Article

A horse race of a different color: Stroop interference patterns with transformed words.

Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance (Impact Factor: 3.11). 11/1984; 10(5):622-39. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.622
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT Four experiments investigated Stroop interference using geometrically transformed words. Over experiments, reading was made increasingly difficult by manipulating orientation uncertainty and the number of noncolor words. As a consequence, time to read color words aloud increased dramatically. Yet, even when reading a color word was considerably slower than naming the color of ink in which the word was printed, Stroop interference persisted virtually unaltered. This result is incompatible with the simple horse race model widely used to explain color-word interference. When reading became extremely slow, a reversed Stroop effect--interference in reading the word due to an incongruent ink color--appeared for one transformation together with the standard Stroop interference. Whether or not the concept of automaticity is invoked, relative speed of processing the word versus the color does not provide an adequate overall explanation of the Stroop phenomenon.

Full-text

Available from: Kevin Niall Dunbar, Apr 17, 2015
4 Followers
 · 
951 Views
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: A Japanese version of the Stroop test was used to study interference effects produced by conflicting color words written in Kanji, a logographic orthography, and in Kana, a phonetic orthography. Words written in Kanji produced more Stroop interference than words written in Kana, even though words written in Kana were named faster. In another experiment, English readers indicated the spatial location of stimuli in the presence of conflicting arrows or words (e.g., the word left in the right position). Conflicting arrows (i.e., logographic symbols) produced more interference than conflicting words, yet words were named faster. In a Japanese version of the same task, subjects experienced the most difficulty when conflicting Kanji words were presented. But again, words written in Kana were named faster than words written in Kanji. Thus for both Japanese and English readers, logographic symbols were identified faster than phonetic symbols, yet phonetic symbols were named faster than logographic symbols. The findings demonstrate that by varying orthography, it is possible to dissociate word comprehension from word naming.
    The American Journal of Psychology 03/1987; 100(1):15. DOI:10.2307/1422640 · 1.09 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: It is widely accepted that the presentation of a printed word "automatically" triggers processing that ends with full semantic activation. This processing, among other characteristics, is held to occur without intention, and cannot be stopped. The results of the present experiment show that this account is problematic in the context of a variant of the Stroop paradigm. Subjects named the print color of words that were either neutral or semantically related to color. When the letters were all colored, all spatially cued, and the spaces between letters were filled with characters from the top of the keyboard (i.e., 4, #, 5, %, 6, and *), color naming yielded a semantically based Stroop effect and a semantically based negative priming effect. In contrast, the same items yielded neither a semantic Stroop effect nor a negative priming effect when a single target letter was uniquely colored and spatially cued. These findings (a) undermine the widespread view that lexical-semantic activation in word reading is automatic in the sense that it occurs without intention and cannot be derailed, and (b) strengthens the case that both implicit and explicit forms of visual word recognition require spatial attention as a necessary preliminary to lexical-semantic processing.
    Frontiers in Psychology 02/2015; 6:117. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00117 · 2.80 Impact Factor
  • Source