Fig 2 - uploaded by Coline Jaworski
Content may be subject to copyright.
Flight cage used for bumblebee training procedure. Plants are of the subspecies A . majus striatum (yellow-flowered). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0130225.g002 

Flight cage used for bumblebee training procedure. Plants are of the subspecies A . majus striatum (yellow-flowered). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0130225.g002 

Source publication
Article
Full-text available
Understanding how pollinator behavior may influence pollen transmission across floral types is a major challenge, as pollinator decision depends on a complex range of environmental cues and prior experience. Here we report an experiment using the plant Antirrhi-num majus and the bumblebee Bombus terrestris to investigate how prior learning experien...

Context in source publication

Context 1
... scent and in ambient air: E = (A sample A air ) / (k . t . N f ) , with A sample and A air (area units) the peak areas of the floral scent sample and the control ambient air sample respectively, k ((area units).ng -1 ) the response coefficient, t (min) the sampling time, and N f the number of flowers at anthesis of the plant. This equation is a simplified version of that used by [28], as we had no air purge of the chamber in our sampling method. Thus, the incident air flow was equal to the air flow carried through the collector cartridge. This is known that the emission rate of some VOCs can be altered by environmental conditions [41; 42], and especially photosynthetically active radiation and temperature, which led to the development of algorithms on the model VOC isoprene to standardize the emission rate in a range of environmental conditions [43]. However, the different VOCs in the floral blend of A . majus have very variable properties in terms of volatility and chemical class, thus we found that is was not relevant to use such a standardization. We did not find a strong impact of temperature or solar radiation on VOC emission. Some peaks corresponded to molecules known to be atmospheric pollutants or non biogenic VOCs (ethylbenzene, styrene, naphtalene and phtalate derivatives, silicate and chlorate compounds), and other were quantified in large amounts in blank samples. These peaks were considered as contaminants, and they were excluded from the analysis. Rare peaks (present in less than 10% of the sampled plants) were also removed. We assumed that peaks could be detected if the absolute area exceeded 1000 area units, and the signal-to-noise ratio exceeded 10. Quanti- fication thresholds were 10 ng for fatty acid derivatives, 0.75 ng for terpenic compounds, and 2.5 ng for benzenoid compounds, as estimated from the quantification thresholds of external standards (Table A in S1 File). Thus, VOCs below these thresholds were excluded from quantitative analyses (see next paragraph). Finally, peaks for which uncertainty on area estimation was greater than 10% in average were also excluded from quantitative analyses. Statistical analyses of floral scent profiles. We measured 152 VOCs, after excluding four rare ones. We could identify 50 VOCs, and quantify 31 of them plus six unidentified VOCs. Differences in presence / absence of VOCs between subspecies were analyzed for the 152 VOCs separately, by comparing proportions of individuals of each species for which the VOC was recorded with the function prop.test (library stats; [44]). For the 37 quantified VOCs, we performed three analyses on the absolute quantities (in ng.min -1 .flower -1 ), after a log-transfor- mation and centering to reduce effects of variance heterogeneity due to difference in emission rates among VOCs, and to assign a relatively equivalent weight to each VOC. We first performed a principal component analysis of scent profiles to detect the most variable VOCs across plants. We then performed a partial least square-discriminant analysis (PLS-DA, R library mixOmics) to discriminate the scent profiles of A . majus subspecies. This analysis accounts for data sets with multi-collinear explanatory variables and a small number of samples compared to the number of explanatory variables; it also allows to test group separation [45; 46]. In PLS-DA, variables may be selected that explain most of the variance among samples; we selected explanatory VOCs for which the variable importance index was above one, a commonly used threshold. Third, we tested whether the variation of absolute emission rate in each VOC could be explained by subspecies with a two-sample t -test. Finally, we analyzed the emission rate ratios of the 37 quantified VOCs, when the absolute emission rate is normalized by the total emission rate of the 37 VOCs. We performed a correspondence discriminant analysis on the ratios. All statistical tests in our study were performed in the R software (R Development Core Team 2014). Bumblebee training procedure. To test how the learning experience of the bumblebees may influence their preference between A . majus subspecies floral types, we generated three sets of individuals. The first two sets were trained to forage on plants of one subspecies only, and we then tested their choice between plants of different subspecies. The third set was com- posed of naive bumblebees and used as a control. The learning assay was conducted as follows. Each day, two groups of five bumblebees were trained to foraging in a flight cage, one from 9am to 10am and the other from 10am to 11am. The most active bumblebees were isolated from their colony and introduced into the flight cage, through the Y-maze used in the behavioral tests (see below), but with arm ends open, to habituate them to the Y-maze environment. One colony was used each day, and we alternated the colonies. The flight cage consisted of a wooden frame of 1.6 × 1.2 × 0.9 m covered with a fine mesh (Fig 2). Six to eight plants that had from one to five flowers at anthesis each were randomly selected from the pool of available individuals from different geographical origins. They were randomly placed inside the cage, and were randomly permuted after half an hour. The total number of available open flowers was maintained as constant as possible throughout the training, but varied from 11 to 31 (mean 20 ± 1 SEM) due to flowering plant availability. Two groups of plants were used for the two sessions each day, and for three successive days, except the ones that had completed flowering, which were replaced. Flower nectar is naturally refilled in less than 24 h within an Antirrhinum flower (C. Suchet and E. Tastard, pers. obs.), thus all bumblebees had access to similar quantities of nectar throughout the experiment. During the training, bumblebees foraged freely. All foraging events were recorded, and classified as a visit when the bee landed on a flower without entering, or a nectar / pollen collection when the bee entered the flower. At the end of the training session, trained bees were placed together in an empty clean box until the late afternoon. They were supplied with water to avoid dehy- dration, but they were not provided with food to maintain optimal foraging conditions. We also trained the naive bees, using the same protocol as for bees trained on real flowers, but using artificial rewarding spots exempt of visual and olfactory cues. Bumblebee behavioral tests. To test the joint effect of learning experience and plant signal availability on bumblebee choice, we used a factorial design, where we varied bumblebee experience (three modalities: pseudomajus -trained / striatum -trained / naive bees) and the type of plant signals (three modalities: color only, odor only, color and odor). All experiments were conducted in two Y-mazes simultaneously. The bees trained to foraging in a flight cage in the morning were exposed to a choice test in the late afternoon. We conducted choice tests between plants of different subspecies, and control tests between plants of the same subspecies. Control tests were conducted to reveal possible biases influencing bee behavior and bee ability to discriminate against two plants of the same subspecies. The two plants used in a test were selected to have the same number and arrangement of flowers along the stem, and had never been in contact with any insect before. To control for orientation, all tests were repeated with the same bees by varying the left / right position of the two plants at arm ends. Y-mazes were constructed in wood covered by UV-transmitting Plexiglas (Fig 1). The main channel was 200 mm long, 283 mm wide and 200 mm high. The symmetric arms were 300 mm long, 200 mm wide and 200 mm high, and formed a 90° angle. This size is compatible with bee capacity to discriminate flowers, as an inflorescence of two-three flowers represents a spot of about 2 × 8 cm [47]. An air-flow system carried floral scent from arm ends to the main channel at 180mL/min, by air aspiration from a hole made in the front wall of the main channel (Fig 1B). We controlled how floral scent was carried through the Y-maze for the three types of plant signals, by sampling air in each arm and at the entrance of the main channel with the same method used for floral scent sampling. For each VOC, the quantity measured in air samples inside the Y-maze was about 70% of the quantity measured in floral samples collected directly in the plant chamber. We did not detect a bias in scent emission: when the same plant was placed at each arm end, the quantities detected in each arm did not differ, thus floral scent plumes were considered equivalent between arms. Also, we did not detect a difference in scent quantities measured between the two Y-mazes. Finally, we verified that the olfactory plus visual treatment did not alter the VOC quantities compared with the olfactory signal only treatment. Quantities measured in the visual signal only treatment matched VOC quantities measured in ambient air around the Y-maze. Bees were placed inside the Y-maze just above the air flow funnel (Fig 1B), through a UV- transparent plexiglas tube connected to a 100 × 100 mm landing area through a 35 mm-diameter hole. Arm ends were closed with three different sheets corresponding to the three types of plant signals. For the color treatment, sheets were Dura-Lar oriented polyester film. For the odor treatment, sheets were cardboard panels covered with fine mesh outside and occulted light or every other visual cue at the arm ends. For the treatment with both color and odor, sheets were Dura-Lar oriented polyester film, with a centered 30 mm-diameter hole located 20 mm below the roof, and covered by a 50 × 50 mm fine mesh to prevent bees from escaping. Plants were placed symmetrically in 200 × 200 × 900 mm chambers next to the arm ends (Fig 1A). Plant chambers consisted of a wooden frame covered by UV-transparent ...

Similar publications

Article
Full-text available
G-protein signaling components have been attributed many biological roles in plants, but the extent of involvement of G-protein coupled receptor 1 (GCR1) with the Gα (GPA1) remained unknown. To address this, we have performed transcriptomic analyses on Arabidopsis gpa1-5gcr1-5 double mutant and identified 656 differentially expressed genes (DEGs)....
Article
Full-text available
We present a semi-in vitro chemotropism assay that can be used to evaluate the chemoattractant effect of diffusible plant signaling molecules on growing pollen tubes. We constructed an array of microslit channels in a microfluidic device that prevented the passage of randomly growing pollen tubes but permitted ones that are responsive to the chemoa...
Article
Full-text available
Inositol-requiring enzyme 1 (IRE1) is the most conserved transducer of the unfolded protein response that produces either adaptive or death signals depending on the amplitude and duration of its activation. Here, we report that SQUAMOSA PROMOTER-BINDING PROTEIN-LIKE 6 (SPL6)-deficient plants displayed hyperactivation of the endoplasmic reticulum (E...
Article
Full-text available
Plant growth and development are negatively affected by a wide range of external stresses, including water deficits. Especially, plants generally reduce the stomatal aperture to decrease transpiration levels upon drought stress. Advanced technologies, such as wireless communications, the Internet of things (IoT), and smart sensors have been applied...
Article
Full-text available
Peanut yield is severely affected by exchangeable calcium ion (Ca²⁺) deficiency in the soil. Arbuscular mycorrhizal (AM) symbiosis increases the absorption of Ca²⁺ for host plants. Here, we analyzed the physiological and transcriptional changes in the roots of Arachis hypogaea L. colonized by Funneliformis mosseae under Ca²⁺-deficient and -sufficie...

Citations

... It is thought that species separation is maintained because of pollinator preferences for particular morphs, and/or foraging behaviours, such as re-visiting flowers of the same type (e.g. colour, shape and scent, known as flower constancy; Whibley et al., 2006;Tastard et al., 2014;Jaworski et al., 2015). Thus, repressive alleles are important for maintaining parental phenotypes in hybrids. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the early 1900s, Erwin Baur established Antirrhinum majus as a model system, identifying and characterising numerous flower colour variants. This included Picturatum/Eluta, which restricts the accumulation of magenta anthocyanin pigments, forming bullseye markings on the flower face. We identified the gene underlying the Eluta locus by transposon‐tagging, using an Antirrhinum line that spontaneously lost the nonsuppressive el phenotype. A candidate MYB repressor gene at this locus contained a CACTA transposable element. We subsequently identified plants where this element excised, reverting to a suppressive Eluta phenotype. El alleles inhibit expression of anthocyanin biosynthetic genes, confirming it to be a regulatory locus. The modes of action of Eluta were investigated by generating stable transgenic tobacco lines, biolistic transformation of Antirrhinum petals and promoter activation/repression assays. Eluta competes with MYB activators for promoter cis‐elements, and also by titrating essential cofactors (bHLH proteins) to reduce transcription of target genes. Eluta restricts the pigmentation established by the R2R3‐MYB factors, Rosea and Venosa, with the greatest repression on those parts of the petals where Eluta is most highly expressed. Baur questioned the origin of heredity units determining flower colour variation in cultivated A. majus. Our findings support introgression from wild species into cultivated varieties.
... Although the effect of entomopathogenic fungi endophytic colonisation on floral traits such as pollen production has not been studied yet, other plant-microorganism associations such as mycorrhizae have been shown to favour pollen production (del Barrio-Duque et al. 2020). Bees can learn to associate floral cues such as floral scent with nectar and pollen floral resources, where volatile organic compounds (VOCs) are indicators of resource quality (Jaworski et al. 2015, Ito et al. 2021. ...
... The stress-induced increased emission rate of VOCs with a defensive primary function (rather than pollinator attraction) could deter pollinators (Theis, 2006;Schiestl et al., 2014). Avoiding drought-stressed plants based on floral scent signals may be positively selected, or based on associative learning (Jaworski et al., 2015), if altered floral scent is a cue for reduced quality or quantity of floral resources (Wilson Rankin et al., 2020). ...
... Sustained nectar production in C. albidus and S. rosmarinus under drought could help support pollinator biodiversity, since these two plant species play a central role in the pollination network, supporting 37 and 35 pollinator species, respectively (Ropars et al., 2020a). control and drought conditions, would help make this connection (Jaworski et al., 2015;Burkle & Runyon, 2017;Proffit et al., 2020). Our experiment was not designed to disentangle the relative contributions of indirect drought, pollinator-induced and direct drought-induced impacts on plant reproduction, and for this reason the minor changes measured in plant reproduction cannot be unambiguously attributed to either observed changes in the flower visitor community (Kevan & Eisikowitch, 1990) or to plant physiology (Karimmojeni et al., 2014). ...
... Drought impacts were otherwise limited on floral traits and rewards, pollinator visits or reproductive success. Pollinators may adapt to altered floral olfactory and visual signals (Jaworski et al., 2015) but qualitative and quantitative changes in floral resources and therefore potentially in pollinator diet breadth (Schweiger et al., 2010) will affect pollinator fitness and this would deserve further attention. As next steps, we recommend investigating drought impacts in communities where T. vulgaris is more abundant, since this species did show a reduction in floral nectar reward in our experiment, in more diverse Mediterranean plant communities, and in communities with clearly identified specialized and generalist plant and pollinator species. ...
Article
Full-text available
Pollinators are declining globally, with climate change implicated as an important driver. Climate change can induce phenological shifts and reduce floral resources for pollinators, but little is known about its effects on floral attractiveness and how this might cascade to affect pollinators, pollination functions and plant fitness. We used an in situ long‐term drought experiment to investigate multiple impacts of reduced precipitation in a natural Mediterranean shrubland, a habitat where climate change is predicted to increase the frequency and intensity of droughts. Focusing on three insect‐pollinated plant species that provide abundant rewards and support a diversity of pollinators (Cistus albidus, Salvia rosmarinus and Thymus vulgaris), we investigated the effects of drought on a suite of floral traits including nectar production and floral scent. We also measured the impact of reduced rainfall on pollinator visits, fruit set and germination in S. rosmarinus and C. albidus. Drought altered floral emissions of all three plant species qualitatively, and reduced nectar production in T. vulgaris only. Apis mellifera and Bombus gr. terrestris visited more flowers in control plots than drought plots, while small wild bees visited more flowers in drought plots than control plots. Pollinator species richness did not differ significantly between treatments. Fruit set and seed set in S. rosmarinus and C. albidus did not differ significantly between control and drought plots, but seeds from drought plots had slower germination for S. rosmarinus and marginally lower germination success in C. albidus. Synthesis. Overall, we found limited but consistent impacts of a moderate experimental drought on floral phenotype, plant reproduction and pollinator visits. Increased aridity under climate change is predicted to be stronger than the level assessed in the present study. Drought impacts will likely be stronger and this could profoundly affect the structure and functioning of plant–pollinator networks in Mediterranean ecosystems.
... Other insect pollinators are not as widely studied, but their response diversity (varying responses of species to environmental pressures) and cross-scale resilience (resilience of species at varying scales) are beneficial for maintaining a diversity of flora (Senapathi et al., 2017). Insect pollinators also exhibit fidelity, otherwise known as flower constancy, to visit one species of flower continuously, and thereby increasing the probability that conspecific pollen will be transferred to the correct forb species (Jaworski et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Urban prairie gardens/plots are gaining popularity for providing similar ecological services as remnant and restored prairies, which are predominantly found in rural areas. However, it is not known to what extent small urban prairies can sustain the plant-pollinator interactions that are vital to both the insects and the plants. The goal of our research was to examine plant/pollinator interactions in three urban prairies in southwest Missouri and compare them to rural managed/restored prairies using a visit-based approach. Urban prairies were all in Springfield, Mo. and shared similar habitat matrices (within an area of 8 km2); rural prairies were located within 68 km of urban prairies, shared similar habitat matrices to each other, and shared soil edaphic characteristics with an urban prairie. From May through August 2018 in all six prairies, we observed the five most abundant forbs in bloom, the number of pollinator visits by bees, butterflies/moths, wasps, beetles, and flies; and, pollinator fidelity from dawn to dusk. The areas observed within a prairie, hereafter plot(s), were determined randomly by where at least two plants of the same species, out of the five most abundant forbs, were located. Using these criteria of observations on the five most abundant species across six prairies and four months, a total of 66 forb species were identified, with 58 of the species native to tallgrass prairies. However, only eight of the 58 native forb species were shared across urban and rural prairies. Jaccard similarity indices indicate lower similarity of the five abundant forbs within urban plots (9%) when compared to rural plots (24%), and low similarity between urban and rural plots (9%). Insect visitation varied by prairie type (rural/urban), month, and insect group; however, urban plots received 61% of the total visits compared to 39% in rural plots. Bees accounted for 5913 visits out of 10,113 visits recorded; high bee visits were similar in urban and rural plots. Insect fidelity was over 97% and did not significantly differ between rural and urban prairies. Therefore, the lack of similarity among and across urban and rural prairies in dominant species did not affect insect visitation rates or fidelity in our study. Our results suggest that establishment and management of urban prairie gardens and plots of various size may sustain the same or greater levels of pollinator services as rural prairies.
... All experiments were performed in a custom designed and constructed Y-maze chamber (Fig. 1, Fig. 2). The chamber design was selected based on the successful models from both Dyer et al. (2008) and Jaworski et al. (2015) and failed attempts at using both "U" and "T" shaped maze designs. The success of the Y-maze is based on the simple maneuvering and spatial needs for a bee to successfully move through the maze (Conrad et al. 1996). ...
... That is, an inherent bias within individuals towards one direction (AKA handedness). Previous research has indicated that there may be slight directional biases within Y-maze chambers using intact plants (Jaworski et al. 2015 for examining pollinator enhancement from seed mixes in strips, insecticide effects on pollination, and plant mediated effects on pollinator behaviors and colony health. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
A key question in pollination biology is that of how pollinators identify and choose foraging patches. Several approaches have been employed for this, including field studies and large greenhouse flight chambers. Most methods used to date are limited, though, by reliance on a combination of artificial flowers, large spatial scales, or lack of spatially distinct floral patches. To address this issue, we designed and tested a y-maze flight arena and tested it using the bumblebee Bombus impatiens and canola plants. Our results indicate that the system is not biased by environmental conditions, or by an innate “handedness” of Bombus impatiens . We did find that bees made all the expected patch choices when presented with soil, plants without flowers, or plants with flowers. This new method is important and useful as it allows researchers to ask questions of both plant health and insect behavior and the chamber system is modular allowing for simple changes to the setup to focus on different questions.
... Whilst bumblebees are well known for pollinating Antirrhinum plants (Jaworski et al., 2015) and humans have used this genus long as ornamental (Kowarik, 2005), the antagonistic effect of thrips on these plants may be either caused by a strong pollen reduction or by the transmission of pathogens (Ullman et al., 2002). ...
... Tower-feeding foraging worker bumblebees were marked on the thorax with water-soluble paints and used for further experiments. Some bumblebees were used more than once and, in those cases, at least 7 days were left between assays to allow short term learning-associations to disappear from their memories (Jaworski et al., 2015). Consequently, we consider our results in the context of flower naive responses of bumblebees, here testing their innate preferences to the different floral traits. ...
Article
Full-text available
Studies on the selection of floral traits usually consider pollinators and sometimes herbivores. However, humans also exert selection on floral traits of ornamental plants. We compared the preferences of bumblebees (Bombus terrestris), thrips (Frankliniella occidentalis), and humans for flowers of snapdragon. From a cross of two species, Antirrhinum majus and Antirrhinum linkianum, we selected four Recombinant Inbred Lines (RILs). We characterised scent emission from whole flowers and stamens, pollen content and viability, trichome density, floral shape, size and colour of floral parts. We tested the preferences of bumblebees, thrips, and humans for whole flowers, floral scent bouquets, stamen scent, and individual scent compounds. Humans and bumblebees showed preferences for parental species, whereas thrips preferred RILs. Colour and floral scent, in combination with other floral traits, seem relevant phenotypes for all organisms. Remarkably, visual traits override scent cues for bumblebees, although, scent is an important trait when bumblebees cannot see the flowers, and methyl benzoate was identified as a key attractant for them. The evolutionary trajectory of flowers is the result of multiple floral traits interacting with different organisms with different habits and modes of interaction.
... majus subsection Antirrhinum sensu Rothmaler 1956): A. majus pseudomajus with magenta flowers and A. majus striatum with yellow flowers. These subspecies are considered as sister taxa, and the main difference is flower colour (Jaworski et al. 2015). They occur parapatrically in the eastern part of the Pyrenees where they come into contact in a zone stretching 150 km along the Sierra del Cadi in Catalonia, Spain (Whibley et al. 2006; Fig. 1). ...
... Reproductive isolation in A. majus may be caused by the choice of pollinators (Andalo et al. 2010;Suchet et al. 2011;Tastard et al. 2012Tastard et al. , 2014Jaworski et al. 2015). In Antirrhinum, the corolla is made of two lobes that close the flower, and only large bees can access the nectar (mostly Bombus spp and Xylocopa violacea, Suchet et al. 2011;Tastard et al. 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Spatial processes have a major influence on the stability of species interaction networks and their resilience to environmental fluctuations. Here, we combine field observations and a dynamic model to understand how spatial processes may affect a network composed of the flowering plant A ntirrhinum majus , its cohort of pollinators, and a specialist seed‐predator and its parasitoid. The interactions taking place within this system were investigated by determining the fate of flowers and fruits on flowering and fruiting stems at 16 study sites. We then used this information to estimate spatial and temporal variation in the pollination rate, parasitism rate and hyperparasitism rate. We found that the plants were pollinator‐limited, with relatively variable fruit‐to‐flower ratios across sites. On almost all sites, plants were both parasitized and hyperparasitized, at a low to moderate rate. Comparing our field observations with a tritrophic Nicholson‐Bailey model, we found that empirical data are not always consistent with the conditions for local tritrophic persistence. This suggests that other mechanisms such as random disturbances and recolonizations (patch dynamics) or inter‐site migration through metacommunity dynamics (source‐sink dynamics) play a role in this system. Model simulations showed that dispersal could contribute to increasing tritrophic persistence in this system, and that source‐sink structure, not just environmental stochasticity, may cause the observed pattern of spatial variation. Synthesis . We defined and measured metrics related to species interactions and densities. This led us to suggest that the functioning of the A. majus metacommunity is more consistent with source‐sink than patch metacommunity dynamics, highlighting the extent to which dispersal explains the persistence of the system.
Article
Full-text available
A key question in pollination biology is that of how pollinators identify and choose foraging patches. Several approaches have been employed for this, including field studies and large greenhouse flight chambers. Most methods used to date are limited, though, by reliance on a combination of artificial flowers, large spatial scales, or lack of spatially distinct floral patches. To address this issue, we designed and tested a y-maze flight arena and tested it using the bumblebee Bombus impatiens and canola plants. Our results indicate that the system is not biased by environmental conditions, or by an innate “handedness” of Bombus impatiens. We found that bees made all the expected patch choices when presented with soil, plants without flowers, or plants with flowers. This new method is important and useful as it allows researchers to ask questions of both plant health and insect behavior and the chamber system is modular allowing for simple changes to the setup to focus on different questions. Highlights • Y-maze flight arena was designed to evaluate foraging behavior on intact plants. • No evidence of side preference in individual bees. • Behaviors observed in the y-maze appear to correspond to behaviors observed in other settings.
Article
Behaviour, including personality, informs us about the response of animals towards their changing environment. Despite the widespread occurrence of florivorous insects and the important but often underrated ecological roles that they play, the study of florivore behaviour is neglected relative to that of pollinators and other herbivores. Specifically, we do not know how different personality types can develop among florivores and enable them to persist in habitats with an ephemeral and dynamic availability of food resources. To address this knowledge gap, we investigated the following questions: whether the (a) inter‐individual differences of exploration and boldness are consistent; (b) inter‐population differences of exploration and boldness are consistent; (c) exploration and boldness are correlated. We collected individuals of the polyphagous floriphilic katydid, Phaneroptera brevis from four populations from wasteland sites in Singapore and performed a personality assay conducted in an insectary to investigate the exploratory and boldness levels of the individuals and populations. The major novel finding was that the floriphilic P. brevis katydids exhibit population‐level personality types for boldness, but not for exploration. Some katydid individuals were consistently more exploratory and bolder than other individuals. However, contrary to our predictions, we did not find any evidence of behavioural syndromes in the katydid individuals, as the boldness level for individuals was not significantly correlated with exploration for individuals. This suggests that an individual which is more exploratory may not be equally keen to take risks and consume novel food that it encounters. Our findings also suggest that boldness and exploration are linked to ecologically important behaviours, but more studies are needed to better understand population‐level personality and how and why natural selection may favour the evolution of personality in certain populations.