Article

Theoretical note: tests of synergy in sweetener mixtures.

Department of Food Science, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.
Chemical Senses (Impact Factor: 3.22). 09/1998; 23(4):447-51. DOI: 10.1093/chemse/23.4.447
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT Some methods for examining the additivity of sweeteners, and their synergy in mixtures depend upon setting component concentrations on the basis of sweetness equivalence, usually to a sucrose reference. These methods may under- or over-predict the sweetness of a mixture, leading to spurious claims of synergy or mixture suppression. This paper points out one problem with one such popular method, in that the method can lead to a conclusion that a substance would synergize with itself. To the extent that self-synergy is an illogical conclusion for a mixture comparison, such a method should be avoided in tests of synergy. The sweetness equivalence approach is contrasted with a simpler approach based on concentrations that does not lead to conclusions of self-synergy.

0 Bookmarks
 · 
121 Views
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: The purpose of the current study was to determine what effect, if any, the blending of sweeteners has on the time to maximum sweetness intensity of sweeteners. In this study that is comprised of three separate experiments, trained panelists evaluated the time to maximum sweetness intensity of sweeteners tested in both binary and ternary combinations. Sixteen sweeteners that varied widely in chemical structure were evaluated. Sweetener blends containing the protein thaumatin had the latest time to maximum sweetness intensity. As a group, blends containing neohesperidin dihydrochalcone, alitame, stevioside, rebauadioside-A, or neotame had later times to maximum sweetness intensity than blends with sugars and sugar alcohols. Many sweetener blends exhibited times to maximum sweetness intensity that fell intermediate between the earliest and latest of its constituent self-mixtures. These data indicate that the time to maximum sweetness intensity of “late” sweeteners can be shortened by blending with earlier onset sweeteners.
    Food Quality and Preference 01/2007; 18(2):405-415. · 2.43 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Taste is an essential component of flavour. Although taste carries fewer distinctive qualities than odour, it provides an essential base on which aroma builds to generate the widely varying flavour of our foods and beverages. The present review covers a wide range of perceptual/psychophysical topics on taste. The first section reviews the taste research questions of early last century; the second section addresses the psychophysical taste research topics of the last few decades; and in the third section, the question is raised whether the old research topics of 80–100 years ago, which have largely not been answered, are worth reconsidering and pursuing. Copyright © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
    Flavour and Fragrance Journal 10/2001; 16(6):439 - 456. · 1.82 Impact Factor
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Abstract1 Sweet is classically considered to be a single perceptual experience. However,2 diverse compounds can elicit this sensation, suggesting the existence of multiple3 pathways toward this end. This paper presents an overview of chemical theories of4 sweetness, reviews the phylogenetic and behavioral evidence for multiple pathways, and5 presents a summary of recent molecular advances regarding the sweet receptor. Potential6 sites for signal integration are discussed, and potential implications for nutritionists and7 food scientists are discussed.8 9

Full-text

Download
4 Downloads
Available from