Plant Foods Consumed by Pan : Exploring the Variation of Nutritional Ecology Across Africa

Department of Primatology, Max-Planck-Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology (Impact Factor: 2.38). 01/2009; 141(3):476-85. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.21168
Source: PubMed


It has been shown that differences in resource density and nutrient supply affect variation in ranging patterns, habitat use, and sociality. Among nonhuman primates, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (P. paniscus) have often been used as models for the link between social system and habitat ecology. Field reports suggest that resource density is higher in habitats occupied by bonobos (compared to chimpanzee habitats), and in the West (compared to the East) of the range of chimpanzees. In this study we compared diet quality at the level of species and populations using information from nutritional analyses of fruit and leaves consumed by chimpanzees (three) and bonobos (one population). Quality of plant foods was assessed on the basis of a) the concentration of macronutrients, fiber, and anti-feedants, and b) associations of different nutrient components. Overall plant samples collected at each site differed in terms of macronutrient content. However, nutritious quality and gross energy content of food samples were similar suggesting that dietary quality reflects selectivity rather than habitat ecology. The quality of plant foods consumed by bonobos was within the range of chimpanzees and the quality of plant foods consumed by western chimpanzees was not higher than that of eastern chimpanzees. While the results showed significant variation across forests inhabited by Pan, they did not match with geographical patterns between and within Pan species as proposed in previous studies. This suggests that the nutritional quality of the habitat is not always a reliable predictor of the quality of the diet.

Download full-text


Available from: Sylvia Ortmann, Oct 13, 2015
40 Reads
  • Source
    • " 24 . 54 4 . 92 Klainedoxa Mesocarp 15 . 90 9 . 02 Nauclea Mesocarp 19 . 38 10 . 84 Parinari Mesocarp 19 . 15 7 . 34 Sacoglottis Mesocarp 18 . 63 6 . 00 Energy content was measured by burning a sample of the consumed food item in pure oxygen atmosphere using a bomb calorimeter whereby the heat produced is measured in kJ / g dry matter ( details in Hohmann et al . , 2010"
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: The extent to which animal vocalizations are referential has long been debated since it reflects on the evolution of language. Our closest living relative, the chimpanzee, has been shown to have functionally referential food calls in captivity but evidence for such capabilities in the wild is lacking. We investigated the context specificity and function of West African chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) food calls in the wild using all day focal follows of adult males and females of one habituated group in the Taï forest, Côte d’Ivoire. We collected over 750 hours of observation and analysed 379 food calls produced for five different food species and found that higher pitched calls were produced for the most nutritiously rich fruit. Additionally, within this species, chimpanzees modified calls according to tree size, whereby smaller trees elicited higher pitched calls. Moreover, we found that variation in food call structure tended to differentially attract nearby chimpanzees to food patches for this particular species. Our results indicate that chimpanzees subtly vary the acoustic structure of food calls with respect to food patch size for a high quality fruit, and we suggest that arousal alone cannot sufficiently explain the patterns observed. In light of these results, we propose that understanding the information content encoded by acoustic variation in chimpanzee food calls requires receiver knowledge about the natural ecological context, specifically spatial memory of tree locations. Therefore, this study highlights the potential significance of feeding ecology in the evolution of flexibly modulated vocal communication.
    Animal Behaviour 03/2015; 101. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.12.011 · 3.14 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "The Taï Chimpanzees consume native starch from wild nuts and seeds such as the Gabon nut (Coula edulis Baill.) and Kola nut (Cola nitida (Vent) Schott et Endl.) (Hohmann et al. 2010; N'guessan, 2012), and unlike humans, they consume no cooked or processed foods. Our preliminary reference collection of Tai Forest Table 1 Calculus samples analysed using SEMeEDX and OM. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Dental calculus is increasingly recognized as a major reservoir of dietary information. Palaeodietary studies using plant and animal micro remains (e.g. phytoliths, pollen, sponge spicules, and starch grains) trapped in calculus have the potential to revise our knowledge of the dietary role of plants in past populations. The conventional methods used to isolate and identify these micro remains rely on removing them from their microenvironment in the calculus, thus the microenvironment that traps and preserves micro remains is not understood. By using scanning electron microscopy and energy dispersive X-ray spectroscopy (SEM/EDX) on modern chimpanzee calculus from the Taï Forest, Côte d’Ivoire, and human calculus from the Chalcolithic site of Camino del Molino, Spain, we present the first reported observations on characteristics of the matrix setting that are conducive to the survival of starch in dental calculus. We also assess the potential for SEM/EDX to detect starch and differentiate it from structurally and molecularly similar substrates. We demonstrate that SEM/EDX may offer a non destructive technique for studying micro remains in certain contexts. Finally, we compare traditional optical analytical techniques (OM) with less invasive electron microscopy. The results indicate that SEM/EDX and OM are both effective for observing micro remains in calculus, but differ in their analytical resolution to identify different micro remains, and we therefore recommend a sequential use of both techniques.
    Journal of Archaeological Science 09/2014; 49(1):160-169. DOI:10.1016/j.jas.2014.04.016 · 2.20 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "Partitioning relates not only to the species’ dietary ecology [31]–[33], [62], [94], but also to physical aspects of the habitat (i.e., microhabitat) chosen for foraging (and other activities) by the great apes [95]. The combined effects of diet and habitat selection lessen inter-specific competition for resources, thereby improving the opportunities for both gorillas and chimpanzees to maintain nutrient-rich diets [29], [30]. For example, gorillas are more terrestrial and larger than chimpanzees and will therefore exploit different food sources. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Factors influencing the hominoid life histories are poorly understood, and little is known about how ecological conditions modulate the pace of their development. Yet our limited understanding of these interactions underpins life history interpretations in extinct hominins. Here we determined the synchronisation of dental mineralization/eruption with brain size in a 20th century museum collection of sympatric Gorilla gorilla and Pan troglodytes from Central Cameroon. Using delta C-13 and delta N-15 of individuals' hair, we assessed whether and how differences in diet and habitat use may have impacted on ape development. The results show that, overall, gorilla hair delta C-13 and delta N-15 values are more variable than those of chimpanzees, and that gorillas are consistently lower in delta C-13 and delta N-15 compared to chimpanzees. Within a restricted, isotopically-constrained area, gorilla brain development appears delayed relative to dental mineralization/eruption [or dental development is accelerated relative to brains]: only about 87.8% of adult brain size is attained by the time first permanent molars come into occlusion, whereas it is 92.3% in chimpanzees. Even when M1s are already in full functional occlusion, gorilla brains lag behind those of chimpanzee (91% versus 96.4%), relative to tooth development. Both bootstrap analyses and stable isotope results confirm that these results are unlikely due to sampling error. Rather, delta N-15 values imply that gorillas are not fully weaned (physiologically mature) until well after M1 are in full functional occlusion. In chimpanzees the transition from infant to adult feeding appears (a) more gradual and (b) earlier relative to somatic development. Taken together, the findings are consistent with life history theory that predicts delayed development when non-density dependent mortality is low, i.e. in closed habitats, and with the "risk aversion'' hypothesis for frugivorous species as a means to avert starvation. Furthermore, the results highlight the complexity and plasticity of hominoid/hominin development.
    PLoS ONE 07/2014; 9(7). DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0102794 · 3.23 Impact Factor
Show more