Article

Early androgen effects on aggression in children and adults with congenital adrenal hyperplasia.

Department of Behavioral and Social Sciences, School of Medicine, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale 62901-6517, USA.
Psychoneuroendocrinology (Impact Factor: 5.59). 11/1997; 22(7):505-15. DOI: 10.1016/S0306-4530(97)00049-8
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT Males are more likely than females to show aggressive behavior across species, ages, and situations, and these differences may be partly influenced by early hormones. We studied aggression in three samples of subjects with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), who were exposed to high levels of androgen in the prenatal and early postnatal periods. Controls were siblings and first cousins similar in age. In Sample 1, adolescents and adults completed the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ), which includes an Aggression scale. In Sample 2, adolescents and adults completed the MPQ and a paper-and-pencil version of Reinisch's Aggression Inventory. In Sample 3, parents rated the aggression of children aged 3-12, using a modification of Reinisch's Inventory. In all three samples, control males had higher aggression scores than control females. Further, as predicted, females with CAH had higher aggression than control females, but the difference was significant only in adolescents and adults. These results suggest that early androgens contribute to variability in human aggression.

1 Follower
 · 
98 Views
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Aggression is a key component for social behaviour and can have an adaptive value or deleterious consequences. Here, we review the role of sex-related differences in aggressive behaviour in both human and nonhuman primates. First, we address aggression in primates, which varies deeply between species, both in intensity and in display, ranging from animals that are very aggressive, such as chimpanzees, to the nonaggressive bonobos. Aggression also influences the hierarchical structure of gorillas and chimpanzees, and is used as the main tool for dealing with other groups. With regard to human aggression, it can be considered a relevant adaptation for survival or can have negative impacts on social interaction for both sexes. Gender plays a critical role in aggressive and competitive behaviours, which are determined by a cascade of physiological changes, including GABAergic and serotonergic systems, and sex neurosteroids. The understanding of the neurobiological bases and behavioural determinants of different types of aggression is fundamental for minimising these negative impacts. Copyright © 2015. Published by Elsevier Inc.
    Physiology & Behavior 03/2015; 143. DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.053 · 3.03 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: As outlined in prior chapters, gonadal hormones have powerful influences on sexual differentiation of brain and behavior in a wide range of mammals. This raises the question of whether sex differences in hu-man behavior could result in part from similar hor-monal influences. Human behavioral sex differences are currently understood to result from a combina-tion of social, cultural, cognitive, and biological mech-anisms (e.g., Golombok and Fivush, 1994). However, this work is about hormones. In keeping with this, this chapter will focus on one particular aspect of biological influences, the role of gonadal hormones, particularly androgens and estrogens, in the development of hu-man behaviors and human brain structures that show sex differences. Before beginning the review, it will be helpful to de-fine several terms and to outline the theoretical models that have been proposed to explain hormonal influ-ences on sexual differentiation in other species. (Al-though this will provide an overview of relevant an-imal models, other chapters of this work treat these models in greater detail, and interested readers are re-ferred to them for more extensive information.) After the overview of animal models, information about hu-man situations involving hormonal abnormalities will be described, as will the human behaviors that show sex differences and would therefore be hypothesized to be influenced by gonadal hormones. Finally, data on sex-ually differentiated behaviors in humans with hormone abnormalities will be discussed and conclusions as to the extent, nature, and implications of hormonal influ-ences on human sexual differentiation will be drawn.
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: During early development, testosterone plays an important role in sexual differentiation of the mammalian brain and has enduring influences on behavior. Testosterone exerts these influences at times when the testes are active, as evidenced by higher concentrations of testosterone in developing male than in developing female animals. This article critically reviews the available evidence regarding influences of testosterone on human gender-related development. In humans, testosterone is elevated in males from about weeks 8 to 24 of gestation and then again during early postnatal development. Individuals exposed to atypical concentrations of testosterone or other androgenic hormones prenatally, for example, because of genetic conditions or because their mothers were prescribed hormones during pregnancy, have been consistently found to show increased male-typical juvenile play behavior, alterations in sexual orientation and gender identity (the sense of self as male or female), and increased tendencies to engage in physically aggressive behavior. Studies of other behavioral outcomes following dramatic androgen abnormality prenatally are either too small in their numbers or too inconsistent in their results, to provide similarly conclusive evidence. Studies relating normal variability in testosterone prenatally to subsequent gender-related behavior have produced largely inconsistent results or have yet to be independently replicated. For studies of prenatal exposures in typically developing individuals, testosterone has been measured in single samples of maternal blood or amniotic fluid. These techniques may not be sufficiently powerful to consistently detect influences of testosterone on behavior, particularly in the relatively small samples that have generally been studied. The postnatal surge in testosterone in male infants, sometimes called mini-puberty, may provide a more accessible opportunity for measuring early androgen exposure during typical development. This approach has recently begun to be used, with some promising results relating testosterone during the first few months of postnatal life to later gender-typical play behavior. In replicating and extending these findings, it may be important to assess testosterone when it is maximal (months 1 to 2 postnatal) and to take advantage of the increased reliability afforded by repeated sampling.
    02/2015; 6:3. DOI:10.1186/s13293-015-0022-1