Figure 1 - uploaded by Samuel D Gosling
Content may be subject to copyright.
Means for overall Conscientiousness (A) and its facets (B), by age and gender. Single lines show the means for males, and double lines show the means for females. In Panel B, black lines show the means for Self-Discipline, and or gray lines show the means for Order. 

Means for overall Conscientiousness (A) and its facets (B), by age and gender. Single lines show the means for males, and double lines show the means for females. In Panel B, black lines show the means for Self-Discipline, and or gray lines show the means for Order. 

Source publication
Article
Full-text available
Hypotheses about mean-level age differences in the Big Five personality domains, as well as 10 more specific facet traits within those domains, were tested in a very large cross-sectional sample (N = 1,267,218) of children, adolescents, and adults (ages 10-65) assessed over the World Wide Web. The results supported several conclusions. First, late...

Context in source publication

Context 1
... the BFI domain and facet scales were somewhat higher at older ages. Specifically, the mean reliability of the domain scales showed a positive age trend from .75 at age 10 to .83 at age 18, and was either .83 or .84 in each of the older groups. Similarly, the mean reliability of the shorter facet scales showed a positive trend from .54 at age 10 to .67 at age 18, and was between .67 and .69 in each older group. Correlations among the domain scales also differed somewhat by age. Specifically, the mean magnitude of the interscale correlations showed a negative age trend from .22 at age 10 to .16 at age 18, then a positive trend to .26 at ages 61– 65. Despite these age differences in reliability and interscale correlations, the BFI’s intended five-dimensional structure was clearly recovered in principal-components analyses conducted in each age group. In each group, a scree test indicated five meaningful dimensions, and five components were therefore extracted and varimax-rotated. Congruence coefficients were then computed to compare each component in each age group with the corresponding component in the age-21-to-25 group; this was selected as the reference group because the BFI’s intended five-dimensional structure emerged very clearly in it, as in previous studies of emerging-adult samples (e.g., Benet-Mart ́nez & John, 1998). These congruence coefficients averaged .99, and all 100 were at least .96, easily exceeding the standard of .90 that indicates factor replication (Barrett, 1986; Mulaik, 1972). Taken together, these results indicate that the overall structure of the BFI was very consistent across late childhood, adolescence, and adulthood, although there was greater measurement error at younger ages. Presentation of results. Preliminary analyses indicated that (a) almost all of the domain and facet traits assessed by the BFI showed substantial mean-level age differences, (b) some traits showed age trends that differed considerably by gender, and (c) the shapes of some trends were curvilinear and quite complex. These trends could not be closely fit by regression models, even models that included higher order polynomial age terms (quadratic, cubic, quartic, quintic, etc.) and corresponding Age ϫ Gender interaction terms. These preliminary analyses also indicated that the size of the present sample rendered conventional tests of statistical sig- nificance meaningless; for example, in the full sample, a correlation of .003 between two variables would be statistically significant at the ␣ ϭ .001 level, and a correlation of .02 would be significant at the ␣ ϭ 1 ϫ 10 Ϫ 100 level. We therefore present our results with a focus on the patterns and sizes of age and gender differences. In describing these patterns, we refer to “age trends” and “age differences”—rather than developmental “changes,” “increases,” or “decreases”— due to the present research’s cross-sectional design. For the same reason, note that we use prepositional phrases such as “across middle age,” “by late adolescence,” and “from adolescence into adulthood” to refer to cross-sectional age ranges, rather than periods of development over time. We use the T -score metric to index effect sizes; T scores are standard scores with a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10. In terms of Cohen’s (1988) now conventional guidelines for inter- preting effect sizes, a difference of 2 T- score points represents a small effect, a difference of 5 points represents a medium effect, and a difference of 8 points represents a large effect. To control for age and gender effects when converting to T scores, we computed the overall mean of each BFI domain and facet scale by first computing its mean in each of the 112 age- and gender-specific samples (56 years of age ϫ 2 genders) and then averaging these 112 group means. Similarly, we computed the overall standard deviation of each scale as the square root of the pooled within- group variance term from a two-way (56 years of age ϫ 2 genders) analysis of variance—an estimate that controls for between-group variance. This process produced a T -score distribution uninflu- enced by the age and gender differences that are the subject of the present research. In the present sample, all pairwise age differences of at least 2 T -score points were statistically significant at the ␣ ϭ .00001 level. Mean scores on the BFI domain and facet scales, by age and gender, are shown in Figures 1–5. We calculated these group means separately for each individual combination of age and gender (age-10 males, age-10 females, age-11 males, age-11 fe- males, etc.); no smoothing functions have been applied. In each figure, single lines show the means for males, and double lines show the means for females. In Figures 1B, 2B, 3B, 4B, and 5B, black lines show the means for one facet of a domain, and gray lines show the means for the other facet. Conscientiousness. Mean levels of Order, Self-Discipline, and overall Conscientiousness are shown in Figure 1. As hypothesized, overall Conscientiousness showed very different age trends in late childhood and adolescence versus adulthood. Specifically, as shown in Figure 1A, Conscientiousness showed a negative age trend from late childhood into adolescence; the total difference was approximately 3 T -score points, or one third of a standard deviation unit. Conscientiousness then showed a pronounced positive trend from adolescence through emerging adulthood, with a total difference of approximately 7 T -score points. This trend was even more pronounced for females than for males, such that by emerging adulthood females were slightly more conscientious, on average, than were males (by approximately 2 T -score points). At the facet level, Figure 1B illustrates that Self-Discipline and Order showed age trends across these years similar to overall Conscientiousness, with negative trends from late childhood into adolescence (with total differences of 2 or 3 T -score points), then pronounced positive trends through emerging adulthood (with total differences of 5 or 6 T -score points). Gender differences in these two facets, however, related differently with age. Like overall Conscientiousness, a small gender difference in Self-Discipline was first present in emerging adulthood. In contrast, females were more orderly than males, on average, at each age from 10 to 65. Overall Conscientiousness showed a further, although less pronounced, positive trend across early adulthood and middle age, with age differences of approximately 5 more T -score points for both males and females. At the facet level, as hypothesized, these differences were substantial for Self-Discipline (approximately 6 T -score points), but only trivial for Order (approximately 1 T -score point). Altogether, the total age differences from adolescence through middle age were approximately 11 T -score points for overall Conscientiousness and Self-Discipline. These are very large cross-sectional age effects that equal the difference between scores at the 50th and 86th percentiles of a normal distribution. The total age difference for Order was approximately 7 T -score points, a substantial effect that equals the difference between scores at the 50th and 76th percentiles of a normal distribution. Agreeableness. Mean levels of Altruism, Compliance, and overall Agreeableness are shown in Figure 2. As hypothesized, overall Agreeableness showed age trends similar to Conscientiousness, although the trends for Agreeableness were somewhat less pronounced. As shown in Figure 2A, Agreeableness showed a negative trend from late childhood into adolescence (with a total difference of approximately 2 T -score points), a positive trend from adolescence into emerging adulthood (with a total difference of approximately 3 T -score points), and a further positive trend across early adulthood and middle age (with a difference of approximately 3 more T -score points). The total difference of 6 T -score points from adolescence through middle age represents a substantial age effect, equal to the difference between scores at the 50th and 73rd percentiles of a normal distribution. At the facet level, Figure 2B illustrates that Altruism and Compliance showed age trends similar to overall Agreeableness, with only one qualification: Compliance did not show a negative trend from late childhood into adolescence for males. Instead, males showed low levels of Compliance even at age 10, a finding unlikely to surprise elementary-school teachers. Regarding gender differences more generally, at each age females were somewhat more agreeable, altruistic, and compliant, on average, than were males (by approximately 2 T -score points). Neuroticism. Mean levels of Anxiety, Depression, and overall Neuroticism are shown in Figure 3. There were only trivial gender differences in these traits at age 10 (less than 1 T -score point), but, as hypothesized, there were very different age trends for males versus females across late childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. For females, Anxiety and overall Neuroticism showed positive trends into adolescence (with total differences of approximately 3 T -score points), flat trends through emerging adulthood, and then negative trends across early adulthood and middle age (with total differences of approximately 5 T -score points). In contrast, males’ mean levels of Anxiety and overall Neuroticism showed slightly negative trends from late childhood through middle age (with total differences of approximately 2 T -score points). Substantial gender differences in both of these traits (of approximately 5 T -score points) were present by midadolescence, such that females were more prone to anxiety and other negative emotions, on average, than were males. The magnitudes of these gender differences diminished across early adulthood and middle age, although a small difference (of approximately 2 T -score points) was present even at the end of middle age. Age trends ...

Similar publications

Article
Full-text available
The article presents the authors' conceptions regarding value orientations and psychosocial adaptation of personality; there are presented some results obtained in the carried out research with reference to the impact of value orientations on psychosocial adaptation in adolescence. There are briefly presented both, levels of manifestation of psycho...
Article
Full-text available
The current study examined mean level and change in extraversion and neuroticism across adolescence in relation to physiological stress reactivity to social evaluation. Adolescents (n = 327) from the Dutch general population reported on personality measures at five annual assessments. At age 17 years, adolescents participated in a psychosocial stre...

Citations

... This result is consistent with the body of prior research showing that socio-affective and motivational variables drop during the school ages examined in the present study (Gaspard et al., 2020;Orth et al., 2021;Postigo et al., 2022;Scherrer & Preckel, 2019). Age cohort differences could also be partly explained by children's inflated self-views or the higher uncertainty of the younger cohort measures, which typically have greater measurement error (Soto et al., 2011). Yet, teachers and parents report of students' social-emotional tend to confirm these declines in social and emotional skills (Kankaraš & Suárez-Álvarez, 2019;OECD, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The impact of grade repetition on social-emotional skills is well-researched but somewhat inconclusive. Evidence suggests that lower competitive groups can boost self-concept through social comparison. Our main hypothesis is that measuring self-concept just before students are notified of repetition may capture un-intended effects, leading to a "rebound effect". The main objective is to compare the evolution of self-concept and grit between students who were promoted and those who began repeating in 6th grade using matched samples. Method: The sample comprised 5999 students (49 % girls) who were assessed at two-time points, 4th and 8th grade, with mean ages of 9.78 years (SD = 0.29) and 13.91 years (SD = 0.44), respectively. The first assessment was conducted two years before the first possible grade repetition to avoid the impact on self-concept and grit from the threat of repetition. To control selection bias, the repeater group (n = 762) was matched with a promoted group using propensity score matching, including sociodemographic predictors, prior achievement, and pre-repetition. The potential adverse impact of repetition was estimated using a two-level hierarchical-linear-analysis. Results: Adolescents experienced a clear drop in self-concept and grit over time. However, after accounting for sociodemographic differences, prior achievement, and school-level variables, the drop is significantly larger for grade repeaters compared to those who have been promoted. Moreover, grade repetition at later stages is associated with a more adverse impact. Conclusions: Understanding the impact of grade repetition before students face the threat of repeating can help educators and policymakers develop strategies to prevent negative consequences for at-risk students.
... Although it has been established that maintaining trait curiosity throughout the lifespan can be beneficial for a variety of outcomes [5,6,14], the evidence regarding the relationship between chronological age and trait curiosity is somewhat mixed. Some evidence suggests that older age groups have higher trait curiosity, openness, or novelty seeking than younger age groups [19,20], though in some cases, these patterns show increases followed by declines or lack of sufficient data into older age (i.e., over 65 years of age) [21]. In line with this evidence, a comprehensive meta-analysis demonstrated that many personality traits become more stable across the lifespan [22], which may explain a plateau in trait curiosity in older age. ...
Article
Full-text available
Maintaining curiosity in older age may be a key predictor of successful aging, but prior research on the relationship between curiosity and age is mixed, with mounting evidence showing that curiosity declines with age. However, there is evidence suggesting that state curiosity – a situational feeling of curiosity in response to information – may increase with age. Prior work has largely not adequately differentiated state and trait curiosity when examining its relationship with age. In a large lifespan sample (pilot study N = 193; preregistered main study N = 1,218), we assess trait curiosity and state curiosity (using a trivia rating task) to examine the relationship between each construct and age. The results show that, in line with prior work, trait curiosity shows a negative relationship with age, but state curiosity shows a positive relationship with age, while controlling for demographic variables. The results suggest that curiosity may have a more complex relationship with age than previously considered, which can have implications for engagement in cognitive activities in everyday life.
... All three MIMIC models and their fully standardized solutions were estimated via maximum-likelihood estimation and contained the same exogenous predictors (i.e., age, sex, and two dummy variables-MTD vs. controls and PVH vs. controls). 5 Age and sex were included as covariates to control for known personality differences across the life span and by biological sex (Caspi et al., 2005;Roberts et al., 2006Roberts et al., , 2008Roberts & Delvecchio, 2000;Soto et al., 2011;Weisberg et al., 2011). 6 5 A second set of three MIMIC models was also conducted but used the MTD group as the comparison for the group differences. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Voice disorders associated with vocal hyperfunction are some of the most common vocal pathologies. Certain personality traits are thought to be a risk factor for developing these disorders. The trait theory of voice disorders (TTVD) provided a unified framework to understand these relationships. This study re-examines the TTVD by adopting current theories and methods from personality science. Method This cross-sectional study recruited individuals diagnosed with primary muscle tension dysphonia (MTD) or a diagnosis associated with phonotraumatic vocal hyperfunction (PVH), that is, benign bilateral lesions of the lamina propria and bilateral or unilateral vocal fold polyp(s). Participants completed a contemporary personality battery. Data were analyzed via structural equation modeling and compared to vocally healthy controls. Results Several significant differences existed between vocally healthy controls (n = 416) and participants with MTD (n = 71) or PVH (n = 38). Compared to the controls, participants with MTD reported lower levels of Stability, Plasticity, Conscientiousness, Openness/Intellect, and the corresponding aspects of Industriousness and Intellect, respectively. Conversely, the MTD cohort was significantly higher in Neuroticism and its two corresponding aspects, Withdrawal and Volatility. Likewise, when compared to controls, the PVH reported significantly lower levels of Stability, Agreeableness, its aspect Politeness, and the aspect of Industriousness. Finally, compared to the MTD cohort, individuals with PVH were higher on Extraversion, specifically the aspect of Assertiveness, and lower on the aspect of Politeness. Conclusions The current study largely replicates the initial TTVD studies and updates them with a modern theory of personality. These results provide a strong foundation for future investigations to continue to study the relationships between personality traits and voice disorders. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28843052
... Результаты исследований указывают на то, что, например, девочки обладают более высокими показателями нейротизма в подростковом возрасте по сравнению с мальчиками. Причем в данном возрастном периоде у девочек происходит пик нейротизма, если рассматривать в сравнении со всеми возрастными периодами (Soto et al., 2011). Также девочки обладают более высокими эмоциональными способностями, в то время как мальчики обладают более высокой эмоциональной самооценкой (D'Amico & Geraci, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Введение. На современном этапе развития системы образования вопросы формирования и сохранения эмоционального благополучия у школьников приобретают особую значимость. В фокусе данной статьи находится определение структуры эмоционального благополучия школьника и выделение особенностей взаимосвязи его компонентов, в том числе и отдельно по группам – мальчиков и девочек. Методы. Исследование проводилось в школах г. Екатеринбурга. В нем приняли участие 700 школьников 5–9 классов. Из них – 402 мальчика и 298 девочек. Использовался следующий психодиагностический инструментарий: методика «Шкалы позитивного и негативного аффекта» (Е.Н. Осин, 2012); опросник «Психическая устойчивость» (Clough, Earle, & Sewell, 2002, адаптация Малых С.Б., Исматуллиной В.И., Колясникова П.В., Лобасковой М.М.); опросник «Многомерная шкала удовлетворенности жизнью школьников» (Е.С. Хюбнер, 1994, адаптация Сычева О.А., Гордеевой Т.О., Лункиной М.В., Осина Е.Н., Сидневой А.Н.); опросник «Сильные и слабые стороны» (Goodman R., 2001). Для статистической обработки данных использовался корреляционный анализ по методу Спирмана с поправкой на множественные сравнения (метод Холма); эксплораторный факторный анализ с косоугольным вращением (oblimin) с помощью метода MINRES. Результаты. Установлено, что структура эмоционального благополучия школьников представлена тремя компонентами – эмоциональным, когнитивным и деструктивным – и имеет некоторые гендерные особенности. При этом структура эмоционального благополучия девочек соотносится с общей структурой, а структура эмоционального благополучия мальчиков имеет некоторые отличия и образована когнитивным компонентом, негативным эмоциональным компонентом и позитивным эмоциональным компонентом. Обсуждение результатов. Впервые структура эмоционального благополучия рассмотрена не в целом по выборке, а по группам – мальчики и девочки. Полученные представления расширяют имеющиеся в науке представления о феномене эмоционального благополучия. Результаты исследования могут быть использованы для разработки программ психологического сопровождения школьников с акцентом на сохранении их эмоционального благополучия.
... indicating that there is agreement about the valence of the message. We also see some correlations, like the correlations between gender and neuroticism, that are not relevant for our study, but are consistent with earlier research (Soto et al., 2011). ...
... This research has shown a specific pattern of mean-level changes that tend to occur during adolescence that does not occur in other age groups, giving rise to the disruption hypothesis, which states that there are temporal dips in meanlevel traits across adolescence due to changing biological, psychological, and social demands (Soto & Tackett, 2015). Consistent with the disruption hypothesis, some traits show consistent decreases across adolescence (e.g., Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience), that only later begin increasing into young adulthood, capturing a U-shaped pattern (Denissen et al., 2013;Slobodskaya, 2021;Soto et al., 2011). Despite the mean-level changes, Big Five personality traits also maintain a moderate degree of stability across adolescence (Borghuis et al., 2017;McCrae et al., 2002). ...
... Our observed decreases in Extraversion are also in line with mean-level decreases over 1 year found in Italian adolescents assessed at similar ages . Our findings on Conscientiousness are novel in adolescent HEXACO research but correspond with strong Big Five findings of a dip during adolescence in Conscientiousness (Soto et al., 2011). Four of six HEXACO traits showed meanlevel decreases over adolescence, which provides evidence for the disruption hypothesis (Soto & Tackett, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
We used a new English version of the HEXACO-Simplified personality Inventory (HEXACO-SPI) to examine (1) its dimensional structure and (2) the developmental stability and mean-level changes in its personality traits among adolescents. Our results showed that the HEXACO-SPI factor scales were consistent with the a priori hypothesized six-factor structure of HEXACO personality and that the HEXACO-SPI domain scales demonstrated sufficiently high levels of internal reliability, differentiation, and invariance across time. Stability was shown for all domain scales, but especially when considering 1-year estimates (versus 3 or 4-year estimates). Mean-level decreases were observed in Honesty-Humility, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness, with stability in Emotionality and Openness to Experience. Our findings inform research on HEXACO personality in adolescence, including stability and change.
... These differences in findings may result from the influence of socio-cultural and environmental factors on personality traits. Previous studies have emphasised that personality traits can be shaped by cultural norms, socialisation processes, and environmental conditions, leading to variations in traits observed across different populations (Schmitt et al., 2007;Soto et al., 2011;Suprapto et al., 2022). ...
... Moreover, higher levels of neuroticism have been associated with more difficult and challenging acute experiences with psilocybin (Barrett et al., 2017), raising concerns about the potential implications for adolescent users. Given that neuroticism tends to be naturally elevated in adolescents compared to adults (Soto et al., 2011), it likely plays a significant role in psychedelic use and experience. The relationship between neuroticism, adolescent psychedelic use, and mental health merits further investigation. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is still unknown how current trends in psychedelic use among adolescents are related to mental health. This study reports on the prevalence of psychedelic use in a Swedish representative nationwide drug survey and its association with mental health outcomes and user personality. A cross-sectional study using data from the latest 2024 Swedish Council for Information on Alcohol and Other Drugs (CAN) school survey was conducted (N = 10,288 in 9th and 11th grades; response rate: 79%). Psychedelic use was measured along with mental health (General Anxiety, GAD-7, Daily dysfunction, and Satisfaction with Life) and personality traits (Big Five; TIPI). A total of 1.9% reported lifetime psychedelic use. Users had higher GAD-7 scores compared to non-users (M: 9.7 vs. 5.4; Cohen’s d = 0.80) and differed in personality traits with higher neuroticism (d = 0.37) and lower conscientiousness (d = − 0.54). Psychedelic use did not add an explanation to mental health (ΔR² = 0.01) in addition to user personality (ΔR² = 0.32). Adolescent psychedelic use in Sweden is associated with poorer mental health, i.e., increased anxiety. Personality traits, particularly neuroticism, play a significant role, emphasizing the importance of individual differences in understanding adolescent psychedelic use. Public health policies should integrate education on psychedelic use and personality risk factors to inform adolescents and caregivers about potential risks and benefits.
... Regarding personality, this construct was mostly investigated analysing the ve factors identi ed by Costa and McCrae (1999): Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness to experience. While core aspects of personality tend to remain stable across adulthood (Debast et al. 2014;Soto et al. 2011), meaningful mean-level changes have been observed (Roberts & Mroczek 2008). For example, Neuroticism-re ecting emotional instability and negative emotions-typically declines with age, fostering greater emotional stability and resilience (Roberts et al. 2006;Terracciano et al. 2005). ...
... Longitudinal studies of older adults, however, identi ed a shift in this trend after the age of 70, possibly due to the increasing occurrence of negative experiences and self-e cacy declines (Kandler et al. 2015;Wagner et al. 2016). In contrast, Agreeableness, encompassing traits like kindness, cooperation, and empathy, has been observed to increase steadily with age, likely as older adults prioritize nurturing relationships and enhancing others' well-being (Costa & McCrae 1997;Soto et al. 2011). Openness to experience-creativity, curiosity, willingness to engage in new ideas and experiences-has been found to either increase or remain stable depending on life circumstances and personal interests, re ecting enduring intellectual curiosity and engagement with new ideas ( and Older Adults (60-100 years). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Ageing refers to a series of changes occurring throughout the lifespan in cognitive abilities, physical and mental health, and personality traits. While these dimensions have traditionally been studied as separate compartments, recent findings highlight their interdependence and dynamic interplay over time. To investigate their relationships, we analysed data from the Human Connectome Project using a psychometric network approach. Participants were grouped into three age categories: Young (22–35), Middle-aged (36–59), and Older (60–100) adults. We examined the interrelationships among 31 cognitive, psychological, and personality variables using Exploratory Graph Analysis (EGA) to estimate one network per age group and explore how these variables cluster into communities across the lifespan. Networks were then compared using the Network Comparison Test (NCT) to identify age-related differences in both global and local network properties. We observed substantial age-related changes: variables clustered into six communities in the Young Adults group but only into four in both the Middle-aged and Older Adults, suggesting dedifferentiation and reduced domain specificity in the older age groups. The NCT revealed distinct network architectures for each age group, with the most pronounced differences between Young Adults and the two older groups. Additionally, global strength—a measure of overall network connectivity—was significantly lower in Older Adults, indicating that associations among variables were on average weaker. Overall, these findings support the view that ageing is associated with structural transformations in the relationships among cognitive, psychological, and personality domains, following a dedifferentiation trajectory and highlighting the reorganization of behavioural functioning with age.
... As Loyalty incorporates an agreeableness component, which is characterized by the willingness to maintain reciprocal relationships, the temporary dip observed among girls may be explained by the normative decline in Agreeableness typically seen around age 13 (Soto et al. 2011). Moreover, this temporary dip is consistent with previous research indicating reduced sensitivity to Loyalty violations among 13-14 years old compared to 11-12 year-olds (Bretl and Goering 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The Moral Foundations Theory (MFT) identifies innate moral foundations that drive moral judgment, and are assumed to mature at different phases throughout development. However, core developmental aspects of moral foundations, such as normative age and gender differences from an MFT perspective, remain relatively unexplored, particularly in pre‐adulthood. Therefore, the present study investigates age and gender differences in moral foundations during adolescence, a sensitive phase for moral identity development. Methods Cross‐sectional data was collected from 2022 to 2024 in a wide variety of elementary and secondary schools in Flanders, Belgium. A total of 778 adolescents (Mage = 13.14, SDage = 1.76, age range = 10–17 years, 55.4% boys, 44.6% girls) completed 30 moral foundation vignettes. Results Regression analysis revealed normative decreases in all moral foundations with age, with significant curvilinear trends observed for Fairness, Authority, and Sanctity among boys and girls. Gender differences emerged, with girls placing greater emphasis on Care and Liberty, and interaction effects also suggest distinct developmental trajectories of Care, Liberty, and Loyalty across genders. Conclusions These findings indicate that age‐related and gender‐specific changes in moral foundations already emerge during adolescence.