Article

Preterm Birth, Social Disadvantage, and Cognitive Competence in Swedish 18- to 19-Year-Old Men

Department of Women and Children Health, Sachs Children's Hospital, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.
PEDIATRICS (Impact Factor: 5.3). 12/2009; 125(1):e67-73. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2008-3329
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT The aim was to study the impact of a range of gestational ages (GAs) on cognitive competence in late adolescence and how this effect is modified by contextual social adversity in childhood.
This was a register study based on a national cohort of 119664 men born in Sweden from 1973 to 1976. Data on GA and other perinatal factors were obtained from the Medical Birth Register, and information on cognitive test scores was extracted from military conscription at the ages of 18 to 19 years. Test scores were analyzed as z scores on a 9-point stanine scale, whereby each unit is equivalent to 0.5 SD. Socioeconomic indicators of the childhood household were obtained from the Population and Housing Census of 1990. The data were analyzed by multivariate linear regression.
The mean cognitive test scores decreased in a stepwise manner with GA. In unadjusted analysis, the test scores were 0.63 stanine unit lower in men who were born after 24 to 32 gestational weeks than in those who were born at term. The difference in global scores between the lowest and highest category of socioeconomic status was 1.57. Adjusting the analysis for the childhood socioeconomic indicators decreased the effect of GA on cognitive test scores by 26% to 33%. There was also a multiplicative interaction effect of social adversity and moderately preterm birth on cognitive test scores.
This study confirms previous claims of an incremental association of cognitive competence with GA. Socioeconomic indicators in childhood modified this effect at all levels of preterm birth.

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Anders Hjern, Jun 19, 2015
1 Follower
 · 
131 Views
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: High negative reactivity in early childhood interferes with later academic and behavioral adjustment. Thus, investigating the origins of high negative affectivity in early childhood is of high relevance for understanding emotional morbidity after preterm birth. The present work explored (1) direct prematurity-related consequences for negative reactivity, (2) self-regulatory deficits as a mechanism indirectly relating prematurity to negative affectivity and (3) the implications of the interplay between procedural distress in the neonatal period and parenting stress for preterm children's negative reactivity. The sample was comprised of 146 preterm children (very vs. moderately to late preterm) and 86 healthy full-term children, both free of major neurological impairment. Assessment involved negative affect and parenting stress (parent-report; 12, 24 months corrected age, CA), effortful control (behavioral battery, parent report; 24 months CA) and the number of potentially distressing neonatal intensive care procedures as well as severity of illness during the neonatal period (retrospective chart review). There was no direct link from prematurity to a disposition for high negative reactivity in early childhood nor was prematurity indirectly associated with higher negative reactivity through lower levels of effortful control. The relation between neonatal pain and distress and negative affectivity depended on the level of parenting stress with low parenting stress at the end of the first year of children's life buffering the negative influence of neonatal distress. The present findings underscore the importance of complex interactions among environmental factors in processes of emotional plasticity after preterm birth thereby providing critical suggestions for follow-up care.
    Infant Behavior and Development 12/2012; 36(1):124-138. DOI:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.11.002 · 1.67 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: In 2008 werden er in Nederland ruim 13.000 kinderen te vroeg geboren (7,7% van alle geboortes), waarvan 81 procent matig te vroeg werd geboren na een zwangerschapsduur tussen 32 en 36 weken (Stichting Perinatale Registratie Nederland, 2011 ). De afgelopen twee decennia is het aantal matige vroeggeboortes met 30 procent toegenomen (Martin et al., 2010 ). Hoewel de mortaliteitcijfers veel lager zijn bij deze kinderen dan bij ernstig te vroeg geboren kinderen (< 32 weken zwangerschapsduur), zijn de cijfers bijna tien keer hoger dan bij op tijd geboren kinderen (Mathews & MacDorman, 2008 ). Wat betreft neonatale complicaties hebben matig te vroeg geboren kinderen minder problemen dan ernstig te vroeg geboren kinderen, maar in vergelijking met op tijd geboren kinderen hebben ze een verhoogd risico op ademhalingsen voedingsmoeilijkheden, hypoglykemie en hyperbilirubine (Shapiro-Mendoza et al., 2008 ). Daarnaast zijn de hersenen van matig te vroeg geboren kinderen nog onvolgroeid: bij een zwangerschapsduur van 34 weken wegen de hersenen slechts 65 procent van het gewicht dat ze bij 40 weken zwangerschap hebben (Kinney, 2006 ). Ondanks de hoge prevalentie en de neonatale complicaties van matige vroeggeboorte is weinig bekend over de ontwikkelingsuitkomsten van deze kinderen op de lange termijn. In deze overzichtsstudie worden resultaten uit 28 studies over schooluitkomsten (zeven studies), cognitief functioneren (19 studies), gedragsproblemen (zes studies) en psychiatrische stoornissen (zes studies) bij matig te vroeg geboren kinderen gepresenteerd.
    11/2012; 16(5):157-162. DOI:10.1007/s12474-012-0027-z
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: While it is well established that very preterm birth (gestational age at birth < 32 weeks) is related to increased attention problems, there is still considerable uncertainty of the effects of moderate or late preterm birth or smallness for gestational age (SGA) on attention regulation.
    Early Human Development 02/2015; 91(2). DOI:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.01.004 · 1.93 Impact Factor