Article

Olfaction in bird dogs during hunting

SINTEF, Nidaros, Sør-Trøndelag, Norway
Acta Physiologica Scandinavica (Impact Factor: 2.55). 06/1996; 157(1):115-9. DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-201X.1996.479227000.x
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT The ability to catch scent continuously while running, which may be an essential skill for many animals of prey, requires that ambient air flows inward through the nose also during expiration. In this study on bird dogs, the direction of air flow was detected by measuring the temperature in the air inside the nostril. While resting, nose ventilation was synchronous with lung ventilation. While searching for ground scent, the dog was sniffing at a frequency of up to 200 s-1, a strategy which may create turbulence in the nasal passages and thereby enhance transport of scent molecules to the receptors in the ethmoidal cavity. When the bird dog was searching for game while running with its head high against the wind, it maintained a continuous inward air stream through the nose for up to 40s spanning at least 30 respiratory cycles. We suggest that expiratory gas flowing at high velocity from the trachea to the mouth cavity creates a lower pressure than in the nose thus causing an inward air stream through the nose during expiration by a Bernoulli effect.

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Lars Walløe, Jul 01, 2015
1 Follower
 · 
57 Views
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Sniffing, a rhythmic inhalation and exhalation of air through the nose, is a behavior thought to play a critical role in shaping how odor information is represented and processed by the nervous system. Although the mouse has become a prominent model for studying olfaction, little is known about sniffing behavior in mice. Here, we characterized mouse sniffing behavior by measuring intranasal pressure transients in behaving mice. Sniffing was monitored during unstructured exploratory behavior and during performance of 3 commonly used olfactory paradigms: a habituation/dishabituation task, a sand digging-based discrimination task, and a nose poke-based discrimination task. We found that respiration frequencies in quiescent mice ranged from 3 to 5 Hz--higher than that reported for rats. During exploration, sniff frequency increased up to approximately 12 Hz and was highly dynamic, with rapid changes in frequency, amplitude, and waveform. Sniffing behavior varied strongly between tasks as well as for different behavioral epochs of each task. For example, mice performing the digging-based task showed little increase in sniff frequency prior to digging, whereas mice performing a nose poke-based task showed robust increases. Mice showed large increases in sniff frequency prior to reward delivery in all tasks. Mice also showed increases in sniff frequency when nose poking in a nonodor-guided task. These results show that mouse sniffing behavior is highly dynamic, varies with behavioral context, and is strongly modulated by olfactory as well as nonolfactory events.
    Chemical Senses 07/2008; 33(7):581-96. DOI:10.1093/chemse/bjn029 · 3.28 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Sniffing is a rhythmic motor process essential for the acquisition of olfactory information. Recent behavioral experiments show that using a single sniff rats can accurately discriminate between very similar odors and fail to improve their accuracy by taking multiple sniffs. This implies that each sniff has the potential to provide a complete snapshot of the local olfactory environment. The discrete and intermittent nature of sniffing has implications beyond the physical process of odor capture as it strongly shapes the flow of information into the olfactory system. We review electrophysiological studies-primarily from anesthetized rodents-demonstrating that olfactory neural responses are coupled to respiration. Hence, the "sniff cycle" might play a role in odor coding, by allowing the timing of spikes with respect to the phase of the respiration cycle to encode information about odor identity or concentration. We also discuss behavioral and physiological results indicating that sniffing can be dynamically coordinated with other rhythmic behaviors, such as whisking, as well as with rhythmic neural activity, such as hippocampal theta oscillations. Thus, the sniff cycle might also facilitate the coordination of the olfactory system with other brain areas. These converging lines of empirical data support the notion that each sniff is a unit of olfactory processing relevant for both neural coding and inter-areal coordination. Further electrophysiological recordings in behaving animals will be necessary to assess these proposals.
    Chemical Senses 03/2006; 31(2):167-79. DOI:10.1093/chemse/bjj016 · 3.28 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Reduced olfactory ef®ciency in sniffer dogs results mainly from overheating, and causes physiological and behavioural resources to be diverted from concentration on the assigned task and applied instead to methods of body cooling. Dogs do not possess sweat glands, and panting is the main means of cooling the body. Since a dog can either sniff or pant, but can never perform both actions simultaneously, panting causes a decrease in snif®ng rate. In various military operations dogs are required to detect explosives under severe physical conditions. In such situations, the dog's high body temperature elicits rapid panting already at the very beginning of the search task. As a result, its concentration and olfactory ability are diminished from the outset. In the current study we examined the ability of dogs to detect small explosives charges in two situations: (1) while relaxed and therefore only lightly panting; and (2) following exercise on a treadmill and therefore heavily panting. The results revealed an inverse ratio between rate of panting and ef®ciency of the dog's olfactory work, with increased panting resulting in a signi®cant decrease in explosives detection. The decline in ef®ciency was also expressed in longer duration of search period. However, we also found that dogs are able to adjust to working under severe physiological conditions derived from extreme physical activity. Such adjustment can reduce the dog's limitations under strenuous conditions and can be taught by suitable training. # 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
    Applied Animal Behaviour Science 04/2003; 81(2). DOI:10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00274-5 · 1.63 Impact Factor