentional leveling strategies that allow the continued existence of hunter-gatherer egalitarian political life by keeping in check the accumulation of prestige and social influence that could disrupt the balance of power between individuals and families (Boehm, 1999(Boehm, , 2012Fried, 1967;Lee, 1988;. This interpretation of food sharing as an intentional leveling device is also supported by a diverse set of evolutionary models that focus on coalitionary strategies (Boyd, Gintis, & Bowles, 2010;Gavrilets, 2012;Power, 2009) and cultural adaptation (Richerson & Boyd, 2005) as drivers of social conformity. Last but not least, it sits well among recent research exploring singular aspects of human experience like social learning (Boyd, Richerson, & Henrich, 2011), derived social structure (Hill et al., 2011), internalization of norms (Gavrilets & Richerson, 2017), and altruistic social preferences (Silk & House, 2016). ...
... The bulk of social anthropologists and a good many evolutionary anthropologists concur that food sharing is a form of intentional consumption leveling integrating a larger set of intentional leveling strategies that allow the continued existence of hunter-gatherer egalitarian political life by keeping in check the accumulation of prestige and social influence that could disrupt the balance of power between individuals and families (Boehm, 1999(Boehm, , 2012Endicott, 1988;Fried, 1967;Lee, 1988;Wiessner, 1996;Woodburn, 1982). This interpretation of food sharing as an intentional leveling device is also supported by a diverse set of evolutionary models that focus on coalitionary strategies (Boyd, Gintis, & Bowles, 2010;Gavrilets, 2012;Power, 2009) and cultural adaptation (Richerson & Boyd, 2005) as drivers of social conformity. Last but not least, it sits well among recent research exploring singular aspects of human experience like social learning (Boyd, Richerson, & Henrich, 2011), derived social structure (Hill et al., 2011), internalization of norms (Gavrilets & Richerson, 2017), and altruistic social preferences (Silk & House, 2016). ...
... [on-line:] https:// takie jej przejawy jak np. szympans próbujący pomóc odlecieć zranionemu ptakowi 228 . ...
... Do 26 grudnia 2016 r. ten niesamowity film obejrzało ponad 78 mln internautów. 228 "Pewnego dnia Kumi złapała szpaka. W obawie, że może zamęczyć ogłuszonego ptaka (…) opiekun polecił małpie wypuścić zdobycz (…). ...
... Zadziwia różnica w intensyfikacji użycia barwników takich jak ochra, gdy porównujemy znaleziska afrykańskie i europejskie. Wydaje się, że w Afryce powszechne użycie ochry pojawia się wcześ niej 228 . Pamiętajmy jednak o różnicach w sposobie przechowania się szczątków aktywności ludzkiej w zależności od uwarunkowań klimatycznych. ...
The main premise of this work is highlighting how old the human race
actually is. Its origins, as the Author believes, are constantly shifting deeper into
history, reaching now millions of years’ distance.
Our position in relation to animals makes us unique beings, if only because
it is solely us who can meditate on this subject. One should agree that evolution
of our kind is a biological process, but talking about it is exclusively a product of
our culture.
The Author is aware that this book asks more questions than it answers.
This is what it must be at the current level of our knowledge. The dynamic of
archeological, paleontological and genetic discoveries is simply too great at
this point in the 21st century. In reference to the possible criticism that the
Author treats many of the findings presented in this book as hypotheses (and
even working hypotheses), the Author must point out that in his opinion it is
impossible to do otherwise.
We have been on this planet for a very long time, at least several million
years. The concepts of separate species of Homo which appear in academic
discourse are rather vague. The crossbreeding of different kinds of proto-humans
points rather to the community of humanity of these creatures rather than to the
unbridgeable gaps between them.
This phenomenon has various biological and cultural consequences. One
must be aware that such creations of the human spirit, perhaps supported by
264 ABSTRACT
natural abilities, such as morality and, consequently, ethics and the foundations
of the law, are very old. They are as old as we are. This is the fact that the Author
wants to highlight in this book.
Spirituality (symbolic culture), and in general - culture forming social
structures specific for humans, even if not originating in our minds and being only
a manifestation of biological evolution (although it has not been convincingly
explained how this happens solely on the biological path), must still be stored
in our minds. Most likely, they are or at some point become emergent entities in
relation to the underlying factors. They form a new human quality.
Culture, including legal culture, must have human mental space appropriate
for its impact. It is within this culture that validation decisions are made with
respect to the origin of legal norms as such (the origin of natural law) and the
legitimacy of its application in a given case (the impact of primitive law). This
publication makes an attempt to present the research area which appears under
the influence of the achievements of the Stone Age archeology, exploring the
beginnings of our kind.
The ambitious goal of this work is showing the stage on which these
phenomena, extremely important for our existence, were able to occur both
in time, in physical space, as well as in the mental space of our ancestors. This
attempt is an archaeologist’s look at pra-culture with an emphasis on the moment
when law may appear as a product of the human condition, which may be as old
as the condition itself. It is widely known that the older something is, the more
attractive it is to archaeologists.
The work includes numerous references to research on contemporary
primates, which in the face of rapid changes of the world are under threat in their
forest habitats. We must protect them! We are responsible for animals, as well
as for the whole nature. What is more, taking care of nature, we also take care
of ourselves. Protecting the environment is one of our most important tasks at
present. Understanding our responsibility for the world and actively participating
in its protection, in the Author’s opinion, makes much more sense than trying to
equate people with animals.
Key words:
human evolution, The Stone Age archeology, begginings of primitive law
... So, the occasion for reverse dominant collective -moral -action happened whenever a prospective alpha male tried to abduct a potentially fertile female. One model for the emergence of symbolism fits this reverse dominance dynamic and makes specific predictions about the signaling that would arise: the Female Cosmetic Coalitions (FCC) model (Power & Aiello, 1997;Power, 2009;Power et al., 2013). ...
... Following the logic of concealed ovulation, they might try to hide the menstruant's condition so that males would not know. But because the signal has potential economic value by attracting male attention, rather than hiding it, females should do the opposite: make a big display out of it ( Knight et al., 1995;Power, 2009). Whenever a coalition member menstruated, the whole coalition should join in amplifying the signal to attract males and their labor to the coalition. ...
... The process of sexual selection whereby choosy investing males discriminate as to which female to invest in will involve ritualized display by coalitions of related females -the reverse of the usual sexual selection dynamic in mammals and birds (Power, 2009). For the first time in evolution, such a sexually selected display will reference a collectively imagined construct, the presence of "blood," "fertility," or "potency" signaled by red cosmetics. ...
Are there constraints on the social conditions that could have given rise to language
and symbolic cognition? Language has emerged in no other species than humans,
suggesting a profound obstacle to its evolution. If language is seen as an aspect of
cognition, limitations can be expected in terms of computational capacity. But if it
is seen it as fundamentally for communication, then the problems will be found in
terms of social relationships. Below a certain threshold of cooperation and trust, no
language or symbolic communication could evolve (Knight & Lewis, 2017a); this
has been termed a “platform of trust” (Wacewicz, 2017)....
In this chapter, I argue that quite specific social conditions were prerequisite for the evolution of language- and symbol-ready hominins. One of the requirements differentiating our ancestors from other African apes was a switch to mainly female philopatry – females living with their relatives, rather than dispersing at sexual maturity – coevolving with an increasing tendency to egalitarianism....How did increasing egalitarianism affect males and potentially “feminize” male behavior for cooperative offspring care? How were male and female relations affected in the evolution of genus Homo and Homo sapiens?
... Nonetheless, I agree with Chris Knight (2014) in that Boehm's approach in understanding egalitarian relations in humans lacks the role of gender in reverse dominant strategies. As theorised by evolutionary anthropologist Camilla Power, egalitarianism of hunting and gathering societies emerged as a result of female coalitions and their reproductive strategies(Power 2009(Power , 2014a(Power , 2014b(Power , 2014c. ...
This thesis is based on ethnographic research conducted with Mbendjele BaYaka Pygmy hunter-gatherers of Likouala Region, Congo-Brazzaville for eighteen months from 2013 to 2015. The primary goals of this thesis are: (1) to present three key contexts for educating children about Mbendjele practices and values; (2) to analyse ethnographic observations of how these contexts are employed to distinguish the modes of education they exploit; (3) to contrast Mbendjele and outsider-imposed education methods, and how Mbendjele define proper and improper teaching and learning. Mbendjele BaYaka value three main pro-egalitarian, cultural institutions as the primary means of educating children. They are based on public speaking, ridicule and play. I will examine how these institutions are employed in practice with a discussion of content and context. The results indicate that Mbendjele value mostly transmission of pro-egalitarian values, shaping understanding of gender and sexual roles in children, and teaching ways to deal with Non-Mbendjele outsiders. Corporal punishment is rare amongst egalitarian hunter-gatherers. Despite Mbendjele perceiving of it as an improper way of disciplining children, it is often employed in sedentarized context, in conjunction with increasing domestic violence and alcoholism. Indigenous institutions for cultural reproduction are central to understanding how hunter-gatherer picture their own future. Despite good intentions foreign enforcement of institutional schooling can have negative affects on the cultural resilience of Mbendjele sociality and egalitarian values. Understanding how Mbendjele value outsider imposed and their indigenous education institutions contributes to a better understanding of cultural resilience among marginalised ethnic groups, such as Mbendjele.