Article

Recognition of facial emotions among maltreated children with high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder.

Department of Psychology, University of California, 1285 Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA.
Child Abuse & Neglect (Impact Factor: 2.47). 02/2008; 32(1):139-53. DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.09.006
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT The purpose of this study is to examine processing of facial emotions in a sample of maltreated children showing high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Maltreatment during childhood has been associated independently with both atypical processing of emotion and the development of PTSD. However, research has provided little evidence indicating how high rates of PTSD might relate to maltreated children's processing of emotions.
Participants' reaction time and labeling of emotions were measured using a morphed facial emotion identification task. Participants included a diverse sample of maltreated children with and without PTSD and controls ranging in age from 8 to 15 years. Maltreated children had been removed from their homes and placed in state custody following experiences of maltreatment. Diagnoses of PTSD and other disorders were determined through combination of parent, child, and teacher reports.
Maltreated children displayed faster reaction times than controls when labeling emotional facial expressions, and this result was most pronounced for fearful faces. Relative to children who were not maltreated, maltreated children both with and without PTSD showed enhanced response times when identifying fearful faces. There was no group difference in labeling of emotions when identifying different facial emotions.
Maltreated children show heightened ability to identify fearful faces, evidenced by faster reaction times relative to controls. This association between maltreatment and atypical processing of emotion is independent of PTSD diagnosis.

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Amanda E Guyer, Jul 12, 2015
0 Followers
 · 
126 Views
  • Source
    • "For example, Kimonis , Frick, Cauffman, Goldweber, and Skeem (2012) found that male juvenile offenders with secondary-CU traits were more responsive to images involving others' emotional distress than were youth with primary-CU traits; similarly, Sink and Kerig (2011) found that youth with secondary-CU traits were more accurate than those with primary CU in recognizing selfconscious emotions such as disgust. The alternative construction that acquired CU arises as a function of trauma, however, suggests the need to attend to the parallel program of research investigating emotion recognition among traumatized youth, which indicates a heightened reactivity to negative emotions displayed by others (Leist & Dadds, 2009; Masten et al., 2008; Pollak, 2008). Therefore, further research is needed to understand emotion processing among youth who are simultaneously high in CU as well as trauma exposure. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: This study tested theories regarding differences in emotion processing among youth characterized by primary versus acquired callous-unemotional (CU) traits in a sample of 417 detained adolescents (306 boys, 111 girls). Mixture modeling identified 2 groups of youth high in CU, but with different levels of posttraumatic stress symptoms consistent with theoretical conceptualizations of acquired CU as being linked to trauma. Differences between the 2 groups of youth were investigated regarding 3 dimensions of emotion processing: emotion regulation, numbing, and recognition. Compared to youth classified in the primary group, youth classified as acquired CU demonstrated greater difficulty with lack of clarity (OR = 0.53), and nonacceptance of emotions, (OR = 0.57), general numbing of emotions (OR = 0.87), and recognition of disgust (OR = 0.18). Differences in emotion processing reported by youth in the 2 groups are consistent with theories regarding acquired callousness as related to emotional detachment in the aftermath of posttraumatic distress. The results of the current study have implications for the classification of primary and acquired CU, as well as the clinical treatment of youth with these characteristics.
    Journal of Traumatic Stress 08/2014; 27(4). DOI:10.1002/jts.21931 · 2.72 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "This finding supports previous work indicating that participants with a history of maltreatment show an increased sensitivity for negative faces (Dannlowski et al. 2012; Grant et al. 2011). Furthermore, a superior recognition of faces displaying fear and sadness (Leist and Dadds 2009) and shorter reaction times when labeling fearful facial expressions (Masten et al. 2008) was demonstrated. Prior learning plays an important role in how facial expression displays are classified into distinct emotion categories (Pollak and Kistler 2002). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Previous studies have shown that oxytocin (OXT) enhances social cognitive processes. It has also been demonstrated that OXT does not uniformly facilitate social cognition. The effects of OXT administration strongly depend on the exposure to stressful experiences in early life. Emotional facial recognition is crucial for social cognition. However, no study has yet examined how the effects of OXT on the ability to identify emotional faces are altered by early life stress (ELS) experiences. Given the role of OXT in modulating social motivational processes, we specifically aimed to investigate its effects on the recognition of approach- and avoidance-related facial emotions.
    Psychopharmacology 05/2014; 231(24). DOI:10.1007/s00213-014-3631-1 · 3.99 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "In the present study we used images of sad and happy infants to test whether salivary oxytocin levels were related to the evaluation of infant mood as derived from infants' facial emotional expressions. Emotional maltreatment has been associated with processing biases for some negative emotional stimuli such as anger and sadness, yielding heightened responsivity and reactivity [25] [26] [27] [28]. Impaired emotion recognition might result in more problematic interpersonal relationships [29] and more insensitive behavior towards offspring. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Childhood emotional maltreatment has been associated with a higher risk for maltreating one’s own offspring. In the current study, we explored a possible role of oxytocin in mediating the association between childhood emotional maltreatment and participants’ interpretation of infant facial expressions. Oxytocin levels were measured in 102 female participants using saliva samples. They rated the mood of thirteen infants with happy, sad and neutral facial expressions. Emotional maltreatment indirectly influenced responses to happy infant faces by modulating oxytocin levels: Higher self-reported emotional maltreatment was related to higher levels of salivary oxytocin which were in turn related to a more positive evaluation of happy infant expressions, but not to the evaluation of sad infant expressions. Oxytocin receptor polymorphism rs53576 did not moderate the relation between maltreatment experiences and salivary oxytocin levels. Early emotional maltreatment might indirectly affect emotional information processing by altering the oxytonergic system.
    Physiology & Behavior 05/2014; 131. DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.028 · 3.03 Impact Factor
Show more